Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T18:53:30.199Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Spoken word recognition in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 December 2011

TOM LOUCAS*
Affiliation:
University of Reading
NICK RICHES
Affiliation:
University of Reading
GILLIAN BAIRD
Affiliation:
Guy's Hospital, London
ANDREW PICKLES
Affiliation:
De Crespigny Park, London
EMILY SIMONOFF
Affiliation:
De Crespigny Park, London
SUSIE CHANDLER
Affiliation:
Institute of Education, London
TONY CHARMAN
Affiliation:
Institute of Education, London
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Tom Loucas, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Earley Gate, Reading RG6 6AL, United Kingdom. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Spoken word recognition, during gating, appears intact in specific language impairment (SLI). This study used gating to investigate the process in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders plus language impairment (ALI). Adolescents with ALI, SLI, and typical language development (TLD), matched on nonverbal IQ listened to gated words that varied in frequency (low/high) and number of phonological onset neighbors (low/high density). Adolescents with ALI required more speech input to initially identify low-frequency words with low competitor density than those with SLI and those with TLD, who did not differ. These differences may be due to less well specified word form representations in ALI.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alcántara, J. I., Weisblatt, E. J. L., Moore, B. C. J., & Bolton, P. F. (2004). Speech-in-noise perception in high-functioning individuals with autism or Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 11071114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Audacity. (2005). Audacity 1.2.4 [Computer program]. Retrieved November 14, 2005, from http://audacity.sourceforge.net/Google Scholar
Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Meldrum, D., et al. (2006). Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames—The special needs and autism project (SNAP). Lancet, 368, 210215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2009). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.1.05) [Computer program]. Retrieved May 1, 2009, from http://www.praat.org/Google Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2011). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.3.02) [Computer program]. Retrieved March 12, 2010, from http://www.praat.org/Google Scholar
Burnage, G. (1990). CELEX: AGuide for Users. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: CELEX.Google Scholar
Ceponiene, R., Lepisto, T., Shestakova, A., Vanhala, R., Alku, P., Naatanen, R., et al. (2003). Speech–sound-selective auditory impairment in children with autism: they can perceive but do not attend. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 55675572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30, 213236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dockrell, J. E., Messer, D., George, R., & Ralli, A. (2003). Beyond naming patterns in children with WFDs—Definitions for nouns and verbs. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16, 191211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dockrell, J. E., Messer, D., George, R., & Wilson, G. (1998). Children with word-finding difficulties—Prevalence, presentation and naming problems. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 33, 445454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollaghan, C. (1998). Spoken word recognition in children with and without specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 193207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). British Picture Vocabulary Scale (2nd ed.). Berkshire, UK: NFER Nelson.Google Scholar
Edwards, J., & Lahey, M. (1996). Auditory lexical decisions of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39, 12631273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliot, L. L., Hammer, M. A., & Even, K. E. (1987). Perception of gated, highly familiar spoken monosyllabic nouns by children, teenagers, and older adults. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 150157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S. (2005). Speech perception in specific language impairment. In Pisoni, D. & Remez, R. (Eds.), Handbook of Speech Perception (pp. 567588). Malden, MA: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, J. (2004). DMDX Version 3.1. [Computer program]. Retrieved October 18, 2004, from http://www.u.arizona.edu/~kforster/dmdx/dmdx.htmGoogle Scholar
Gaskell, G. M., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1997). Integrating form and meaning: A distributed model of speech perception. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12, 613656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1980). Spoken word recognition processes and the gating paradigm. Perception & Psychophysics, 28, 267283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosjean, F. (1996). Gating. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11, 597604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudry, K., Leadbitter, K., Temple, K., Slonims, V., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., et al. (2010). Preschoolers with autism show greater impairment in receptive compared with expressive language abilities. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45, 681690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanner, L. (1946). Irrelevant and metaphorical language in early infantile autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 103, 242246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., & Dawson, G. (2005). Links between social and linguistic processing of speech in preschool children with autism: Behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Developmental Science, 8, F1F12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised—A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loucas, T., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chandler, S., Meldrum, D., et al. (2008). Autistic symptomatology and language ability in autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 11841192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loucas, T., Riches, N. G., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chandler, S., et al. (2010). Speech perception and phonological short-term memory capacity in language impairment: Preliminary evidence from adolescents with ASD and SLI. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45, 275286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19, 136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mainela-Arnold, E., Evans, J. L., & Coady, J. A. (2008). Lexical representations in children with SLI: Evidence from a frequency-manipulated gating task. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 381393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnuson, J. S., Dixon, J. A., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2007). The dynamics of lexical competition during spoken word recognition. Cognitive Science, 31, 133156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W. (1990). Activation, competition, and frequency in lexical access. In Altmann, G. T. M. (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: The first Sperlonga meeting. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition, 25, 71102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Welsh, A. (1978). Processing interactions during word-recognition in continuous speech. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 2963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGregor, K. K., Newman, R. M., Reilly, R. M., & Capone, N. C. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 9981014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metsala, J. L. (1997). An examination of word frequency and neighborhood density in the development of spoken-recognition. Memory & Cognition, 25, 4756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (1999). Recognition of gated words by children with specific language impairment: An examination of lexical mapping. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 735743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 2743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norbury, C. F. (2004). Factors supporting idiom comprehension in children with communication disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 11791193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Conti-Ramsden, G., Falcaro, M., Simkin, Z., Charman, T., et al. (2009). Loss of language in early development of autism and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 843852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plaisted, K. C. (2001). Reduced generalization: An alternative to weak central coherence. In Burack, J. A., Charman, A., Yirmiya, N., & Zelazo, P. R. (Eds.), Development and autism: Perspectives from theory and research. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rapin, I., & Dunn, M. (2003). Update on the language disorders of individuals on the autistic spectrum. Brain & Development, 25, 166172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L. (2004). Growth models of developmental language disorders. In Rice, M. L. & Waren, S. F. (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 207240). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, M. L., Oetting, J. B., Marquis, J., Bode, J., & Pae, S. (1994). Frequency of input effects on word comprehension of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 106122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2010). Sentence repetition in adolescents with specific language impairments and autism: An investigation of complex syntax. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45, 4760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, J. A., Rice, M. L., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Tense marking in children with autism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 429448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russo, N., Zecker, S., Trommer, B., Chen, J., & Kraus, N. (2009). Effects of background noise on cortical encoding of speech in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism nd Developmental Disorders, 39, 11851196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communication Question. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (2000). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (3rd ed. UK; CELF-3 UK). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Storkel, H. L., & Morrisette, M. L. (2002). The lexicon and phonology: Interactions in language acquisition. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 2437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006). Defining language phenotypes in autism. Clinical Neuroscience Research, 6, 219224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., and Lord, C. (2005). Language and communication in autism. In Volkmar, F., Paul, R., Klin, A., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Handbook of autism and PDD (pp. 335364). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Trauner, D., Wulfeck, B., Tallal, P., & Hesselink, J. (2000). Neurological and MRI profiles of children with developmental language impairment. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 42, 470475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyler, L. K., & Wessels, J. (1983). Quantifying contextual contributions to word-recognition processes. Perception & Psychophysics, 38, 409420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volden, J., & Lord, C. (1991). Neologisms and idiosyncratic language in autistic speakers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 109130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walley, A. C. (1993). The role of vocabulary development in children's spoken word recognition and segmentation ability. Developmental Review, 13, 286350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walley, A. C., Michela, V. L., & Wood, D. R. (1995). The gating paradigm: Effects of presentation format on spoken word recognition by children and adults. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 343351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed., rev.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
World Health Organization. (1993). Mental disorders: A glossary and guide to their classification in accordance with the 10th Revision of the International Classification of Diseases—Research Diagnostic Criteria (ICD-10). Geneva: Author.Google Scholar