Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:32:25.007Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Discourse characteristics of reticent children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Mary Ann Evans*
Affiliation:
University of Guelph
*
Mary Ann Evans, Department of Psychology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada N1G 2W1

Abstract

This study contrasted the interactions of less talkative children and their teacher with those of their peers during classroom “Sharing Time.” Seven reticent children and seven normal peers were observed and audiotape-recorded during 15 sessions across the school year. In addition to speaking less, reticent children engaged in less complex speech than their peers: They spoke more often about objects in the “here and now,” spoke about one topic at a turn, and spoke in shorter utterances. Questions were more frequently directed to the reticent children, but while peers responded to these questions as invitations to contribute further to the topic, reticent children frequently failed to respond to them in like manner. It is suggested that both anxiety and subtle language delays may contribute to the poorer discourse skills reticent children display.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1987

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bloom, L., Rocissano, L., & Hood, L. (1976). Adult-child discourse: Developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic interactions. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 521552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, B., & Lloyd, H. (1975). A clinical study of children not speaking at school. Journal of the Association of Workers of Maladjusted Children, 3, 2737.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. L. (1980). The deployment of consciousness in the production of a narrative. In Freedle, R. O. (Ed.), Advances in discourse processes: The pear stories (Vol. 3, pp. 949). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Cheek, J. J., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 330339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colligan, R. C. (1979). Predictive validity of the Myklebust Pupil Rating Scales: A two-year follow-up. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 12, 264267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Constable, C. (1983). Creating communicative context. In Winitz, H. (Ed.), For clinicians by clinicians. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. (1979). Sociolinguistic patterns in adult-child interactions. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Crozier, W. R. (1979). Shyness as a dimension of personality. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 121128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, D., & Plomin, (1985). Origins of individual differences in infant shyness. Developmental Psychology, 21, 118121.Google Scholar
Dickson, W. P. (1982). Creating communication-rich classrooms: Insights from the Sociolinguistic and referential traditions. In Wilkinson, L. C. (Ed.), Communicating in the classroom. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, R., & Wells, G. H. (1980). Enabling factors in adult-child discourse. First Language, 1, 4669.Google Scholar
Evans, M. A. (1985). Play beyond play: Its role in formal informative speech. In Pellegrini, A. & Galda, L. (Eds.), Play, language and stories (pp. 129143). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Evans, M. A., & Rubin, K. H. (1979). Hand gestures as a communicative mode in school-age children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 135, 189196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feshback, S., Adelman, H., & Williamson, W. F. (1974). Early identification of children with high risk of learning failure. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 12, 264267.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. J. (1979). On fluency. In Fillmore, C. J., Kemple, D., & Wang, W. S. (Eds.), Individual differences in language ability and language behavior. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
French, P., & MacLure, M. (1981). Teachers' questions, pupils' answers: An investigation of questions and answers in the infant classroom. First Language, 2, 3145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, R. J. (1975). The young child who does not talk. Clinical Pediatrics, 14, 403406.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1975). Requests and responses in children's speech. Journal of Child Language, 2, 4165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, E. M., & Thomas, A. (1967). Children's behavioral style and the teacher's appraisal of their intelligence. Journal of School Psychology, 5, 292300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubbell, R. D. (1977). On facilitating spontaneous talking in young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 42, 216231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keenan, E. O., & Klein, E. (1975). Coherency in children's discourse. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 365380.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. O., & Schieffelin, B. (1976). Topic as a discourse notion: A study of topic in the conversations of children and adults. In Li, C. (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 336384). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Koekkoek, C. S. (1985, 04). Adult interactions with language delayed children. In Evans, M. A. (Chair), Language disabled children and their verbal environment. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto.Google Scholar
Lazarus, P. J. (1982). Correlation of shyness and self-esteem for elementary school children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55, 810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michaels, S. (1983). The role of adult assistance in children's acquisition of literate discourse strategies. Volta Review, 79, 7296.Google Scholar
Mueller, E. (1972). The maintenance of verbal exchanges between young children. Child Development, 43, 930938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, D. C. (1971). Talk, silence and anxiety. Psychological Bulletin, 75(4), 244260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsen-Fulero, L., & Conforti, J. (1983). Child responsiveness to mother questions of varying type and presentation. Journal of Child Language, 10, 495520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, E. B., Olsen, T. F., & Throckmorton, M. C. (1960). Social case work with elementary school children who do not talk at school. Social Work, 3, 6470.Google Scholar
Pederson, D. J. (1968). Report to the Alabama County School District and Title III, Area of Pennsylvania, of the incidence of reticence in their respective school districts. Alameda County PACE Centre.Google Scholar
Phillips, G. M., & Metzger, N. J. (1973). The reticent syndrome: Some theoretical considerations about etiology and treatment. Speech Monographs, 40, 220230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1971). The language and thought of the child (Gabain, M. & Gabain, R., Trans.). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1926)Google Scholar
Reed, G. F. (1963). Elective mutism in children: A reappraisal. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 4, 99.Google Scholar
Rosinski, M., & Newhoff, M. (1962, 06). Conversational assertiveness and responsiveness in language disordered children. Paper presented at the meeting of the Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI.Google Scholar
Seitz, S., & Hoekenga, R. (1974). Modelling as a training tool for retarded children and their parents. Mental Retardation, 12, 2831.Google Scholar
Sluckin, A. (1977). Children who do not talk at school. Child: Care, Health and Development, 13, 6979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, H. W., Parker, T., Wilkinson, A., Hegion, A., & Fish, E. (1976). Predictive value of teacher ratings with young children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 507517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teitelbaum, N., & Wollner, S. (1982). The input of a mother, teacher and clinician to a languageimpaired child. Working Papers in Experimental Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology. 11, 123.Google Scholar
Van Kleeck, A., & Street, R. (1982). Does reticence just mean talking less? Qualitative differences in the language of talkative and reticent preschoolers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 11, 609629.Google Scholar
Wanska, S. K., & Bedrosian, J. L. (1985, April). Topical and pragmatic intent in mother-child discussion. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto.Google Scholar
Wilson, B. (1969). Relationship between self-esteem and communication problems in classrooms. Unpublished master's thesis, Pennsylvania State University.Google Scholar
Wood, D., McMahon, L., & Cranstoun, Y. (1980). Working with under fives. The Oxford Preschool Research Project, 5. London: Grant Mclntyre Limited.Google Scholar
Wood, H. A., & Wood, D. J. (1984). An experimental evaluation of the effects of five styles of teacher conversation on the language of hearing-impaired children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 4562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, H. L. (1968). A clinical study of children who refuse to talk in school. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 7, 603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimbardo, F. (1977). Shyness: What it is and what to do about it. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar