Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T09:17:46.708Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

American Sign Language syntactic and narrative comprehension in skilled and less skilled readers: Bilingual and bimodal evidence for the linguistic basis of reading

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2008

CHARLENE CHAMBERLAIN*
Affiliation:
McGill University
RACHEL I. MAYBERRY
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Charlene Chamberlain, 12028 62 Street, Edmonton, Alberta T5W 4C8, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

We tested the hypothesis that syntactic and narrative comprehension of a natural sign language can serve as the linguistic basis for skilled reading. Thirty-one adults who were deaf from birth and used American Sign Language (ASL) were classified as skilled or less skilled readers using an eighth-grade criterion. Proficiency with ASL syntax, and narrative comprehension of ASL and Manually Coded English (MCE) were measured in conjunction with variables including exposure to print, nonverbal IQ, and hearing and speech ability. Skilled readers showed high levels of ASL syntatic ability and narrative comprehension whereas less skilled readers did not. Regression analyses showed ASL syntactic ability to contribute unique variance in English reading performance when the effects of nonverbal IQ, exposure to print, and MCE comprehension were controlled. A reciprocal relationship between print exposure and sign language proficiency was further found. The results indicate that the linguistic basis of reading, and the reciprocal relationship between print exposure and “through the air” language, can be bimodal, as in being a sign language or a spoken language, and bilingual, as in being ASL and English.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Allen, T. E. (1986). Patterns of academic achievement among hearing impaired students: 1974 and 1983. In Schildroth, A. N. & Karchmer, M. A. (Eds.), Deaf children in America (pp. 161206). San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Allen, T. E. (1994). Who are the deaf and hard-of-hearing students leaving high school and entering post-secondary education? Washington, DC: Gallaudet University, Pelavin Research Institute.Google Scholar
Anthony, D. (1971). Seeing essential English. Anaheim, CA: Anaheim Union High School District, Educational Services Division.Google Scholar
Boudreault, P., & Mayberry, R. I. (2006). Grammatical processing in American Sign Language: Age of first-language acquisition effects in relation to syntactic structure. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 608635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bragg, B., & Bergman, E. (1981). Tales from a clubroom. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 11421157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamberlain, C., & Mayberry, R. I. (2000). Theorizing about the relationship between ASL and reading. In Chamberlain, C., Morford, J., & Mayberry, R. I. (Eds.), Language acquisition by eye (pp. 221260). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dickenson, D. K., & McCabe, A. (2001). Bringing it all together: The multiple origins, skills, and environmental supports of early literacy. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 16, 186202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickenson, D. K., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Poe, D. M. (2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 465481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, cognition, and the brain. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fischer, S. D. (1978). Sign language and creoles. In Siple, P. (Ed.), Understanding language through sign language research (pp. 309331). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gates, A. (2002). The effects of delayed first-language exposure on language acquisition: A case study. Unpublished Masters Thesis. McGill University.Google Scholar
Geers, A. E., & Moog, J. S. (1987). Predicting spoken language acquisition of profoundly hearing-impaired children. Volta Review, 89, 6986.Google Scholar
Gottardo, A., Siegel, L. S., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). The assessment of adults with reading disabilities: What can we learn from experimental tasks? Journal of Research in Reading, 20, 4254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanson, V. L., & Flowler, C. A. (1987). Phonological coding in word reading: Evidence from hearing and deaf readers. Memory and Cognition, 15, 199207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hargraves, L. M. (2002). The linguistic system of a deaf language learner: Examining the effects of delayed language exposure. Unpublished masters thesis, McGill University.Google Scholar
Hoffmeister, R. (2000). A piece of the puzzle: ASL and reading comprehension in deaf children. In Chamberlain, C., Morford, J. P., & Mayberry, R. I. (Eds.), Language acquisition by eye. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karchmer, M. A., & Mitchell, R. E. (2003). Demographic and achievement characteristics of deaf and hard-of-hearing students. In Marschark, M. & Spencer, P. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (pp. 2137). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
LaSasso, C., & Davey, B. (1987). The relationship between lexical knowledge and reading comprehension for prelingually, profoundly hearing-impaired students. Volta Review, 89, 211220.Google Scholar
Leach, J. M., Scarborough, H. S., & Rescorla, L. (2003). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 11421157.Google Scholar
Linebarger, M., Schwartz, M., & Saffran, E. (1983). Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic aphasics. Cognition, 13, 361392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacGinitie, W. H., & MacGinitie, R. K. (1992). Gates–MacGinite reading tests (2nd Canadian ed.). Scarborough, Canada: ITP Nelson.Google Scholar
MacSweeney, M., Woll, B., Campbell, R., McGuire, P., David, A. S., Williams, S. C. R., et al. (2002). Neural systems underlying British Sign Language and audio-visual English processing in native users. Brain, 125, 15831593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayberry, R. I. (2007). When timing is everything: Age of first-language acquisition effects on second-language learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 537549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayberry, R. I., & Lock, E. (2003). Age constraints on first versus second language acquisition: Evidence for linguistic plasticity and epigenesis. Brain and Language, 87, 369383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayberry, R. I., Lock, E., & Kazmi, H. (2002). Linguistic ability and early language exposure. Nature, 417, 38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayer, C., & Wells, G. (1996). Can the linguistic interdependence theory support a bilingual–bicultural model of literacy education for deaf students? Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 4, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moores, D. F., & Sweet, C. (1990). Relationships of English grammar and communicative fluency in reading in deaf adolescents. Exceptionality, 1, 97106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 342356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Padden, C., & Ramsey, C. (2000). American Sign Language and reading ability in deaf children. In Chamberlain, C., Morford, J. P., & Mayberry, R. I. (Eds.), Language acquisition by eye. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Sandak, R. (2000). Reading optimally builds on spoken language: Implications for deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 3250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perlmutter, D. (2008). Nobilior est vulgaris: Dante's hypothesis and sign language poetry. To appear in DeLuca, D., Lindgren, K., & Napoli, D. J. (Eds.), Signs and voices: Language, arts, and identify in the Deaf community. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Zatorre, R. J., Gauna, K., Nikelski, E. J., Dostie, D., & Evans, A. C., et al. (2000). Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing signed languages: Implications for the neural basis of human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 97, 1396113966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poizner, H., Klima, E. S., & Bellgui, U. (1987). What the hands reveal about the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Psychological Corporation. (1995). The Stanford Achievement Test (9th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace Educational Measurement.Google Scholar
Quigley, S. P., & Paul, P. V. (1989). English language development. In Wang, M. C., Reynolds, M. C., & Walberg, H. J. (Eds.), The handbook of special education: Research and practice (Vol. 3, pp. 321). Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Cooper, D. H. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of the connection between oral language and early reading. The Journal of Educational Research, 95, 259272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandler, W., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2006). Sign language and linguistic universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61, 17281743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schick, B., & Moeller, M. P. (1992). What is learnable in manually coded English sign systems? Applied Psycholinguistics, 3, 313340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E. (1993). Where does knowledge come from? Specific associations between print exposure and information acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 211229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 402433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, N. L. (1979). How children understand stories: A developmental analysis. In Katz, L. G. & Glockner, M. Z., Watkins, C., & Spencer, M. J. (Eds.), Current topics in early childhood education (Vol. II, pp. 261290). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Stokoe, W. (1970). Sign language diglossia. Studies in Linguistics, 21, 2741.Google Scholar
Strich, T. G., & James, J. H. (1984). Listening and reading. In Pearson, P. D. (Ed.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 293317). New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Strong, M., & Prinz, P. M. (1997). A study of the relationship between American Sign Language and English literacy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics Canada. (1996). Reading the future: A portrait of literacy in Canada. Ottawa: Author.Google Scholar
Traxler, C. B. (2000). The Stanford Achievement Test (9th ed.). National norming and performance standards for deaf and hard-of-hearing students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 337348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waters, G. S., & Doehring, D. G. (1990). Reading acquisition in congenitally deaf children who communicate orally: Insights from an analysis of component reading, language and memory skills. In Carr, T. & Levy, B. (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 323373). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Weber-Fox, C. M., & Neville, H. J. (1996). Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 231256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1981). Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Wolk, S., & Schildroth, A. N. (1986). Deaf children and speech intelligibility: A national survey. In Schildroth, A. N. & Karchmer, M. A. (Eds.), Deaf children in America (pp. 139159). San Diego, CA: College–Hill Press.Google Scholar