Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T06:31:07.181Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early metalinguistic awareness of derivational morphology: Observations from a comparison of English and French

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2009

LYNNE G. DUNCAN*
Affiliation:
University of Dundee
SÉVERINE CASALIS
Affiliation:
Université de Lille 3
PASCALE COLÉ
Affiliation:
Université de Provence
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Lynne G. Duncan, School of Psychology, University of Dundee, Dundee DD1 4HN, Scotland, UK. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This cross-linguistic comparison of metalinguistic development in French and English examines early ability to manipulate derivational suffixes in oral language games as a function of chronological age, receptive vocabulary, and year of schooling. Data from judgment and production tasks are presented for children aged between 5 and 8 years in their first, second, or third school year in the United Kingdom and France. The results suggest that metamorphological development is accelerated in French relative to English. The French advantage encompasses knowledge of a broader range of suffixes and a markedly greater facility for generalizing morphological knowledge to novel contexts. These findings are interpreted in relation to the word formation systems of English and French, and the educational context in each country.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 58(Serial No. 238, No. 10), 1166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, L. (2003). English prefixation—A typological shift? Acta Linguistica Hungarica, 50, 3340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berko, J. (1958). The child's learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding: Development and disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Browne, D. (1997). Putting knowledge to work. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 353354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In Feldman, L. B. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12, 169190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F., & Nomanbhoy, D. M. (1993). Phonological and morphological awareness in 1 st-graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 177195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F., Stone, C. A., & Katz, L. A. (2001). The effects of phonological transparency on reading derived words. Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 249274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casalis, S., & Colé, P. (2009). On the relationship between morphological and phonological awareness: Effects of training in kindergarten and in first grade reading. First Language, 29, 113142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casalis, S., Colé, P., & Sopo, D. (2004). Morphological awareness in developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 114138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casalis, S., & Louis-Alexandre, M.-F. (2000). Morphological analysis, phonological analysis and learning to read French: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writing, 12, 303335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1978). Awareness of language: Some evidence from what children say and do. In Sinclair, A., Jarvella, R., & Levelt, W. J. M. (Eds.), The child's conception of language (pp. 1743). New York: Springer–Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V., & Cohen, S. R. (1984). Productivity and memory for newly formed words. Journal of Child Language, 11, 611625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V., & Hecht, B. F. (1982). Learning to coin agent and instrument nouns. Cognition, 12, 124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colé, P., Royer, C., Leuwers, C., & Casalis, S. (2004). Derivational knowledge and reading acquisition for French beginning-readers from Grade 1 to Grade 3. Année Psychologique, 104, 701750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, D. (2003). The Cambridge encyclopedia of the English language (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cutler, A., & Clifton, C. (1984). The use of prosodic information in word recognition. In Bouma, H. & Bouwhuis, D. G. (Eds.), Attention and performance: Vol. 10. Control of language processes (pp. 183196). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dauer, R. M. (1983). Stress-timing and syllable-timing reanalyzed. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 5162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., & Bryant, P. (2005). What young children do and do not know about the spelling of inflections and derivations. Developmental Science, 8, 583594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delattre, P. (1965). Comparing the phonetic features of English, French, German and Spanish: An interim report. Heidelberg: Julius Groos Verlag.Google Scholar
Dell, G. (1986). A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derwing, B. L. (1976). Morpheme recognition and the learning of rules for derivational morphology. Revue Canadienne de Linguistique, 21, 3866.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derwing, B. L., & Baker, W. J. (1979). Recent research on the acquisition of English morphology. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition (pp. 209223). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Duncan, L. G., Colé, P., Seymour, P. H. K., & Magnan, A. (2006). Differing sequences of metaphonological development in French and English. Journal of Child Language, 33, 369399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, L. G., Seymour, P. H. K., & Hill, S. (1997). How important are rhyme and analogy in beginning reading? Cognition, 63, 171208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, L. G., Seymour, P. H. K., & Hill, S. (2000). A small to large unit progression in metaphonological awareness and reading? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 10811104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Pintillie, D. (1982). British Picture Vocabulary Scale (BPVS). Windsor: NFER–Nelson.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Thériault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). Échelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody (EVIP). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Elliot, C., Murray, D., & Pearson, L. (1983). British Abilities Scales, manual 4. Windsor: NFER–Nelson.Google Scholar
Finkenstaedt, T., & Wolff, D. (1973). Ordered profusion: Studies in dictionaries and the English lexicon. Heidelberg: Winter.Google Scholar
Freyd, P., & Baron, J. (1982). Individual-differences in acquisition of derivational morphology. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 282295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giraudo, H., & Grainger, J. (2003). A supralexical model for French derivational morphology. In Assink, E. M. H. & Sandra, D. (Eds.), Reading complex words: Cross-language studies (pp. 139157). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gombert, J. E. (1992). Metalinguistic development. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Gonnerman, L. M., Seidenberg, M. S., & Andersen, E. S. (2007). Graded semantic and phonological similarity effects in priming: Evidence for a distributed connectionist approach to morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 323345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goswami, U., & East, M. (2000). Rhyme and analogy in beginning reading: Conceptual and methodological issues. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 6393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, J. B., & Baayen, R. H. (2005). Shifting paradigms: Gradient structure in morphology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 342348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaffré, J.-P. (2006). Orthographes et litéracies: Les particularités du Français. Les Cahiers du CENTAL, 1, 5159.Google Scholar
Jarmulowicz, L. D. (2002). English derivational suffix frequency and children's stress judgments. Brain and Language, 81, 192204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarmulowicz, L. (2006). School-aged children's phonological production of derived English words. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 294308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986). From meta-processes to conscious access—Evidence from children's metalinguistic and repair data. Cognition, 23, 95147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1997). Promissory notes, genetic clocks, and epigenetic outcomes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 355360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lefavrais, P. (1963). Du diagnostic de la dyslexie á l'étude clinique de la lecture. Un nouvel instrument: Le test “l'Alouette.” Revue de Psychologie Appliquée, 13, 189207.Google Scholar
Lété, B., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Colé, P. (2004). MANULEX: A grade-level lexical database from French elementary-school readers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 156166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, D. J., & Windsor, J. (1996). Children's analysis of derivational suffix meanings. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 209216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Longtin, C. M., Seguí, J., & Halle, P. A. (2003). Morphological priming without morphological relationship. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 313334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahony, D., Singson, M., & Mann, V. (2000). Reading ability and sensitivity to morphological relations. Reading and Writing, 12, 191218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mann, V., & Singson, M. (2003). Linking morphological knowledge to English decoding ability: Large effects of little suffixes. In Assink, E. M. H. & Sandra, D. (Eds.), Reading complex words: Cross-language studies (pp. 125). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Manza, L., & Reber, A. S. (1997). Representing artificial grammar: Transfer across stimulus forms and modalities. In Berry, D. C. (Ed.), How implicit is implicit learning? (pp. 73106). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melinger, A. (2003). Morphological structure in the lexical representation of prefixed words: Evidence from speech errors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 335362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moats, L. C. (2000). Speech to print: Language essentials for teachers. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 134147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pacton, S., Fayol, M., & Perruchet, P. (2005). Children's implicit learning of graphotactic and morphological regularities. Child Development, 76, 324339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning out of the lab: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 401426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pillon, A. (1998). Morpheme units in speech production: Evidence from laboratory-induced verbal slips. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 465498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pitt, M. A., & Samuel, A. G. (1990). The use of rhythm in attending to speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 564573.Google ScholarPubMed
Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (2000). Lexical and nonlexical print-to-sound translation of disyllabic words and nonwords. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 342364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., & New, B. (2004). The broth in my brother's brothel: Morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 10901098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S., & Gonnerman, L. M. (2000). Explaining derivational morphology as the convergence of codes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 353361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidler, S. (1988). Untersuchung zum Erwerb von Wortbildungsregeln Deverbativa Nomina (Agens und Instrument) im Französischen. Unpublished MA thesis, Universität Hamburg.Google Scholar
Sénéchal, M., & Kearnan, K. (2007). The role of morphology in reading and spelling. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 35, 297325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuart, M., Dixon, M., Masterson, J., & Gray, B. (2003). Children's early reading vocabulary: Description and word frequency lists. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 585598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singson, M., Mahony, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and morphological skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and Writing, 12, 219252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taft, M. (1994). Interactive-activation as a framework for understanding morphological processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 271294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T. (2001). The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 3769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valtin, R. (1984). The development of metalinguistic abilities in children learning to read and write. In Downing, J. & Valtin, R. (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read (pp. 207226). New York: Springer–Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Nakai, S., DePaolis, R. A., & Halle, P. (2004). The role of accentual pattern in early lexical representation. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 336353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Windsor, J. (1994). Children's comprehension and production of derivational suffixes. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 409417.Google ScholarPubMed
Zhang, J., Anderson, R. C., Packard, J., Wu, X., & Tang, S. (2009). Development of knowledge about compound word structures in Chinese and English. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar