Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T04:18:38.032Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The atypical pattern of irony comprehension in autistic children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2022

Francesca Panzeri*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Milan - Bicocca, Piazza dell’Ateneo Nuovo 1, 20126 Milano, Italy
Greta Mazzaggio
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, TN, Italy Center for Cognitive Science of Language, School of Humanities, University of Nova Gorica, Vipavska 13, SI-5000 Nova Gorica, Slovenia
Beatrice Giustolisi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Milan - Bicocca, Piazza dell’Ateneo Nuovo 1, 20126 Milano, Italy
Silvia Silleresi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Milan - Bicocca, Piazza dell’Ateneo Nuovo 1, 20126 Milano, Italy
Luca Surian
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, TN, Italy
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Nonliteral language understanding has always been recognized as problematic in autistic individuals. We ran a study on 26 autistic children (mean age = 7.3 years) and 2 comparison groups of typically developing children, 1 matched for chronological age, and 1 of younger peers (mean age = 6.11 years) matched for linguistic abilities, aiming at assessing their understanding of ironic criticisms and compliments, and identifying the cognitive and linguistic factors that may underpin this ability. Autistic participants lagged behind the comparison groups in the comprehension of both types of irony, and their performance was related to mindreading and linguistic abilities. Significant correlations were found between first-order Theory of Mind (ToM) and both types of irony, between second-order ToM and ironic compliments, and between linguistic abilities and ironic criticisms. The autistic group displayed an interesting, and previously unattested in the literature, bimodal distribution: the great majority of them (n = 18) displayed a very poor performance in irony understanding, whereas some (n = 6) were at ceiling. We discuss these results in terms of two different profiles of autistic children.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental condition characterized and diagnosed by impairments in social communication and social interaction and in the presence of restricted, repetitive behaviors or interests (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5, 2013). Even if structural language abilities are not taken into account for the diagnosis of autism, a significant heterogeneity in this competence is found: among the autistic individuals who develop language, some individuals present intact linguistic abilities, with performances similar to typically developing (TD) peers; others manifest mixed expressive/receptive language impairment, with a linguistic profile comparable to those of children with language disorder (Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, Reference Kjelgaard and Tager-Flusberg2001). It is important to note, although, that using specific tasks targeting computationally complex structures (see, for instance, Durrleman, Delage, et al., Reference Durrleman, Delage, Prévost and Tuller2017 for passives), even within the group of highly verbal children, some individuals appear not to encounter problems, whereas others show deficits, and these weaknesses seem not to be related to verbal memory skills (Meir & Novogrodsky, Reference Meir and Novogrodsky2020) or to nonverbal intelligence scores (Prévost et al., Reference Prévost, Tuller, Zebib, Barthez, Malvy and Bonnet-Brilhault2018).

Despite phenotypical variability in the severity and heterogeneity of linguistic and intelligence profiles of abilities (Silleresi et al., Reference Silleresi, Prevost, Zebib, Bonnet-Brilhault, Conte and Tuller2020; WHO, 2018), pragmatics is acknowledged as the most consistently and universally impaired linguistic domain in autism (Baron-Cohen, Reference Baron-Cohen1988; Dewey & Everard, Reference Dewey and Everard1974; Tager-Flusberg, Reference Tager-Flusberg1981; Tager-Flusberg et al., Reference Tager-Flusberg, Paul, Lord, Volkmar, Paul and Klin2005; Young et al., Reference Young, Diehl, Morris, Hyman and Bennetto2005), even in those individuals who score in the normal range on tests of IQ and display structural language skills in the norm. Within the broad area of pragmatics, individuals with autism encounter specific difficulties in those abilities that require reasoning about other persons’ communicative intentions. In fact, only some pragmatic tasks require the attribution of specific beliefs and intentions to the speaker, whereas others might be solved via the enrichment of what has been literally said, but without using mentalistic skills (see the discussion in Domaneschi & Bambini, Reference Domaneschi, Bambini, Fridland and Pavese2020). Recent studies found that the autistic condition permits to solve these more “linguistic pragmatic” tasks, such as interpreting indirect speech acts (Deliens et al., Reference Deliens, Papastamou, Ruytenbeek, Geelhand and Kissine2018) or deriving scalar implicatures (Andrés-Roqueta & Katsos, Reference Andrés-Roqueta and Katsos2020), whereas “social pragmatic” tasks such as those that require inferring speakers’ communicative intentions (e.g., lies, jokes, or pretense) remain difficult to solve.

As Geurts et al. (Reference Geurts, Kissine, van Tiel, Morsanyi and Byrne2020) reminds us, the problems in handling nonliteral language have been spelled out as a core feature of autism since the very beginning: Kanner (Reference Kanner1944) mentioned that autistic individuals tend to display excessive “literalness,” and Asperger (1944/Reference Asperger and Frith1991) suggested that they usually lack the understanding of jokes. The centrality of this deficit for the characterization of autism is also confirmed by its mention in the latest edition of the DSM-5: one of the diagnostic criteria for autism is the difficulty in understanding what is not explicitly stated (e.g., making inferences) and nonliteral or ambiguous meanings of language (e.g., idioms, humor, metaphors, multiple meanings that depend on the context for interpretation).

Nonliteralness can be broadly defined as the discrepancy between the linguistic meaning and the conveyed meaning of an utterance. Typically, the term nonliteral language refers to figurative modes of speech, such as irony, jokes, metaphor, hyperbole, or understatement (Gibbs, Reference Gibbs1994). Recent reviews of studies on figurative language in the autistic population confirmed a general impairment compared to TD comparison groups in the interpretation of idioms and proverbs (Morsanyi & Stamenković, Reference Morsanyi and Stamenković2021) and of metonymies, metaphors, and irony (Kalandadze et al., Reference Kalandadze, Norbury, Nærland and Næss2018; Melogno et al., Reference Melogno, Pinto and Levi2012). However, the nature of the difficulties autistic individuals encounter in handling this type of language is still a matter of debate. In her pioneering work, Happé (Reference Happé1993) found that autistic children were impaired in both metaphors and irony and attributed their difficulties to deficits in basic (for metaphors) or advanced (for irony) mentalistic abilities. Subsequent studies, however, questioned the role of mindreading in metaphor understanding and instead highlighted the contribution of linguistic knowledge (Norbury, Reference Norbury2005). In fact, Kalandadze et al. (Reference Kalandadze, Norbury, Nærland and Næss2018) emphasized how the impairment in figurative speech was marginal or even nonsignificant when autistic individuals were compared to TD peers matched for language skills. At the same time, Vulchanova et al. (Reference Vulchanova, Saldaña, Chahboun and Vulchanov2015) underlined that the existence of highly verbal autistic children who still exhibit difficulties in metaphor or idiom comprehension calls for other possible explanations, and they pointed to weak central coherence, a core feature of the autistic dimension. A recent and lively debate concerns precisely the question of whether metaphors might be correctly understood thanks to sufficient lexical competence but without an egocentric perspective that does not take into consideration the speaker’s point of view. Kissine (Reference Kissine2021), in particular, considers the autistic condition as an ideal testing ground and concludes that since autistic participants with preserved linguistic skills can understand metaphors, these do not necessarily require mindreading skills. On the other hand, Kalandadze et al. (Reference Kalandadze, Bambini and Næss2019) emphasize the role of different aspects of the task used to test metaphor comprehension, and along the same lines Mazzarella & Noveck (Reference Mazzarella and Noveck2021) underline how the full appreciation of some metaphors might in fact require perspective-taking skills.

Quite interestingly, however, even if it is highly debated whether other types of figurative language necessarily require mindreading, almost all scholars believe that irony, the main focus of our study, is indeed a social pragmatic phenomenon and that its comprehension requires mentalistic skills (for a notable exception, see Katsos & Andrés-Roqueta, Reference Katsos and Andrés-Roqueta2021, who propose that, under particular situations, an egocentric appreciation of irony may take place).

Since the seminal work of Happé (Reference Happé1993), autistic individuals have been considered seriously impaired in irony comprehension, as they have difficulties going beyond the literal meaning in order to identify the speaker’s real intention. This deficit was related to difficulties in mindreading, which were proposed to be a distinguishing and core feature of autism. In particular, it was claimed that the inability to assume another person’s perspective affected the recognition of the speaker’s ironic intent.

In recent years, however, a growing body of research has highlighted that some autistic individuals could—at the behavioral level—successfully pass tasks designed to tackle both meta-representational cognitive abilities and pragmatic skills connected to sarcasm or irony understanding (Colich et al., Reference Colich, Wang, Rudie, Hernandez, Bookheimer and Dapretto2012; Glenwright & Agbayewa, Reference Glenwright and Agbayewa2012). The debate thus moved to ascertaining which cognitive mechanisms might help autistic individuals successfully pass mindreading and pragmatic tasks (Colich et al., Reference Colich, Wang, Rudie, Hernandez, Bookheimer and Dapretto2012; Pexman et al. Reference Pexman, Rostad, McMorris, Climie, Stowkowy and Glenwright2011; for a more general theoretical debate, see Kissine, Reference Kissine2021). To tackle this issue, we first outline a characterization of verbal irony and its acquisition in children with typical development or with autism, and then we present a new experimental study.

Verbal irony

As already alluded to, verbal irony is a type of nonliteral language in which what a speaker intends to communicate (the speaker’s meaning) clashes with the literal meaning conveyed by the sentence and is typically (even if not always, Wilson & Sperber, Reference Wilson and Sperber2012) the opposite of what was said (the sentence meaning). Thus, if after an extremely boring party Ann says to Chloe “The party was great fun!”, she is communicating her contemptuous attitude toward the idea that the party was fun, and any interlocutor who correctly recognizes her communicative intent will come to believe that Ann thinks that the party was anything but fun (speaker’s meaning). In this sense, she is in fact making a criticism (speaker’s attitude). Indeed, ironic comments are always evaluative, and even if the most common form of irony is represented by literally positive remarks that are used to blame (ironic criticisms, also defined as sarcasm), the reverse is also possible. A literally negative statement can be ironically used to communicate its opposite meaning, as when Ann comments to Bill who got straight A’s, “Your exam went very badly.” In addition to being rarer, ironic compliments are harder to understand and to appreciate, at least when they do not echo a preceding statement (thus, the preceding example becomes more natural in a context in which Bill manifested his fear that the exam would go very badly; Wilson & Sperber, Reference Wilson and Sperber2012). This difference has been linked to the so-called asymmetry of affect: ironic criticisms are easier (and more widespread) because they echo a positive expectation that can be easily retrieved even if not explicitly stated; on the other hand, ironic compliments make reference to negative expectations that do not constitute the standard (Kreuz & Link, Reference Kreuz and Link2002).

Assuming the hearer’s perspective, the most powerful cue to detect the speaker’s ironic intent is the incongruence between the context and the statement (see, a.o., Rivière et al., Reference Rivière, Klein and Champagne-Lavau2018), even if ironic compliments might require the presence of an explicit statement to be correctly perceived. To avoid misunderstandings, the ironic speaker can (but does not need to) also display the so-called ironic markers, that is, meta-communicative cues that alert the audience that his or her comment should not be interpreted literally. In addition to particular linguistic choices (e.g., extreme adjectives or syntactic constructions such as topicalization), the ironic speaker can display specific gestural or phonological cues, such as the ironic tone of voice (Burgers & van Mulken, Reference Burgers, van Mulken and Attardo2017).

The acquisition of irony

The complexity of the mechanisms required to comprehend irony is reflected in its late acquisition by TD children. Studies that investigated the comprehension of irony within a developmental perspective found that TD children start comprehending irony around the age of 5 years, but its full appreciation may not be realized until up to 8 years of age (Creusere, Reference Creusere2000; Dews et al., Reference Dews, Winner, Kaplan, Rosenblatt, Hunt, Lim, McGovern, Qualter and Smarsh1996; Filippova & Astington, Reference Filippova and Astington2008; Harris & Pexman, Reference Harris and Pexman2003, a.o.). The correct detection of the speaker’s meaning has been reported to precede the inference of the speaker’s attitude (Hancock et al., Reference Hancock, Dunham and Purdy2000). Moreover, children’s comprehension of ironic criticisms usually precedes that of ironic compliments (Pexman & Glenwright, Reference Pexman and Glenwright2007). Various proposals have been put forth to explain the acquisition asymmetry between ironic criticisms and compliments. As already noted, ironic criticisms are more common than ironic compliments and thus might constitute more conventionalized forms of irony: children might have more often encountered instances of sarcasm during their normal conversational exchanges, and thus, they might be keener to recognize and appreciate them (Pexman & Glenwright, Reference Pexman and Glenwright2007). Other scholars emphasized that ironic compliments are particularly challenging because they require the negation of a (inherently) negative statement (Giora, Reference Giora1995), and the processing of a double negation might be too demanding for children. More generally, the asymmetry in acquisition might simply reflect the asymmetry of affect previously discussed: ironic criticisms are indeed easier than ironic compliments (also for adults) because they make reference to positive expectations about the outcome of an event (e.g., the party will be fun), whereas ironic compliments refer to negative expectancies (e.g., the exam will go badly). As human beings, we tend to have positive feelings about how things will go, and children might possibly be even more optimistic. Therefore, it is easier to retrieve positive antecedents compared to negative ones (Kumon-Nakamura et al., Reference Kumon-Nakamura, Glucksberg and Brown1995). In fact, it has been shown that if ironic compliments echo an explicitly stated negative antecedent, children do not find them harder than ironic criticisms (Hancock et al., Reference Hancock, Dunham and Purdy2000, exp. 2; Nakassis & Snedeker, Reference Nakassis and Snedeker2002).

Many scholars have investigated the factors that might promote the development of the recognition of irony. Sullivan et al. (Reference Sullivan, Winner and Hopfield1995) argued that sophisticated cognitive abilities connected to the so-called Theory of Mind (ToM) are required to distinguish lies from jokes. ToM is an umbrella term that refers to the mentalistic abilities required to attribute and reason about others’ mental states, such as emotions, intentions, and beliefs. This mindreading competence is crucial to interpret and predict people’s behavior and to maintain good social relations; it has also been linked to the recognition of the speaker’s communicative intent. Let us again take the example of the ironic criticism regarding the boring party that Ann attended. When Ann comments to Chloe, “The party was great fun!”, she is saying something that is false in that situation. If Ann thinks that Chloe knows that the party was not at all fun, then she wants Chloe to recognize her jocular intent. If, on the other hand, Ann thinks that Chloe does not know that the party was boring, then she wants to mislead her and she is lying. In other words, to distinguish lies from jokes, an advanced second-order ToM is required (i.e., predicting one person’s state of mind about another person’s state of mind), since the speaker must hold a belief about his or her interlocutor’s state of knowledge. Indeed, different scholars linked children’s difficulties in irony recognition to their still immature mentalistic skills and found a correlation between advanced ToM levels and irony understanding in TD children (Nilsen et al., Reference Nilsen, Glenwright and Huyder2011; Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Winner and Hopfield1995; Winner & Leekam, Reference Winner and Leekam1991). At the same time, the development of ToM is tightly linked to language proficiency (Milligan et al., Reference Milligan, Astington and Dack2007), and studies that controlled for language competence found that not only ToM but also language correlated with irony comprehension in TD children (Filippova & Astington, Reference Filippova and Astington2008). Angeleri and Airenti (Reference Angeleri and Airenti2014) hypothesized that the relation between ToM and irony was spurious and resulted from language skills affecting both ToM and irony.

Irony comprehension in autism

One of the defining characteristics of autism is the presence of persistent deficits in social communication and social interaction, and it is therefore not surprising that the area of pragmatic communication has been extensively investigated in this population.

Focusing on irony comprehension, apparently contrasting results have been reported. Indeed, the interpretive skills required for irony appreciation are some of the skills thought to be particularly problematic for autistic individuals. In particular, it has been suggested that the deficit in comprehension of ironic language found among autistic individuals may be related to the presence of coexisting cognitive difficulties in the areas of ToM (Happé, Reference Happé1993; Peterson et al., Reference Peterson, Wellman and Slaughter2012; Tager-Flusberg, Reference Tager-Flusberg2000), in keeping track of diverse perspectives (Kissine, Reference Kissine2012) and in structural language skills (vocabulary and syntax) (Norbury, Reference Norbury2004, Reference Norbury2005; Whyte et al., Reference Whyte, Nelson and Scherf2014). Happé (Reference Happé1993) conducted the first experimental research to make explicit connections between ToM and figurative language comprehension in individuals with ASD. She found that only the autistic participants (with heterogeneous verbal and nonverbal intelligence scores) who passed both first and second-order ToM tasks could understand ironic utterances. MacKay and Shaw (Reference MacKay and Shaw2004) reported that when asked to interpret ironic utterances, children with high-functioning autism tended to offer rephrasing of the statement as answers or provided explanations that involved reinterpreting the context to make the literal meaning fit. Martin and McDonald (Reference Martin and McDonald2004) tested individuals with Asperger syndrome (AS) presenting them short stories that ended with a lie or a joke and that contained questions about (first- and second-order) mental attributions and about the speaker’s meaning and intent. They found a delay compared to TD comparison groups and a correlation between ToM and identification of the speaker’s communicative intent (even if, since questions were presented within the same stories, this effect might be due to a search for coherence). Similar findings were found by Kaland et al. (Reference Kaland, Møller-Nielsen, Callesen, Mortensen, Gottlieb and Smith2002): they tested adolescents with AS on various tasks targeting, among other things, (white) lies, misunderstanding, double bluff, and irony, and they found an impairment in the questions requiring justification of the corresponding mental state. Saban-Bezalel et al. (Reference Saban-Bezalel, Dolfin, Laor and Mashal2019) compared the performance in irony comprehension between 20 autistic children and a comparison group of TD children matched not only for age, vocabulary, and executive functions but also for ToM (autistic children succeeded on the second-order ToM task 80% of the time). They found that even if autistic children showed a remarkable ability in the appreciation of the comic strips (79% accuracy), their performance still lagged behind that of TD peers (89%). Interestingly, the difference between the two groups disappeared when other mentalizing abilities assessed with the Hinting Test (which requires understanding other persons’ intentions) were taken into account. Deliens et al. (Reference Deliens, Papastamou, Ruytenbeek, Geelhand and Kissine2018) tested autistic adolescents and young adults without language impairment or intellectual disability (their verbal and nonverbal intelligence scores were above 70) in a task that required recognizing the (sincere or) ironic intent of a statement using three different cues: incongruence with the context, prosodic information, and facial expressions (cues presented cumulatively and in isolation). Autistic participants detected the ironic intended meaning to a lesser extent than TD controls but relied on the same cues (context incongruence more than acoustical or visual cues). Deliens et al. also collected eye-tracking data and found that the autism group fixated the incorrect object longer than the comparison group.

In the last decade, several studies using implicit or neural measures of irony comprehension have been conducted, and interesting findings have emerged. Pexman et al. (Reference Pexman, Rostad, McMorris, Climie, Stowkowy and Glenwright2011) tested 18 children and adolescents with high-functioning autism (with a mean age of 11 years) and 2 comparison groups matched for chronological and linguistic age (LA) on a task assessing ironic criticisms and ironic compliments. They found no delay compared to the comparison TD groups on the questions assessing the speaker’s meaning or the speaker’s intent—possibly because the requested answers (forced choice in the first case and selection of an object in the second case) posed minimal verbal demands; nevertheless, the group with autism provided lower humor ratings for the ironic remarks, suggesting that they did not appreciate the social functions of irony. Moreover, response latency and eye gaze measures suggested that autistic individuals were processing irony differently from their TD peers.

The atypical processing of sarcasm in autistic individuals is also confirmed by studies that use neuroimaging techniques (functional magnetic resonance imaging) to discover the neural mechanisms involved in the elaboration of sarcasm. All these studies converge in highlighting that, even if children and adolescents with high-functioning autism did not differ from their TD peers in behavioral responses to ironic remarks (Colich et al., Reference Colich, Wang, Rudie, Hernandez, Bookheimer and Dapretto2012; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Cherkassky, Mason, Keller, Minshew and Just2013) or had a nevertheless satisfactory performance (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Lee, Sigman and Dapretto2006), their brain activity was atypical compared to TD peers. Wang et al. (Reference Wang, Lee, Sigman and Dapretto2006) found that even if the neural networks involved in the processing of the speaker’s sincere or ironic intent were the same in children with and without autism, the autistic group showed hyperactivity in the right frontal and bilateral temporal regions, and the increased cortical recruitment was interpreted as reflecting a compensatory mechanism and/or increased neural effort needed to perform the task. Additionally, Colich et al. (Reference Colich, Wang, Rudie, Hernandez, Bookheimer and Dapretto2012) found that, despite the strong similarities in the neural networks activated for the processing of ironic items, the brain activity of autistic children was considerably more bilaterally distributed and more widespread, extending to regions such as the left temporal pole and the medial prefrontal cortex, typically engaged during mentalizing tasks. Williams et al. (Reference Williams, Cherkassky, Mason, Keller, Minshew and Just2013) found activation differences in key language-processing regions (left middle temporal, left pars triangularis, left pars opercularis, left medial frontal, and right middle temporal) in the autism group compared to the TD group. Specifically, the authors found that participants on the autism spectrum (both children and adults) showed lower coordination within the left hemisphere language network during irony comprehension than controls. However, in contrast to Colich et al., autistic participants did not show an increase in functional connectivity for ironic texts compared to literal texts. Such a difference in results might be due to a difference in tasks, since in Colich et al.’s (Reference Colich, Wang, Rudie, Hernandez, Bookheimer and Dapretto2012) study, children were explicitly asked to interpret a character’s communicative intentions, and this more demanding task might have led to a recruitment of this brain region (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Cherkassky, Mason, Keller, Minshew and Just2013: 299). Finally, both Colich et al. (Reference Colich, Wang, Rudie, Hernandez, Bookheimer and Dapretto2012) and Williams et al. (Reference Williams, Cherkassky, Mason, Keller, Minshew and Just2013) detected less activation in the left pars triangularis region in autistic children. As highlighted by Williams et al. (Reference Williams, Cherkassky, Mason, Keller, Minshew and Just2013), this region has been associated with semantic processing, and a lack of an increase in activation might be associated with a diminished appreciation of the ironic information.

To summarize, even if impairments in figurative understanding in autism are supported by overwhelming evidence, some issues need to be addressed. Regarding the factors that might promote irony comprehension, many studies found correlations between ToM and sarcasm understanding, but only a few studies conducted on autistic participants investigated whether linguistic skills might intervene as a mediating factor. This is particularly relevant because recent research has suggested a close developmental link between ToM and language skills (Angeleri & Airenti, Reference Angeleri and Airenti2014), in particular, mastery of complement clauses (Durrleman, Burnel, et al., Reference Durrleman, Burnel and Reboul2017). Moreover, except for Pexman et al. (Reference Pexman, Rostad, McMorris, Climie, Stowkowy and Glenwright2011), previous studies have tested only one type of verbal irony, which is sarcasm. Even if they are less common than ironic criticisms, ironic compliments might constitute an interesting testing ground because they might highlight similarities and differences in the way autistic or TD children derive the speaker’s meaning and intent. Finally, some recent studies found that autistic individuals showed a good understanding of irony, obtaining overall scores above chance level or even comparable to those of the TD comparison groups (Colich et al., Reference Colich, Wang, Rudie, Hernandez, Bookheimer and Dapretto2012; Pexman et al., Reference Pexman, Rostad, McMorris, Climie, Stowkowy and Glenwright2011; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Lee, Sigman and Dapretto2006; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Cherkassky, Mason, Keller, Minshew and Just2013). A more in-depth investigation of the profile of autistic individuals who pass these irony tasks might shed light on the factors that promote its understanding and thus offer valuable indications for rehabilitation programs that might help them develop this ability.

The present study

We conducted a study that aimed to test the understanding of ironic criticisms and ironic compliments in autistic children, comparing their performance to two comparison groups of TD children, one matched on chronological age (CA) and the other matched on linguistic competence. In particular, we were interested in identifying what might facilitate irony comprehension, and thus, we investigated the links with both ToM skills and linguistic abilities.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-six autistic children (24 male and 2 female) with a mean age of 7.26 years (SD = 2.02, range = 3.75–10.25 years) and 52 TD children took part in the study. All participants lived in Italy and were native speakers of Italian. Participants with autism were recruited and diagnosed at the Hospital ‘Azienda Provinciale per i Servizi Sanitari’ (APSS) in Trento, Italy, and were included in the study if they were considered verbal children according to the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., Reference Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, Risi, Gotham and Bishop2012) and DSM-5 (2013) criteria by medical professionals. The individual and mean scores obtained in the ADOS are reported in Table 1, along with their scores on the language test (Test di comprensione grammaticale of the BVL, Marini et al., Reference Marini, Marotta, Bulgheroni and Fabbro2015) and nonverbal IQ test (Italian standardization of the Raven Colored Progressive Matrices (Raven), Belacchi et al., Reference Belacchi, Scalisi, Cannoni and Cornoldi2008). These tests are described in detail below.

Table 1. Characteristics of the autistic participants: gender, age (in months), and scores at the ADOS, Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (Raven), and grammatical comprehension task (BVL). Participants are listed ordered by age

H Participants that will fit into the “at-ceiling group,” see the Individual analysis in the Results section; L Participants that will fit into the “at-floor group,” see the Individual analysis in the Results section.

Autistic children were paired one by one with a group of CA-matched TD children (CA, N = 26, 17 male and 9 female, mean age = 7.26, SD = 2.02, range = 4–10.25) and with another group of TD children paired one by one based on their score in the grammatical comprehension task of the BVL (Marini et al., Reference Marini, Marotta, Bulgheroni and Fabbro2015) and thus matched for LA (N = 26, 14 male and 12 female, mean age = 6.11, SD = 1.70, range = 4.08–9.83). Children in the LA group were significantly younger than the autistic children (t = −2.15, p = .03).

The TD children were recruited in kindergartens and primary schools in the provinces of Vicenza and Verona (Italy). This study is a part of extensive research on pragmatic deficits in the autistic population that has been approved by the University of Trento’s Ethical Committee, and all parents provided written informed consent prior to beginning the experiment.

All children were also tested for fluid nonverbal intelligence using the Italian standardization of the Raven Colored Progressive Matrices (Belacchi et al., Reference Belacchi, Scalisi, Cannoni and Cornoldi2008). This test comprises 3 sets of 12 items (maximum score = 36), with increasing difficulty and complexity within and across sets. Children are presented with a pattern in which a piece is missing, and their task is to choose, among six alternatives, the piece that best completes the matrix. To achieve this task, children must infer rules, manage a hierarchy of goals, and form high-level abstractions (Carpenter et al., Reference Carpenter, Just and Shell1990). Linguistic abilities were assessed with the grammar comprehension task of the BVL (Marini et al., Reference Marini, Marotta, Bulgheroni and Fabbro2015). Children had to select the picture, out of four alternatives, that corresponds to the sentence pronounced by the researcher. In 40 items, the test allows an assessment of several morphosyntactic features that involve the expression of gender, number (singular/plural), clitic pronouns, negation, passive structures, and relative clauses.

For general linguistic abilities, ASD and LA were matched one by one for scores in the grammatical task, whereas ASD scored significantly lower than CA (t = 2.47, p = .02). On the other hand, there were no significant differences between the ASD and the two TD groups in the Raven scores (all ps > .3). The characteristics of the three groups are summarized in Table 2.

Table 2. Characteristics of the three groups of participants: mean age (in months) and mean raw scores and standard deviations at the Raven, the grammatical comprehension task (BVL) for the group of children with ASD and for the two TD groups matched for chronological age (CA) and for linguistic age (LA)

Materials

Complete experimental materials are available in the OSF repository (https://osf.io/wydpc/?view_only=3e6dc187c26942e98eb175c12e30138e).

ToM tasks

We tested children’s mentalistic abilities using one task targeting first-order ToM and another task (with three test questions) targeting second-order ToM. First-order ToM was evaluated with an unexpected-contents task (Gopnik & Astington, Reference Gopnik and Astington1988): children were shown a Band-Aid box that actually contained playing cards and were asked to say what another person who had not opened the box would say if asked “What’s inside the Band-Aid box?”. To answer correctly (i.e., “Band-Aids”), the child has to demonstrate their ability to understand that people can have a false belief. We assessed second-order ToM with a modified version of Enrique’s Birthday Party (Hutchins et al., Reference Hutchins, Prelock and Bonazinga-Bouyea2014) that included target questions assessing second-order ignorance (Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Winner and Hopfield1995) and second-order false belief (requiring a forced choice answer and its justification). For ToM tasks, the score was 0/1 for first-order ToM and ranged from 0 to 3 (one point each for correct answers to the second-order ignorance question, second-order false belief, and its justification) for second-order ToM.

Irony comprehension task

To assess irony comprehension, we used an adapted version of the task used by Panzeri et al. (Reference Panzeri, Giustolisi and Zampini2020; Reference Panzeri, Cavicchiolo, Giustolisi, Di Berardino, Ajmone, Vizziello, Donnini and Zanetti2021) that consists of 10 short stories involving 2 characters interacting and with a concluding remark that needs to be interpreted either literally (control stories: N = 4) or ironically (ironic compliments: N = 3; ironic criticisms: N = 3). The task was created on PowerPoint and presented on a laptop computer; the stories were recorded to avoid uncontrolled and unwanted changes in prosody. The final remarks were pronounced with the corresponding sincere or ironic intonation, and the task was presented auditorily to the participants, accompanied by some images to capture the children’s attention.

After hearing each story, children had to answer three questions that were meant to evaluate i) detection of the speaker’s meaning (to assess if the child correctly interpreted the final remark as literal or ironic), ii) context recognition (control), and iii) understanding of the speaker’s attitude (to assess if the child correctly interpreted the final remark as a compliment or a criticism). The second context recognition question was used to check whether participants understood what truly happened in the story; they were asked to choose the correct picture among four (the target, the competitor, and two distractors). Examples of ironic stories and related test questions are reported in Table 3 (ironic criticism) and Table 4 (ironic compliment).

Table 3. Example of a story in the ironic criticism condition (English translation)

Table 4. Example of a story in the ironic compliments condition (English translation)

All stories (literal and ironic) were presented in a pseudorandomized order: Two stories of the same type (e.g., two ironic criticisms) were never presented consecutively.

Procedure

All children were tested individually in a quiet room of their kindergarten or school (TD children) or in the Hospital ‘Azienda Provinciale per i Servizi Sanitari’ (APSS) in Trento (autistic children). The testing lasted for approximately 1 hr; autistic children and preschoolers were tested in two different sessions to avoid participants becoming tired or distracted. The tasks were administered in a fixed order: Raven, ToM tasks, grammatical BVL task, and irony task.

Results

Complete analysis code and data are available in the OSF repository (https://osf.io/wydpc/?view_only=3e6dc187c26942e98eb175c12e30138e).

The results are divided into several sections, one for the type of analysis. All statistical analyses were conducted with R version 4.0.3.

ToM abilities

The results are reported in Table 5. The first-order ToM score was significantly lower in the ASD group than in the two TD comparison groups (ASD vs. CA: z = 3.87, p = .0001; ASD vs. LA: z = 3.66, p = .0002). Additionally, the second-order score was significantly lower in the ASD group than in the two TD groups (ASD vs. CA: t = 4.85, p < .0001; ASD vs. LA: t = 3.10, p = .0027).

Table 5. ToM abilities: aggregate results (mean and standard deviations) for first-order ToM (ToM 1, maximum score 1) and second-order ToM (ToM 2, maximum score 3)

Irony comprehension task: Ironic versus literal stories

First, we compared literal and ironic stories considering all three questions (speaker’s meaning, context, and speaker’s attitude). Accuracy was analyzed as a dichotomous variable (1 = correct/0 = wrong) by means of mixed effects logistic regressions. Significance tests were performed through likelihood ratio comparisons between models. Considering the three groups, our comparisons of interest were between ASD children and the two TD groups (CA and LA).

The main results are illustrated in Figure 1. Accuracy in literal stories was high in all three groups (ASD M = .99, SD = .05; CA M = .97, SD = .06; and LA M = .91, SD = .19), whereas accuracy dropped in ironic stories, with the lowest accuracy in the ASD group (M = .31, SD = .4), the highest accuracy in the CA group (M = .79, SD = .28), and an intermediate score in the LA group (M = .58, SD = .28).

Figure 1. Boxplots representing the distribution of participants mean accuracy (y-axis) in the three groups (x-axis) in ironic (white) and literal (gray) stories. The “X” symbols represent mean values.

We performed a logistic regression analysis comparing two models with the following random structure: by-participants and by-items varying intercepts, and by-participants varying slopes for the ironic/literal variable. The first model was a full model with group, type of story (ironic/literal), and their interaction, and the second model dropped the interaction term. The comparison between the two models indicated that the interaction was significant (χ 2 (2) = 10.41, p = .005).

To investigate the interaction, we performed a post hoc analysis considering the following comparisons: ASD versus CA and ASD vs. LA in ironic and literal stories and ironic vs. literal stories in the three groups. The results showed that the difference in accuracy between the ironic and literal stories was significant in all three groups. Moreover, accuracy on ironic stories was significantly lower in the ASD group than in the two TD groups, whereas the difference in accuracy in literal stories was not significant (Table 6).

Table 6. Ironic versus literal stories analysis: results summary

Irony comprehension task: Ironic criticisms versus ironic compliments

In this section, we focus on the comparison between ironic criticisms and ironic compliments. In this analysis, we considered only the two questions directly linked to irony comprehension (speaker’s meaning and speaker’s attitude), and we directly compared the performance in these two questions. As in the previous section, answers to each question were coded as 1, correct, or 0, wrong, and the dichotomous variable accuracy was analyzed by means of mixed effects logistic regressions. Our comparisons of interest were between ASD children and CA and LA children.

In all groups, accuracy was lower in ironic compliments compared to ironic criticisms, with the greatest difference in the LA group (ASD compliments: M = .27, SD = .43; criticisms: M = .34, SD = .4; CA compliments: M = .74, SD = .37; criticisms: M = .83, SD = .27; LA compliments: M = .46, SD = .4; criticisms: M = .70, SD = .33; see also Figure 2).

Figure 2. Boxplots representing the distribution of participants mean accuracy (y-axis) in the three groups (x-axis) in ironic compliments (white) and ironic criticisms (gray). The “X” symbols represent mean values.

We performed a logistic regression analysis comparing several models with by-participants varying intercepts. The first model was a full model with group, type of irony (compliments/criticisms), question (meaning/attitude), and their interaction, including the three-way interaction. Then, we removed one by one the predictors that did not significantly contribute to the models’ goodness of fit (Table 7). A summary of the final model is given in Table 8. Confirming the previous analysis, accuracy in ironic stories was significantly higher in CA children than in ASD children and in LA children than in ASD children. Moreover, across groups, accuracy was higher in criticisms than in compliments. No significant difference was found between the meaning and attitude questions.

Table 7. Irony comprehension task: model’s selection summary

Table 8. Ironic compliments versus ironic criticisms analysis: results summary

Irony comprehension task: Individual analysis

The analysis thus far considered all 26 ASD participants as a group. As seen in Figures 1 and 2, the performance within the group was extremely variable; therefore, in this section, we present individual results. As in Panzeri et al. (Reference Panzeri, Cavicchiolo, Giustolisi, Di Berardino, Ajmone, Vizziello, Donnini and Zanetti2021), we calculated an ironic criticism and an ironic compliment score. We assigned one point for each correct answer to the two irony-related questions (detection of speaker’s meaning and of speaker’s attitude) in each of the three stories per type of irony. Then, each individual had a score (max 6 points) for ironic criticisms and one (max 6 points) for ironic compliments. As Figure 3 shows, the score distribution was not homogeneous: 12/26 participants (46%) scored 0/6 both in ironic compliments and criticisms, and 6/26 participants (23%) scored 6/6. Of the remaining eight participants, the vast majority had a score of 0 in compliments and a very low score (1 or 2) in criticisms.

Figure 3. Irony scores – individual ASD results in compliments (white bar) and criticisms (gray bar).

To draw a comparison, we also considered individual scores for TD children. As one can see in Figure 4, it is clear that the situation is quite different: first, no TD child had a score of 0 both in compliments and in criticisms, and second, the distribution from low to high scores looks smoother than for ASD children (this is visible considering the LA group, since many participants of the CA group performed at ceiling).

Figure 4. Irony scores – individual TD results in compliments (white bar) and criticisms (gray bar). To a better visualization, we kept the two TD groups separated (top: CA; bottom: LA).

Relationship between irony comprehension and cognitive or linguistic factors in autistic children

To better understand the pattern of results of the ASD group, in the last step of the analysis, we investigated the relationship between age, morphosyntactic (BVL) score, first- (ToM 1) and second-order (ToM 2) ToM scores, ADOS score, and performance in the irony task.

We performed two different analyses. First, we considered all ASD participants, and we ran an exploratory correlation analysis between the scores of compliments and criticisms and the various biographical, linguistics, and cognitive measures. To obtain a baseline, we performed the correlation analysis also considering TD children (taking both groups, CA and LA, together).

Then, to account for the bimodal distribution characterizing the performance of ASD children described in the previous section, we classified the ASD participants according to their performance in the irony task. Children with a score of a maximum of 2 correct answers out of 12 were grouped together (“at-floor group,” 18 participants), and children with a score of at least 10 correct answers out of 12 were combined in a second group (“at-ceiling group,” 6 participants. They all scored 12/12). Therefore, two participants were not considered. Subsequently, we compared the two groups considering the same biographical, linguistic, and cognitive measures used in the correlation analysis.

Spearman’s correlation analysis

Spearman’s coefficients for TD children are reported in Table 9, and those for ASD children are reported in Table 10. Significant correlations are highlighted in bold.

Table 9. TD group (N = 52), Spearman’s correlation coefficients; significant correlations are in bold (*** = p < .0001; ** = p < .001; * = p < .05)

Table 10. ASD group (N = 26), Spearman’s correlation coefficients; significant correlations are in bold (*** = p < .0001; ** = p < .001; * = p < .05)

Even if these analyses do not permit a direct comparison between the ASD and TD populations, it is interesting to note that the overall picture is different. As expected, in the groups of TD children (Table 9), improvement in all areas was linked to age (except for first-order ToM because of an at-ceiling effect). Focusing on irony comprehension, correlations with age, linguistic competence, and even nonverbal intelligence were found for both types of irony, whereas scores in second-order ToM were correlated with ironic compliments only. In the ASD group, however, the pattern was different. Age did not have any impact on irony comprehension; grammatical skills were related to ironic criticisms only, and mindreading skills were positively correlated with accuracy in both types of irony, even if the correlation between second-order ToM and ironic criticisms did not reach statistical significance.

At-floor versus at-ceiling ASD group

Descriptive statistics about the two groups are provided in Table 1.

At first glance, strong differences between the two groups were found in the two ToM scores. To evaluate our first observations and statistically compare the two groups, we ran a series of unpaired two-sample Wilcoxon tests. The results confirm that participants of the “at-ceiling group” significantly outperformed participants of the “at-floor group” in both ToM measures (ToM 1 and ToM 2 scores), whereas the other measures did not significantly differ between the two groups (Table 12).

Discussion

Our aim was to investigate the comprehension of ironic criticisms and ironic compliments in autistic children. By relying on previous results, we predicted significant delays in this competence, which involves the attribution of mental states and intentions to the speaker. In contrast to other studies (see, a.o., Hancock et al., Reference Hancock, Dunham and Purdy2000; Pexman & Glenwright, Reference Pexman and Glenwright2007), we did not find a difference in accuracy between questions assessing the speaker’s meaning and the speaker’s attitude: if a participant correctly detected the speaker’s intended meaning, then they would also correctly infer the speaker’s praising or blaming attitude. This might depend on the fact that, unlike other studies that required children to rate speaker’s level of meanness/niceness using a Likert scale, we proposed a forced choice question (Was the speaker making a compliment or criticism?). On the other hand, we were also interested in identifying the factors that might promote the development of this skill. The results showed that our sample of autistic children lagged behind TD children, not only those with the same CA but also the younger ones matched for language level. This finding contrasts with what emerged in the review of Kalandadze et al. (Reference Kalandadze, Norbury, Nærland and Næss2018), who noticed that the performance of autistic participants in figurative language tasks was in fact only marginally different, or even comparable, to that of TD peers matched for LA. However, Chahboun et al. (Reference Chahboun, Vulchanov, Saldaña, Eshuis and Vulchanova2016) found that their sample of high-functioning autistic participants lagged behind TD peers matched for language competence in the comprehension of metaphors.

Importantly, autistic children encountered specific problems in the detection of ironic remarks but correctly interpreted all stories that required a literal interpretation. Statistical analyses highlighted that ironic compliments were harder to understand than ironic criticisms, and this was the case for all groups of participants. Notably, even though the final remark of our task always echoed a preceding statement (in the story reported in Table 3, for instance, the mother’s final comment “You are a real bungler!” referred to the daughter’s fear that she would bungle things), still making the negative expectation explicit was not enough to override the asymmetry of affect. These results differed from the findings reported in Hancock et al. (Reference Hancock, Dunham and Purdy2000) and Nakassis and Snedeker (Reference Nakassis and Snedeker2002). Thus, our findings confirm that ironic compliments are inherently more difficult to recognize, and we assume that their complexity is reflected in their being uncommon. If familiarity with instances of irony were to facilitate its recognition, we could expect a correlation between age and accuracy in irony recognition, since the older children get, the more likely it is for them to encounter ironic exchanges. This is what we found for the group of TD children, with age linked to the comprehension of both ironic criticisms and ironic compliments. In addition to irony, grammatical abilities and nonverbal intelligence also ameliorate, together with second-order ToM. This “typicality” in the development of TD children is what makes it difficult to disentangle the factors that promote the mastering of irony comprehension: In addition to age, verbal and nonverbal intelligence were also related to both types of irony. Quite surprisingly, however, in the TD groups, mentalistic abilities were not correlated with ironic criticism comprehension, and only a significant correlation was found in the case of second-order ToM and ironic compliments.

When we turn to the group of ASD children, the picture is rather different. First, age significantly correlated with verbal and nonverbal intelligence (strong positive correlations). In contrast, the correlations between age and mentalistic reasoning and age and irony understanding did not reach significance. This result seems to corroborate the idea that these two abilities, which inherently require the attribution of mental states and intentions to other persons, are impaired in this population and do not seem to be compensated simply by age. At the same time, it is important to note that these data contrast with other studies that did find relationships between CA on the one hand and mentalistic abilities and sarcasm recognition on the other hand (e.g., Happé, Reference Happé1995; Peterson et al., Reference Peterson, Wellman and Slaughter2012); thus, they must be interpreted cautiously because they might be specific to our sample.

It is notable, however, that the ADOS score does not seem to capture autistic children’s difficulties in these specific skills, since we only found an inverse correlation with raw scores in the Raven task. Focusing on irony comprehension, when we consider autistic children as a group, the situation is not clear. Ironic compliment comprehension significantly correlated with both levels of ToM scores, whereas ironic criticism comprehension correlated with grammatical comprehension and with first-order ToM scores (moderate positive correlations).

The picture can be sharpened by zooming into individual data. Autistic children reached an overall accuracy of approximately 30% in ironic stories (27% for ironic compliments and 34% for ironic criticisms), but this overall score is in fact obtained by a clear bimodal distribution of the participants’ performance. The great majority of autistic children (18 out of 26) failed almost all questions assessing irony understanding (at least 10 errors out of 12, and 12 of these children failed all 12 questions); the other 6 children, on the other hand, responded correctly to all 12 questions investigating ironic criticisms and ironic compliments. Such a sharp partitioning of the population in “at-floor” and “at-ceiling” performers is not present in the groups of TD children, and interestingly, it was not attested either in the other atypical populations tested with the same irony comprehension task (Panzeri et al. Reference Panzeri, Giustolisi and Zampini2020; Reference Panzeri, Cavicchiolo, Giustolisi, Di Berardino, Ajmone, Vizziello, Donnini and Zanetti2021). To search for the factors that might promote irony recognition in autistic children, we believe that, instead of considering the autistic children as a group, it might be more relevant to delineate the profiles of the children who fail from those who succeed in irony understanding. Even if we are fully aware that the difference in size of the groups (18 vs. 6 children) and, particularly, that the very low number of children performing at ceiling do not permit us to draw firm conclusions, the data reported in Tables 11 and 12 are worth discussing. Despite the fact that children who demonstrate a perfect understanding of the ironic stories were slightly older and had higher scores in all the tested cognitive and linguistic abilities (compared to those who fail almost all questions related to irony), only in the case of ToM did this difference reach statistical significance. The at-ceiling performers had significantly higher scores than the at-floor performers in both first-order ToM (.52 vs.11) and second-order ToM (1.33 vs.28). This result, at first glance, seems to support the idea that higher-order mentalistic skills are required to master irony, and in particular to identify the speaker’s communicative intent (distinguishing lies from jokes). On the other hand, a closer inspection of the data casts some doubts on the necessary links between ToM skills and irony understanding: within the group of at-ceiling performers, the variation in ToM scores was high, and two (out of six) of the children who responded correctly to all questions investigating irony understanding failed both ToM tasks. This result, then, questions the idea that passing mindreading tasks is a necessary condition for performing well in irony comprehension—even if we must acknowledge the fact that ToM abilities were assessed with only two tasks.

Table 11. Summary statistics (means and standard deviations) of various biographical, linguistics, and cognitive measures in ASD children

* The age range is 45–177 months for the “at-floor group” and 60–123 months for the “at-ceiling group.”

Table 12. ASD at-floor versus ASD at-ceiling group: unpaired two-samples Wilcoxon tests results. Significant differences between the two groups are marked in bold

The most striking result of our study, however, is the sharp bimodal distribution in at-ceiling and at-floor performers. This is different from the protracted process that leads TD children to fully master irony. After an initial phase in which TD children adhere to a literal interpretation of the ironic remark, they start realizing that the remark is incongruent with the situation; in this phase, however, children might struggle in identifying the speaker’s communicative intent and/or attitude. Moreover, TD children recognize ironic criticisms earlier and better than ironic compliments. Looking at individual performances, then, a shallow curve is typically found, as in Figure 4: some children fail most questions, others start demonstrating some understanding of ironic criticisms, while still misinterpreting ironic compliments, then other children begin to finally grasp all criticisms and some compliments, then eventually begin to master all types of ironic remarks. Our group of autistic children, however, was in fact split into two: the majority (18 children) failed all (N = 12) or almost all of the questions; six children responded correctly to all of them. For the first group, the at-floor performers, this result appears to be perfectly in line with the characterization of the profile of the condition of autism. As extensively discussed in the introduction, individuals with autism experience difficulties in inferring other persons’ intentions, and to understand irony and figurative language in general, interlocutors must go beyond the literal meaning of the sentence to recognize the speaker’s communicative intent. The presence of autistic children who respond correctly to all irony questions might be remarkable, but other studies found good performances in these tasks (Colich et al., Reference Colich, Wang, Rudie, Hernandez, Bookheimer and Dapretto2012; Pexman et al., Reference Pexman, Rostad, McMorris, Climie, Stowkowy and Glenwright2011; Saban-Bezalel et al., Reference Saban-Bezalel, Dolfin, Laor and Mashal2019; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Lee, Sigman and Dapretto2006; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Cherkassky, Mason, Keller, Minshew and Just2013). What is truly unexpected, although, is the absence of “intermediate” performers: only two autistic children (out of 26) had a 50% accuracy, the others were either at 100% or below 17%. This suggests that autistic participants either misunderstand ironic remarks, sticking to a literal interpretation and being consistent with this wrong interpretation, or they behave as if they found the key to solve the enigma of irony. Several scholars noticed that some autistic individuals appear to have “found the key” to respond correctly to tasks requiring mindreading, using strategies that appear to be different from those employed by TD peers. Happé (Reference Happé1995) found that autistic participants who had better scores in ToM tasks were those with higher verbal abilities and commented on this result, hypothesizing that they could solve mentalistic tasks “in a verbally mediated fashion,” “in an unusually conscious and logical way, for example, looking as if they are doing ‘mental arithmetic’ before eventually giving the correct answer” (Happé, Reference Happé1995: 852). Additionally, Frith et al. (Reference Frith, Morton and Leslie1991) proposed that the few autistic individuals who could pass ToM tasks behaved as if they had extracted general rules to account for specific situations that they would still deem totally irrational. Notice that this intellectually based strategy might require “routes that are slow and cumbersome, disrupting the timing of their responses” (Bowler, Reference Bowler1992: 888). There is evidence that autistic individuals who pass mentalistic tasks are slower in solving them (Bowler, Reference Bowler1997; Kaland et al., Reference Kaland, Smith and Mortensen2007), and neuroimaging techniques have found different activation patterns (Kana et al., Reference Kana, Maximo, Williams, Keller, Schipul, Cherkassky, Minshew and Just2015; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kyeong, Cheon, Park, Oh, Chun, Park, Kim and Song2016; Yuk et al., Reference Yuk, Urbain, Pang, Anagnostou, Buchsbaum and Taylor2018). Recently, Eigsti & Irvine (Reference Eigsti and Irvine2021) found that autistic adolescents were slower, compared to TD controls, in responding to false belief test questions when they were performing the task under verbal load. This finding is consistent with the proposal that there are two types of mindreading processes, one explicit and verbal and the other implicit and nonverbal, and autistic adolescents tend to rely on the former.

As discussed in the introductory section, in the case of sarcasm processing, the brain activity of autistic individuals was atypical compared to TD peers (Colich et al., Reference Colich, Wang, Rudie, Hernandez, Bookheimer and Dapretto2012; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Lee, Sigman and Dapretto2006; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Cherkassky, Mason, Keller, Minshew and Just2013). These observations lead us to hypothesize that the autistic children who performed at the ceiling in our sample were in fact using a “cognitive” strategy to detect ironic remarks, possibly based on simple rules, such as “if speakers said something blatantly false, then they are ironic and mean the opposite.” Notice that the perfect performance on ironic compliments might be easily explained assuming that these children were simply applying a rule, which permits to solve equally well ironic blames and ironic praises, without taking into account the different types of social norms they refer to (the “asymmetry of affect”), and Pexman et al. (Reference Pexman, Rostad, McMorris, Climie, Stowkowy and Glenwright2011) claimed that autistic individuals could not appreciate the social functions of irony.

Conclusion

The results of our study can be read in two ways. Focusing on global performance, we confirmed that autistic children lagged behind TD peers, not only those of the same CA but also the younger ones matched for linguistic competence. Moreover, positive correlations between ToM and irony detection were found. Nevertheless, zooming into the individual performances, we revealed an unexpected bimodal distribution. We hypothesized that the children who failed (almost) all questions related to ironic stories would show a “typical” profile, which met the pattern of responses expected from autistic children. On the other hand, those who showed an unexpected pattern of responses, answering correctly to all questions investigating not only ironic criticisms but also the rarer and usually harder ironic compliments, represented an “atypical” profile in autistic children. We speculated that these “atypical” children were in fact using an intellectually or verbally based compensatory strategy to solve this task. Our hypotheses are merely speculative, since we did not use any measure that might capture their processing strategies.

Crucially, as briefly discussed in the introduction, a bimodal distribution has also been found in studies investigating structural aspects of language (Kjelgaard & Tager Flusberg, Reference Kjelgaard and Tager-Flusberg2001, a.o.), and interestingly, the presence of “high-” and “low-performers” in language-related tasks seems to crosscut the presence or absence of intellectual disability (see also Silleresi et al., Reference Silleresi, Prevost, Zebib, Bonnet-Brilhault, Conte and Tuller2020). To the best of our knowledge, the presence of spared and impaired language abilities in a group of autistic individuals with comparable nonverbal mental competence has been attested for structural language aspects but has not been discussed for pragmatic tasks. This raises the question of whether the results of the present study are imputable to the well-known phenotypical variability and heterogeneity of linguistic and cognitive abilities in the autistic condition or whether future and more fine-grained analyses might delineate the profile of individuals who are able to comprehend ironic remarks and to attribute complex mental states to the others. Finally, it is still a matter of debate whether autistic individuals who exhibit good or even optimal performance in irony comprehension tasks are also able to understand and appreciate irony in real-life situations (Mognon et al., Reference Mognon, Scholten, Hukker and Hendriks2021). More generally, additional research is needed to test the replicability of the current findings and to address the following limitations. The number of participants in the current study was limited; a larger population sample, and in particular a larger sample including children with low nonverbal IQ, should make it possible to better address the question of the number and relative prevalence of patterns of responses on the comprehension of ironic criticisms and ironic compliments in autistic children. Regarding the identification of clusters of abilities in autism, we think that several pieces are still missing from the puzzle. In the irony comprehension task, the group of autistic children lagged behind both comparison groups of TD children and the younger groups matched for LA. This result might indicate that either autistic children are simply delayed, and they might possibly catch up as they grow older, or they are impaired, showing little or no spontaneous improvement with age. The fact that in our study we did not find correlations between CA on the one hand and irony comprehension (and ToM scores) on the other hand might be read as indirect evidence that irony recognition constitutes a real deficit (and not a simple delay) in autistic individuals. Nevertheless, as already noted, these data must be read cautiously because we do not know the extent to which they can be generalized. We believe that a longitudinal approach may help show the evolution of profiles across time and introduce further implications for the clinical aspects of this condition. We thus suggest that in future research, the concept of chronogeneity, that is, the heterogeneity of profiles of abilities in relation to the dimension of time, should be introduced (Georgiades et al., Reference Georgiades, Bishop and Frazier2017).

Our work has taken a step forward in identifying the comprehension of irony in autistic children, but other factors that we were unable to investigate in this study should be taken into consideration in future analyses, such as executive functions, which might play an important role in the development of irony comprehension and/or of mentalistic abilities. Moreover, the use of gaze detection techniques or neuroimaging could shed light on processing function in the two profiles we detected in our study. Finally, additional research is needed to investigate how the comprehension of irony may evolve from a longitudinal perspective. Concerning the comparison with other clinical groups, future research could include a direct comparison with children with Social Communication Disorder (DSM-5) and children with William Syndrome to determine whether the phenotypical realizations of irony comprehension in these conditions are different. Nevertheless, we believe that the results of the present investigation raise interesting questions that future research might fruitfully tackle.

Acknowledgements

This work has been supported by grants from the Fondazione ONLUS Marica De Vincenzi. We thank all the children and families who took part in our research. We are also grateful for the support from the clinicians of the Azienda Provinciale per i Servizi Sanitari Provincia Autonoma di Trento, and particularly to Dott. Stefano Calzolari and Dott.ssa Giulia Guglielmetti.

Authorship

FP conceived the experimental question, developed the tasks, and drafted the Discussion section. GM recruited the children, supervised the testing, and drafted the Method section. BG performed the statistical analyses and drafted the Results section. SS drafted the Introduction and the Conclusion section. LS supervised the work. All authors discussed the results. All authors revised and accepted the final version of the paper.

References

Andrés-Roqueta, C., & Katsos, N. (2020). A distinction between linguistic and social pragmatics helps the precise characterization of pragmatic challenges in children with autism spectrum disorders and developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(5), 14941508. doi such as those that require inferring speakers’ https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00263 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angeleri, R., & Airenti, G. (2014). The development of joke and irony understanding: A study with 3- to 6-year-old children. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 68(2), 133146. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
APA (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington DC: APA. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 Google Scholar
Asperger, H. (1944). Die “Autistischen Psychopathen” im Kindesalter. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 117(1), 76136. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01837709 Google Scholar
Asperger, H. (1991). “Autistic psychopathy” in childhood. In Frith, U. (Ed.), Autism and Asperger syndrome (pp. 3792). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1988). Social and pragmatic deficits in autism: Cognitive or affective? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18(3), 379402. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02212194 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belacchi, C., Scalisi, T. G., Cannoni, E., & Cornoldi, C. (2008). CPM Coloured Progressive Matrices. Standardizzazione italiana. Firenze: Giunti OS.Google Scholar
Bowler, D. M. (1992). “Theory of Mind” in Asperger’s Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(5), 877893. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb01962.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowler, D. M. (1997). Reaction times to mental state and non-mental state questions in false belief tasks by high-functioning individuals with autism. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 6(3), 160165. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00538988 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgers, C., & van Mulken, M. (2017). Humor markers. In Attardo, S. (Ed.) The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 385399). Taylor & Francis. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315731162-27 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97(3), 404. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.404 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chahboun, S., Vulchanov, V., Saldaña, D., Eshuis, H., & Vulchanova, M. (2016). Can you play with fire and not hurt yourself? A comparative study in fgurative language comprehension between individuals with and without autism spectrum disorder. PLoS ONE, 11(12), e0168571. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168571 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colich, N. L., Wang, A. T., Rudie, J. D., Hernandez, L. M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Dapretto, M. (2012). Atypical neural processing of ironic and sincere remarks in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Metaphor and Symbol, 27(1), 7092. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2012.638856 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creusere, M. A. (2000). A developmental test of theoretical perspectives on the understanding of verbal irony: Children’s recognition of allusion and pragmatic insincerity. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(1–2), 2945. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327868MS151&2_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deliens, G., Papastamou, F., Ruytenbeek, N., Geelhand, P., & Kissine, M. (2018). Selective pragmatic impairment in autism spectrum disorder: Indirect requests versus irony. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(9), 29382952. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3561-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewey, M. A., & Everard, M. P. (1974). The near-normal autistic adolescent. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 4(4), 348356. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02105378 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dews, S., Winner, E., Kaplan, J., Rosenblatt, E., Hunt, M., Lim, K., McGovern, A., Qualter, A., & Smarsh, B. (1996). Children’s understanding of the meaning and functions of verbal irony. Child Development, 67(6), 30713085. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131767 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domaneschi, F., & Bambini, V. (2020). Pragmatic competence. In Fridland, E. & Pavese, C. (Eds.), Routledge Handbook of Skill and Expertise (pp. 419430). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durrleman, S., Burnel, M., & Reboul, A. (2017). Theory of mind in SLI revisited: Links with syntax, comparisons with ASD. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 52(6), 816830. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12317 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durrleman, S., Delage, H., Prévost, P., & Tuller, L. (2017). The comprehension of passives in autism spectrum disorder. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 2(1), 88. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.205 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eigsti, I. M., & Irvine, C. A. (2021). Verbal mediation of theory of mind in verbal adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Language Acquisition, 28(2), 195213. https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2021.1877705 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79, 126138. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01115.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U., Morton, J., & Leslie, A. M. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: Autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 14(10), 433438. https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(91)90041-R CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgiades, S., Bishop, S. L., & Frazier, T. (2017). Editorial Perspective: Longitudinal research in autism–introducing the concept of ‘chronogeneity’. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(5), 634636. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12690 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geurts, B., Kissine, M., & van Tiel, B. (2020). Pragmatic reasoning in autism. In Morsanyi, K. & Byrne, R. M. (Eds.) Thinking, reasoning, and decision making in autism (pp. 113134). Routledge. http://doi.org/10.4324/9781351060912-6 Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. Jr. (1994). The poetics of mind. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Giora, R. (1995). On irony and negation. Discourse Processes, 19(2), 239264. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539509544916 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenwright, M., & Agbayewa, A. S. (2012). Older children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders can comprehend verbal irony in computer-mediated communication. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 628638. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.09.013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child Development, 2637. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130386 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hancock, J. T., Dunham, P. J., & Purdy, K. (2000). Children’s comprehension of critical and complimentary forms of verbal irony. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1(2), 227248. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD010204 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Happé, F. G. E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101119. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90026-R CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happé, F. G. E. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843855. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00909.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, M., & Pexman, P. M. (2003). Children’s perceptions of the social functions of verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 36, 147165. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326950DP3603_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, T. L., Prelock, P. A., & Bonazinga-Bouyea, L. (2014). Technical Manual for the Theory of Mind Inventory and Theory of Mind Task Battery. Unpublished copyrighted manuscript. Available at: https://www.theoryofmindinventory.com/ Google Scholar
Kaland, N., Møller-Nielsen, A., Callesen, K., Mortensen, E. L., Gottlieb, D., & Smith, L. (2002). A new advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(4), 517528. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaland, N., Smith, L., & Mortensen, E. L. (2007). Response times of children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome on an ‘advanced’ test of theory of mind. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(2), 197209. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0152-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalandadze, T., Bambini, V., & Næss, K. A. B. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of studies on metaphor comprehension in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: Do task properties matter?. Applied Psycholinguistics, 40(6), 14211454. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716419000328 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalandadze, T., Norbury, C., Nærland, T., & Næss, K. A. B. (2018). Figurative language comprehension in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analytic review. Autism, 22(2), 99117. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316668652 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kana, R. K., Maximo, J. O., Williams, D. L., Keller, T. A., Schipul, S. E., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N.J., & Just, M. A. (2015). Aberrant functioning of the theory-of-mind network in children and adolescents with autism. Molecular Autism, 6(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-015-0052-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanner, L. (1944). Early infantile autism. The Journal of Pediatrics, 25(3), 211217. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-3955(16)30693-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katsos, N., & Andrés-Roqueta, C. (2021). Where next for pragmatics and mind reading? A situation-based view (Response to Kissine). Language, 97(3), e184e197. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2021.0036 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, E., Kyeong, S., Cheon, K. A., Park, B., Oh, M. K., Chun, J. W., Park, H., Kim, J., & Song, D. H. (2016). Neural responses to affective and cognitive theory of mind in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Neuroscience Letters, 621, 117125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2016.04.026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kissine, M. (2012). Pragmatics, cognitive flexibility and autism spectrum disorders. Mind & Language, 27(1), 128. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2011.01433.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kissine, M. (2021). Autism, constructionism and nativism. Language, 97(3), e139e160. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2021.0055 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2–3), 287308. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960042000058 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreuz, R. J., & Link, K. E. (2002). Asymmetries in the use of verbal irony. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21(2), 127143. https://doi.org/10.1177/02627X02021002002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kumon-Nakamura, S., Glucksberg, S., & Brown, M. (1995). How about another piece of pie: The allusional pretense theory of discourse irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(1), 3. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.124.1.3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule, (ADOS-2) (2nd ed.). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
MacKay, G., & Shaw, A. (2004). A comparative study of figurative language in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 20, 1332. https://doi.org/10.1191/0265659004ct261oa CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marini, A., Marotta, L., Bulgheroni, S., & Fabbro, F. (2015). Batteria per la Valutazione del Linguaggio in Bambini dai 4 ai 12 anni (BVL_4-12). Firenze: Giunti OS.Google Scholar
Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non- literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 311328. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:jadd.0000029553.52889.15 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazzarella, D., & Noveck, I. (2021). Pragmatics and mind reading: The puzzle of autism (Response to Kissine). Language, 97(3), e198e210. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2021.0037 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meir, N., & Novogrodsky, R. (2020). Syntactic abilities and verbal memory in monolingual and bilingual children with High Functioning Autism (HFA). First Language, 40(4), 341366. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723719849981 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melogno, S., Pinto, M. A., & Levi, G. (2012). Metaphor and metonymy in ASD children: A critical review from a developmental perspective. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(4), 12891296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.04.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622646. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mognon, I., Scholten, I., Hukker, V., & Hendriks, P. (2021). Pragmatics is not a monolithic phenomenon, and neither is theory of mind: Response to Kissine. Language, 97(3), e218e227. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2021.0039 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morsanyi, K., & Stamenković, D. (2021). Idiom and proverb processing in autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-021-00079-4 Google Scholar
Nakassis, C., & Snedeker, J. (2002). Beyond sarcasm: Intonation and context as relational cues in children’s recognition of irony. In Proceedings of the twenty-sixth Boston University conference on language development. Cascadilla Press, Somerville, MA (pp. 429–440).Google Scholar
Nilsen, E. S., Glenwright, M., & Huyder, V. (2011). Children and adults understand that verbal irony interpretation depends on listener knowledge. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(3), 374409. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2010.544693 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norbury, C.F. (2004). Factors supporting idiom comprehension in children with communication disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47(5), 11791193. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/087)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norbury, C.F. (2005). The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: Evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 23(3), 383399. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X26732 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panzeri, F., Cavicchiolo, S., Giustolisi, B., Di Berardino, F., Ajmone, P. F., Vizziello, P., Donnini, V., & Zanetti, D. (2021). Irony comprehension in children with cochlear implants: The role of language competence, Theory of Mind and prosody recognition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. https://doi.org/10.1044/2021_JSLHR-20-00671 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panzeri, F., Giustolisi, B., & Zampini, L. (2020). The comprehension of ironic criticisms and ironic compliments in individuals with Down syndrome: Adding another piece to the puzzle. Journal of Pragmatics, 156, 223234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2019.08.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Slaughter, V. (2012). The mind behind the message: Advancing theory-of-mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or Asperger syndrome. Child Development, 83(2), 469485. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01728.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pexman, P. M., & Glenwright, M. (2007). How do typically- developing children grasp the meaning of verbal irony? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 178196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.06.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, E. A., Stowkowy, J., & Glenwright, M. R. (2011). Processing of ironic language in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 10971112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1131-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prévost, P., Tuller, L., Zebib, R., Barthez, M. A., Malvy, J., & Bonnet-Brilhault, F. (2018). Pragmatic versus structural difficulties in the production of pronominal clitics in French-speaking children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 3, 117. https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518799643 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rivière, E., Klein, M., & Champagne-Lavau, M. (2018). Using context and prosody in irony understanding: Variability amongst individuals. Journal of Pragmatics, 138, 165172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2018.10.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saban-Bezalel, R., Dolfin, D., Laor, N., & Mashal, N. (2019). Irony comprehension and mentalizing ability in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 58, 3038. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2018.11.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silleresi, S., Prevost, P., Zebib, R., Bonnet-Brilhault, F., Conte, D., & Tuller, L. (2020). Identifying language and cognitive profiles in autistic children via a cluster analysis exploration: Implications for the new ICD-11. Autism Research, 13(7), 11551167. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2268 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, K., Winner, E., & Hopfield, N. (1995). How children tell a lie from a joke: The role of second-order mental state attributions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13(2), 191204. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1995.tb00673.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1981). On the nature of linguistic functioning in early infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11(1), 4556. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01531340 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2000). Understanding the language and communicative impairments in autism. In International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 23, pp. 185205). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0074-7750(00)80011-7 Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., Lord, C., Volkmar, F., Paul, R., & Klin, A. (2005). Language and communication in autism. Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 1, 335364. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939345.ch12 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vulchanova, M., Saldaña, D., Chahboun, S., & Vulchanov, V. (2015). Figurative language processing in atypical populations: The ASD perspective. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 24. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain, 129(4), 932943. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO (2018). ICD-11. Retrieved from https://icd.who.int.Google Scholar
Whyte, E.M., Nelson, K.E., & Scherf, K.S. (2014). Idiom, syntax, and advanced theory of mind abilities in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(1), 120130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D. L., Cherkassky, V. L., Mason, R. A., Keller, T. A., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2013). Brain function differences in language processing in children and adults with autism. Autism Research, 6(4), 288302. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1291 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, D., & Sperber, D. (2012). Explaining irony. In Meaning and relevance (pp. 123145). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winner, E., & Leekam, S. (1991). Distinguishing irony from deception: Understanding the speaker’s second-order intention. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(2), 257270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, E. C., Diehl, J. J., Morris, D., Hyman, S. L., & Bennetto, L. (2005). The use of two language tests to identify pragmatic language problems in children with autism spectrum disorders. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36(1), 6272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuk, V., Urbain, C., Pang, E. W., Anagnostou, E., Buchsbaum, D., & Taylor, M. J. (2018). Do you know what I’m thinking? Temporal and spatial brain activity during a theory-of-mind task in children with autism. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 34, 139147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.08.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of the autistic participants: gender, age (in months), and scores at the ADOS, Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (Raven), and grammatical comprehension task (BVL). Participants are listed ordered by age

Figure 1

Table 2. Characteristics of the three groups of participants: mean age (in months) and mean raw scores and standard deviations at the Raven, the grammatical comprehension task (BVL) for the group of children with ASD and for the two TD groups matched for chronological age (CA) and for linguistic age (LA)

Figure 2

Table 3. Example of a story in the ironic criticism condition (English translation)

Figure 3

Table 4. Example of a story in the ironic compliments condition (English translation)

Figure 4

Table 5. ToM abilities: aggregate results (mean and standard deviations) for first-order ToM (ToM 1, maximum score 1) and second-order ToM (ToM 2, maximum score 3)

Figure 5

Figure 1. Boxplots representing the distribution of participants mean accuracy (y-axis) in the three groups (x-axis) in ironic (white) and literal (gray) stories. The “X” symbols represent mean values.

Figure 6

Table 6. Ironic versus literal stories analysis: results summary

Figure 7

Figure 2. Boxplots representing the distribution of participants mean accuracy (y-axis) in the three groups (x-axis) in ironic compliments (white) and ironic criticisms (gray). The “X” symbols represent mean values.

Figure 8

Table 7. Irony comprehension task: model’s selection summary

Figure 9

Table 8. Ironic compliments versus ironic criticisms analysis: results summary

Figure 10

Figure 3. Irony scores – individual ASD results in compliments (white bar) and criticisms (gray bar).

Figure 11

Figure 4. Irony scores – individual TD results in compliments (white bar) and criticisms (gray bar). To a better visualization, we kept the two TD groups separated (top: CA; bottom: LA).

Figure 12

Table 9. TD group (N = 52), Spearman’s correlation coefficients; significant correlations are in bold (*** = p < .0001; ** = p < .001; * = p < .05)

Figure 13

Table 10. ASD group (N = 26), Spearman’s correlation coefficients; significant correlations are in bold (*** = p < .0001; ** = p < .001; * = p < .05)

Figure 14

Table 11. Summary statistics (means and standard deviations) of various biographical, linguistics, and cognitive measures in ASD children

Figure 15

Table 12. ASD at-floor versus ASD at-ceiling group: unpaired two-samples Wilcoxon tests results. Significant differences between the two groups are marked in bold