No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
A Commentary on Werker (2017): Limitations of the laboratory and the role of variability in language learning
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 11 September 2018
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Commentaries
- Information
- Copyright
- © Cambridge University Press 2018
References
REFERENCES
Abramson, A. S., & Lisker, L. (1972). Voice timing in Korean stops. In A. Rigault & R. Charbonneau (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 439–446). The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Adriaans, F., & Swingley, D. (2012). Distributional learning of vowel categories is supported by prosody in infant-directed speech. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 34, 72–77.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. L., Morgan, J. L., & White, K. S. (2003). A statistical basis for speech sound discrimination. Language and Speech, 46, 155–182 doi: 10.1177/00238309030460020601
Google Scholar
Apfelbaum, K. S., & McMurray, B. (2011). Using variability to guide dimensional weighting: Associative mechanisms in early word learning. Cognitive Science, 35, 1105–1138 doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01181.x
Google Scholar
Buckler, H., Oczak-Arsic, S., Siddiqui, N., & Johnson, E. K. (2017). Input matters: Speed of word recognition in 2-year-olds exposed to multiple accents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 164, 87–100 doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.06.017
Google Scholar
Curtin, S., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2011). Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 492–504 doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2010.12.002
Google Scholar
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306.Google Scholar
Feldman, N. H., Myers, E. B., White, K. S., Griffiths, T. L., & Morgan, J. L. (2013). Word-level information influences phonetic learning in adults and infants. Cognition, 127, 427–438 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.02.007
Google Scholar
Garnica, O. K. (1973). The development of phonemic speech perception. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Göksun, T., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2010). Trading spaces: Carving up events for learning language. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 33–42 doi: 10.1177/1745691609356783
Google Scholar
Göksun, T., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Imai, M., Konishi, H., & Okada, H. (2011). Who is crossing where? Infants’ discrimination of figures and grounds in events. Cognition, 121, 176–195 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.07.002
Google Scholar
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexicosemantic system. Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81.Google Scholar
Hay, J., Graf Estes, K., Wang, T., & Saffran, J. (2015). From flexibility to constraint: The contrastive use of lexical tone in early word learning. Child Development, 86, 10–22 doi: 10.1111/cdev.12269
Google Scholar
Hollich, G. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Brand, R. J., Brown, E., Chung, H. L., … Rocroi, C. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65, i–135 doi: 10.1111/1540-5834.00090
Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning “gated” by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110–120 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00572.x
Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M., & Liu, H. M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 9096–9101 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1532872100
Google Scholar
Liu, L., & Kager, R. (2014). Perception of tones by infants learning a non-tone language. Cognition, 133, 385–394 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.004
Google Scholar
Mattock, K., & Burnham, D. (2006). Chinese and English infants’ tone perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization. Infancy, 10, 241–265 doi: 10.1207/s15327078in1003_3
Google Scholar
May, L., & Werker, J. F. (2014). Can a click be a word? Infants’ learning of non-native words. Infancy, 19, 281–300 doi: 10.1111/infa.12048
Google Scholar
Namy, L. L. (2001). What’s in a name when it isn’t a word? 17-month-olds’ mapping of nonverbal symbols to object categories. Infancy, 2, 73–86 doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0201_5
Google Scholar
Narayan, C. R., Werker, J. F., & Beddor, P. S. (2010). The interaction between acoustic salience and language experience in developmental speech perception: Evidence from nasal place discrimination. Developmental Science, 13, 407–420 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00898.x
Google Scholar
Newman, R. S. (2008). The level of detail in infants’ word learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 229–232 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00580.x
Google Scholar
Polka, L., & Werker, J. (1994). Developmental changes in perception of nonnative vowel contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 421–435 doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.2.421
Google Scholar
Rost, G. C., & McMurray, B. (2009). Speaker variability augments phonological processing in early word learning. Developmental Science, 12, 339–349 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00786.x
Google Scholar
Schvachkin, N. K. (1973). The development of phonemic speech perception in early childhood. In D. Ferguson & D. Slobin (Eds.), Studies of child language development (pp. 91–127). New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. (Original work published 1948).Google Scholar
Singh, L. (2008). Influences of high and low variability on infant word recognition. Cognition, 106, 833–870 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.002
Google Scholar
Singh, L., Tam, J. H., Chan, C., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014). Influences of vowel and tone variation on emergent word knowledge: A cross‐linguistic investigation. Developmental Science, 17, 94–109 doi: 10.1111/desc.12097
Google Scholar
Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks. Nature, 388, 381–382 doi: 10.1038/41102
Google Scholar
Swingley, D. (2009). Contributions of infant word learning to language development. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 3617–3622 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0107
Google Scholar
Swingley, D. (2016). Two-year-olds interpret novel phonological neighbors as familiar words. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1011–1023 doi: 10.1037/dev0000114
Google Scholar
Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13, 480–484 doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00485
Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. (2002). From speech perception to word learning and beyond. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 111, 2454 doi: 10.1121/1.4778462
Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49–63 doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80022-3
Google Scholar
White, K. S., & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Sub-segmental detail in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 114–132 doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2008.03.001
Google Scholar
White, K. S., Yee, E., Blumstein, S. E., & Morgan, J. L. (2013). Adults show less sensitivity to phonetic detail in unfamiliar words, too. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 362–378 doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.01.003
Google Scholar
Yeung, H. H., Chen, K. H., & Werker, J. F. (2013). When does native language input affect phonetic perception? The precocious case of lexical tone. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 123–139 doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.09.004
Google Scholar