Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T17:00:57.289Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Aspects of linguistic awareness in reading comprehension

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Ellen Bialystok*
Affiliation:
York University
*
Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele Street, Downsview, Ontario, M3J 1P3, Canada

Abstract

The relation between reading comprehension and linguistic awareness is discussed in terms of a set of processing skills common to both these activities. Two metalinguistic skill components are identified and examined for their relative contribution to reading comprehension. The general hypothesis is that these skill components explain a significant portion of the variance in reading comprehension. The two skill components are operationalized in terms of a battery of metalinguistic tasks, each of which is claimed (by means of task analysis) to rely primarily on one or the other processing component for its solution. A study is reported in which 8-year-old children are tested with these metalinguistic tasks, general measures of intelligence, and a test of reading comprehension. The results show that the relation among performance on the metalinguistic tasks is strongest for those tasks relying on the same processing skill component and that one of these components is most significant in determining the child's level of reading comprehension. These findings are interpreted in terms of the underlying linguistic processes involved in reading.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1982). From conversation to composition: The role of instruction in a developmental process. In Glaser, R. (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1979). Explicit and implicit judgments of L2 grammaticality. Language Learning, 29, 81103.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1986a). Children's concept of word. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 1332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1986b). Factors in the growth of linguistic awareness. Child Development, 57, 498510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Mitterer, J. O. (in press). Metalinguistic differences among three kinds of readers. Journal of Educational Psychology.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Ryan, E. B. (1985). A metacognitive framework for the development of first and second language skills. In Forrest-Pressley, D. L., MacKinnon, G. E., & Waller, T. G. (Eds.), Meta-cognition, cognition, and human performance. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Blackman, B. A. (1984). Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 610622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1982). Starting to talk worse: Clues to language acquisition from children's late speech errors. In Strauss, S. (Ed.), U-Shaped behavior growth. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature, 27, 746747.Google Scholar
Brown, A. (1980). Metacognitive development and reading. In Spiro, R. J., Bruce, B. C., & Brewer, W. F. (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brown, A. L., Bransford, J. D., Ferrara, R. A., & Campione, J. C. (1983). Learning, remembering, and understanding. In Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M. (Eds.). Carmichael's manual of child psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1981). Cognitive processes in reading: Models based on readers' eye fixations. In Lesgold, A. M. & Perfetti, C. A. (Eds.), Interactive processes in reading (pp. 177213). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Chomsky, C. (1979). Consciousness is related to linguistic awareness. Paper presented at the International Reading Association Seminar on Linguistic Awareness and Learning to Read, Victoria, BC.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. (1978). Children's minds. Glasgow: Fontana.Google Scholar
Downing, J. A. (1979). Reading and reasoning. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1979). Linguistic insight: Threshold of reading acquisition. In Waller, T. G. & MacKinnon, G. E. (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1981). Cognitive monitoring. In Dickson, D. P. (Ed.), Children's oral communication skills. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Forrest-Pressley, D. L., & Waller, T. G. (1984). Cognition, meta-cognition, and reading. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, B., & Routh, D. K. (1980). Phonemic analysis and severe reading disability in children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 9, 115119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golinkoff, R. M. (1976). A comparison of reading comprehension processes in good and poor readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 11, 623659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. (1978). Critique: Phonemic awareness skills and reading achievement. In Murray, F. B., & Pikulski, J. J. (Eds.), The acquisition of reading: Cognitive, linguistic and perceptual prerequisites. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Hakes, D. T. (1980). The development of metalinguistic abilities in children. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986). From metaprocess to conscious access: Evidence from children's metalinguistic and repair data. Cognition, 28, 95147.Google Scholar
Mattingly, I. G. (1984). Reading, linguistic awareness, and language acquisition. In Downing, J., & Valtin, R., (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Mitterer, J. O. (1982). There are at least two kinds of poor readers: Whole-word poor readers and recoding poor readers. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 445461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reber, A. S. & Lewis, S. (1977). Implicit learning: An analysis of the form and structure of a body of tacit knowledge. Cognition, 5, 333361.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). The structure and acquisition of reading II: The reading process and the acquisition of the alphabetic principle. In Reber, A. S. & Scarborough, D. L. (Eds.), Toward a psychology of reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ryan, E. B., & Ledger, G. W. (1979). Grammaticality judgments, sentence repetitions, and sentence corrections of children learning to read. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 6, 2340.Google Scholar
Ryan, E. B., & Ledger, G. W. (1984). Learning to attend to sentence structure: Links between metalinguistic development and reading. In Downing, J. & Valtin, R. (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Saywitz, K., & Wilkinson, C. (1982). Age-related differences in metalinguistic awareness. In Kuczaj, S. A. (Ed.), Language development: Vol. 2. Language, thought and culture. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 166.Google Scholar
Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. L., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1982). Metalinguistic awareness and language development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 449468.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Powell, J. S. (1983). The development of intelligence. In Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Valtin, R. (1984). The development of metalinguistic abilities in children learning to read and write. In Downing, J. & Valtin, R. (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar