Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T17:25:40.317Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The use of pronominal case in English sentence interpretation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 August 2010

YUKI YOSHIMURA*
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Amherst
BRIAN MacWHINNEY
Affiliation:
Carnegie Mellon University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Yuki Yoshimura, Department of Languages, Literatures and Cultures, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, 161 Presidents Drive, Amherst, MA 01003. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study examined adult English native speakers' processing of sentences in which pronominal case marking conflicts with word order. Previous research has shown that English speakers rely heavily on word order for assigning case roles during sentence interpretation. However, in terms of cue reliability measures, we should expect English pronominal case to be nearly as strong a cue as word order. The current study examined this issue by asking subjects to interpret grammatical and ungrammatical sentences in which case competes with word order. The results indicated that word order remains the strongest cue in English, even when the case-marking cue is available. However, for noncanonical word orders, the case-marking cue had a strong effect on sentence interpretation.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bates, E., McDonald, J. L., MacWhinney, B. M., & Appelbaum, M. (1991). A maximum likelihood procedure for the analysis of group and individual data in aphasia research. Brain and Language, 40, 231265.Google Scholar
Childers, J. B., & Tomasello, M. (2001). The role of pronouns in young children's acquisition of the English transitive construction. Developmental Psychology, 37, 739748.Google Scholar
Devescovi, A., D'Amico, S., Smith, S., Mimica, I., & Bates, E. (1998). The development of sentence comprehension in Italian and Serbo–Croatian: Local versus distributed cues. In Hillert, D. (Ed.), Syntax and semantics: Vol. 31. Sentence processing: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 345377). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. L., & MacWhinney, B. (1999). Sentence processing strategies in children with expressive and expressive-receptive specific language impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 34, 117134.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. (1992). On the adequacy of the competition model. Language, 68, 812830.Google Scholar
Gordon, P., Grosz, B., & Gilliom, L. (1993). Pronouns, names, and the centering of attention in discourse. Cognitive Science, 17, 311317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Hopper, P. J., & Traugott, E. C. (1993). Grammaticalization. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kaan, E. (2001). Effects of NP type on the resolution of word-order ambiguities. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 529547.Google Scholar
Luce, R. D. (1959). Individual choice behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1982). Basic syntactic processes. In Kuczaj, S. (Ed.), Language acquisition: Vol. 1. Syntax and semantics (pp. 73136). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1987). The competition model. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 249308). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2005). Extending the competition model. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 6984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Bates, E. (Eds.). (1989). The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., Bates, E., & Kliegl, R. (1984). Cue validity and sentence interpretation in English, German, and Italian. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 127150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B., St.James, J. D., Schunn, C., Li, P., & Schneider, W. (2001). STEP—A System for Teaching Experimental Psychology using E-Prime. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 33, 287296.Google Scholar
Massaro, D. (1987). Speech perception by ear and eye. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (1986). The development of sentence comprehension strategies in English and Dutch. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 317335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (1987). Assigning linguistic roles: The influence of conflicting cues. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 100117.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L., & Heilenman, K. (1991). Determinants of cue strength in adult first and second language speakers of French. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 313348.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L., & MacWhinney, B. (1989). Maximum likelihood models for sentence processing research. In MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E. (Eds.), The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing (pp. 397421). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McDonald, J. L., & MacWhinney, B. (1991). Levels of learning: A microdevelopmental study of concept formation. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 407430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pléh, C. (1989). The development of sentence interpretation in Hungarian. In MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E. (Eds.), The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing (pp. 158184). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Prince, E. (1998). On the limits of syntax, with reference to topicalization and left-dislocation. In Culicover, P. & McNally, L. (Eds.), Syntax and semantics. The limits of syntax (Vol. 29, pp. 281302). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasaki, Y. (1991). English and Japanese interlanguage comprehension strategies: An analysis based on the competition model. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 4773.Google Scholar
Sasaki, Y. (1994). Paths of processing strategy transfers in learning Japanese and English as foreign languages. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 4372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasaki, Y. (1997). Individual variation in a Japanese sentence comprehension task: Form, functions, and strategies. Applied Linguistics, 18, 508537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasaki, Y., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). Language acquisition research based on the competition model. In Shirai, Y. (Ed.), Handbook of Japanese psycholinguistics (pp. 318328). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). The item-based nature of children's early syntactic development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 156163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Kemenade, A. (1987). Syntactic and morphological case in the history of English. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossRefGoogle Scholar