Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T09:06:41.641Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Detecting stress patterns is related to children's performance on reading tasks

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2009

NICOLÁS GUTIÉRREZ-PALMA*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Jaén
MANUEL RAYA-GARCÍA
Affiliation:
Universidad de Jaén
ALFONSO PALMA-REYES
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Nicolás Gutiérrez-Palma, Departamento de Psicología, Universidad de Jaén, Campus las Lagunillas s/n, Edificio D2, 23071 Jaén, Spain. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This paper investigates the relationship between the ability to detect changes in prosody and reading performance in Spanish. Participants were children aged 6–8 years who completed tasks involving reading words, reading pseudowords, stressing pseudowords, and reproducing pseudoword stress patterns. Results showed that the capacity to reproduce pseudoword stress patterns accounted for a unique portion of the variance in text reading, after controlling for phonological awareness, phoneme sensitivity, and working memory. In addition, stress sensitivity predicted children's performance in stressing pseudowords. These results suggest that stress sensitivity may affect fluency in reading as well as word stress learning.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alameda, J. R., & Cuetos, F. (1995). Diccionario de frecuencias de las unidades lingüísticas del Castellano. Oviedo, Spain: Servicio de publicaciones de la Universidad de Oviedo.Google Scholar
Black, M., & Byng, S. (1986). Prosodic constraints on lexical access in reading. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 369409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, M., & Byng, S. (1989). Re-stressing prosody: A reply to Cutler, Howard, and Patterson. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 8592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1958). Words and things: An introduction to language. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2004). Stress regularity or consistency? Reading aloud Italian polysyllables with different stress patterns. Brain and Language, 90, 318325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cappa, S. F., Nespor, M., Ielasi, W., & Miozzo, A. (1997). The representation of stress: Evidence from an aphasic patient. Cognition, 65, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colombo, L. (1992). Lexical stress effect and its interaction with frequency in word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 9871003.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In Dornic, S. (Ed.), Attention and performance VI. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., & Rastle, K. (1994). Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 11971211.Google Scholar
Cuetos, F., Rodríguez, B., & Ruano, E. (1996). PROLEC: Batería de evaluación de los procesos lectores de los niños de educación primaria. Madrid: TEA ediciones, S.A.Google Scholar
Cutler, A., Dahan, D., & Donselaar, W. (1997). Prosody in the comprehension of spoken language: A literature review. Language & Speech, 40, 141201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Defior, S., Jiménez Fernández, G., & Serrano, F. (2006). Reading acquisition: A transversal study of Spanish children. The International Journal of Learning, 12, 115122.Google Scholar
Defior, S., Martos, F., & Cary, L. (2002). Differences in reading acquisition development in two shallow orthographies: Portuguese and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 135148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donselaar, W., Koster, M., & Cutler, A. (2005). Exploring the role of lexical stress in lexical recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A 251273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, E., Peperkamp, S., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). A robust method to study “stress deafness.” Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 110, 16061618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. D. (1998). Learning to parse? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27, 285319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiménez, J. E., & Ortiz, M. R. (1995). Conciencia fonológica y aprendizaje de la lecutra: Teoría, evaluación e intervención. Madrid: Síntesis.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetry, V., Wade-Woolley, L., Kolinsky, R., & Mousty, F. (2006). The role of stress processing abilities in the development of bilingual reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 349362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U. (2002). In the beginning was the rhyme? A reflection on Hume, Hatcher, Nation, Brown, Adams, and Stuart. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 4757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U. (2003). How to beat dyslexia. The Psychologist, 16, 462465.Google Scholar
Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S., et al. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 1091110916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grainger, J., Muneaux, M., Farioli, F., & Ziegler, J. C. (2005). Effects of phonological and orthographic neighbourhood density interact in visual word recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 981998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutiérrez, N. (2003). El acento léxico y su funcion en el reconocimiento de palabras escritas en adultos y en niños [Lexical stress and its role in written word recognition by adults and children]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Universidad de Granada, Spain.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez, N., & Palma, A. (2004). Lexical stress and reading: A study with children. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 2, 143160.Google Scholar
Kelly, M. H. (2004). Words onset patterns and lexical stress in English. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 231244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, M. H., Morris, J., & Verrekia, L. (1998). Orthographic cues to lexical stress: Effects on naming and lexical decision. Memory & Cognition, 26, 822832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech poduction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marín, J., & Carrillo, M. S. (1999). Test Colectivo de Eficacia Lectora (TECLE). Unpublished manuscript. Universidad de Murcia, Departamento de Psicología Básica y Metodología.Google Scholar
Martínez, J., & Pérez, E. (2004). Frecuencias del castellano escrito en niños de 6 a 12 años [Frequencies of written Spanish in 6- to 12-year-old children]. Salamanca, Spain: Servicio de publicaciones de la Universidad Pontificia de Salamanca.Google Scholar
Miceli, G., & Caramazza, A. (1993). The assignment of word stress in oral reading: Evidence from a case of acquired dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 273296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2007). Prosody of syntactically complex sentences in the oral reading of young children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 839853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Protopapas, A. (2006). On the use and usefulness of stress diacritics in reading Greek. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 171198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quilis, A. (1971). Caracterización fonética del acento en Español. Travaux de Linguistique et de Littérature, 9, 5372.Google Scholar
Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (2000). Lexical and nonlexical print-to-sound translation of disyllabic words and pseudowords. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 342364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1989). The psychology of reading. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice–Hall.Google ScholarPubMed
Scaraborough, H. S., Ehri, L. C., Olson, R. K., & Fowler, A. E. (1998). The fate of phonemic awareness beyond the elementary school years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 115142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Costa, A. (1997). Metrical information in speech segmentation in Spanish. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 883887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serrano, F., González-Trujillo, M. C., & Defior, S. (2002). Phonological awareness differences between children and adults. In Trindade, N. (Ed.): Literacia e cidadania. Evora, Portugal: Centro de Investigação em Educação Paulo Freire.Google Scholar
So, D., & Siegel, L. S. (1998). Learning to read Chinese: Semantic, syntactic, phonological and working memory skills in normally achieving and poor Chinese readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 9, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastian-Gallés, N., & Cutler, A. (2001). Segmental and suprasegmental mismatch in lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 412432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stainthorp, R. (1998). Phonological sensitivity and reading: Evidence from precocious readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 21, 5368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wade-Woolley, L., Goetry, V., & Lung, J. (2004). Stress processing and word reading in Spanish and Mandarin adult learners of English. Paper presented at the Eleventh Annual Meeting of TripleSR, Amsterdam, June 27–30.Google Scholar
Wechler, D. (1997). WISC-R: Escala de inteligencia Wechler para niños revisada (3rd ed.). Madrid: TEA.Google Scholar
Whalley, K., & Hansen, J. (2006). The role of prosodic sensitivity in children's reading development. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 288303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, C. (2006a). Sensitivity to speech rhythm and the development of reading. In Columbus, F. (Ed.), Trends in cognitive psychology research. New York: Nova Science.Google Scholar
Wood, C. (2006b). Metrical stress sensitivity in young children and its relationship to phonological awareness and reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 270287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, C., & Terrell, C. (1998). Poor readers’ ability to detect speech rhythm and perceive rapid speech. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 397413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zorzi, M., Houghton, G., & Butterworth, B. (1998). Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 11311161.Google Scholar