Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T01:50:05.447Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relations of digit naming speed with three components of reading

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Carl Spring*
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis
John M. Davis
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis
*
Carl Spring, Department of Education, University of California, Davis, CA 95616

Abstract

The goal of the present investigation was to identify the reading processes that are impaired in children whose digit naming speeds are slow. Continuous digit naming speed was assumed to measure the automaticity with which character codes may be accessed in memory, and the automaticity of this process was assumed to be a prerequisite for the accurate performance of higher level reading processes. In Study I it was found, for children in grades 1 through 3, that digit naming speed was reliably correlated with reading of both irregularly spelled words and pronounceable nonsense words, and that most of the variance shared by irregular-word and nonsense-word reading was accounted for by digit naming speed. These results were interpreted as evidence that character-identification automaticity is equally important to the direct-access and speech-recoding routes of word recognition. Models of how character-identification automaticity might affect each of these routes are discussed. In Study 2 it was found, for children in grades 4 through 10, that the correlation of digit naming speed with reading comprehension was significantly smaller than its correlation with word recognition, and that the former correlation was completely accounted for when either WISC-R verbal IQ or word-recognition accuracy was controlled. These results are contrasted with comparable results reported by other investigators, and methodological differences are noted.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1983). Reading comprehension and the assessment of acquisition of word knowledge. In Hutson, B. (Ed.), Advances in reading/language research (pp. 231256). Greenwich, CT: JAI.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1979). Working memory and reading. In Kolers, P. A., Wrolstad, M., & Bouma, H. (Eds.), Processing of visible language: Vol. 1 (pp. 355370). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J. (1979). Orthographic and word specific mechanisms in children's reading of words. Child Development, 48, 587594.Google Scholar
Baron, J., & Treiman, R. (1980). Use of orthography in reading and learning to read. In Kavanaugh, J. F. & Venezky, R. L. (Eds.), Orthography, reading, and dyslexia (pp. 171189). Baltimore: Park.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In Underwood, G. (Ed.), Strategies of information processing (pp. 151216). London: Academic.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid “automized” naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doctor, E. A., & Coltheart, M. (1980). Children's use of phonological encoding when reading for meaning. Memory and Cognition, 8, 195209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eakin, S., & Douglas, V. I. (1971). Automatization and oral reading problems in children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 4, 2633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1980). The development of orthographic images. In Frith, U. (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp. 311338). London: Academic.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1979). The mnemonic value of orthography among beginning readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 2640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Chambers, S. M. (1973). Lexical access and naming time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 627635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederiksen, J. R., & Kroll, J. F. (1976). Spelling and sound: Approaches to the internal lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 361379.Google Scholar
Gilbert, N., Spring, C., & Sassenrath, J. (1977). Effect of overlearning and similarity on transfer in word recognition. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 591598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, M. D., & McClelland, J. L. (1979). Processing determinants of reading speed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 151181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, A. F. (1979). The cognitive and neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: A theoretical framework and review. Cognition, 7, 1933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, A. F. (1981). Children with reading and spelling retardation: Functioning of whole-word and correspondence-rule mechanisms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 22, 171178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, A. F., & Share, D. L. (1983). Phonological receding and reading acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 4, 103147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, R. B., & Shankweiler, D. (1985). Repetitive naming and the detection of word retrieval deficits in the beginning reader. Cortex, 21, 617625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerlinger, F. N., & Pedhazur, E. J. (1973). Multiple regression in behavioral research. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence: Brown University Press.Google Scholar
Laberge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambert, N. M., & Sandoval, J. (1980). The prevalence of learning disabilities in a sample of children considered hyperactive. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 8, 3350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenneberg, E. N. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, A. M., Mattingly, I. G., & Turvey, M. T. (1972). Language codes and memory codes. In Melton, A. W. & Martin, E. (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 307334). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lovett, M. W. (1987). A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed criteria of normal and deficient reading skill. Child Development, 58, 234260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otto, H., & Pizillo, C. (1970). Effect of intralist similarity on kindergarten pupils' rate of word acquisition and transfer. Journal of Reading Behavior, 3, 1419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Finger, E., & Hogaboam, T. (1978). Sources of vocalization latency differences between skilled and less skilled young readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 730739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Lesgold, A. M. (1979). Coding and comprehension in skilled reading and implications for reading instruction. In Resnick, L. B. & Weaver, P. A. (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading: Vol. 1 (pp. 5784). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1984). The skill of typing. Scientific American, 250(2), 128135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seymour, P. H. K., & Porpodas, C. D. (1980). Lexical and non-lexical processing of spelling in dyslexia. In Frith, U. (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp. 443473). London: Academic.Google Scholar
Shaffer, L. H. (1973). Latency mechanisms in transcription. In Kornblum, S. (Ed.), Attention and performance: Vol. 4 (pp. 435446). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. (1980). The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 294305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spring, C. (1976). Encoding speed and memory span in dyslexic children. The Journal of Special Education, 10, 3540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spring, C. (1987). [Digit naming speed as a predictor of reading disability]. Unpublished raw data.Google Scholar
Spring, C., & Capps, C. (1974). Encoding speed, rehearsal, and probed recall of dyslexic boys. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 780786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spring, C., & Farmer, R. (1975). Perceptual span of poor readers. Journal of Reading Behavior, 7, 297305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spring, C., & Perry, L. (1983). Naming speed and serial recall in poor and adequate readers. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 141145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1981). Relationships between word decoding speed, general name-retrieval ability, and reading progress in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 809815.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., Feeman, D. J., & Cunningham, A. E. (1983). The development of the relation between letter-naming speed and reading ability. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21, 199202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vellutino, F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Venezky, R. L. (1970). The structure of English orthography. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M. (1986). Rapid alternating stimulus naming in the developmental dyslexias. Brain and Language, 27, 360379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, M., Bally, H., & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development, 57, 9881000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar