Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T03:20:04.579Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Orthographic influences, vocabulary development, and phonological awareness in deaf children who use cochlear implants

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2009

DEBORAH JAMES*
Affiliation:
University of Newcastle
KAUKAB RAJPUT
Affiliation:
Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children NHS Trust, UK
JULIE BRINTON
Affiliation:
Institute of Sound and Vibration Research, UK
USHA GOSWAMI
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Deborah James, School of Education, Communication and Language Sciences, University of Newcastle, King George VI Building, Newcastle upon Tyne NE1 7RU, UK. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

In the current study, we explore the influence of orthographic knowledge on phonological awareness in children with cochlear implants and compare developmental associations to those found for hearing children matched for word reading level or chronological age. We show an influence of orthographic knowledge on syllable and phoneme awareness in deaf and hearing children, but no orthographic effect on rhyme awareness. Nonorthographic rhyme awareness was a significant predictor of reading outcomes for all groups. However, whereas receptive vocabulary knowledge was the most important predictor of word reading variance in the cochlear implant group, rhyme awareness was the only important predictor of word reading variance in the reading level matched hearing group. Both vocabulary and rhyme awareness were equally important in predicting reading in the chronological age-matched hearing group. The data suggest that both deaf and hearing children are influenced by orthography when making phonological judgments, and that phonological awareness and vocabulary are both important for reading development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Allen, T. E. (1986). Patterns of academic achievement among hearing impaired students: 1974 and 1983. In Schildroth, A. & Karchmer, M. (Eds.), Deaf children in America. San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Archbold, S., O'Donoghue, G., & Nikolopoulos, T. (1998). Cochlear implants in children: An analysis of use over a three-year period. American Journal of Otology, 19, 328331.Google Scholar
Boothroyd, A., & Boothroyd-Turner, D. (2002). Postimplantation audition and educational attainment in children with prelingually acquired profound deafness. Annals of Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngology, 111, 7984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, R., & Wright, H. (1988). Deafness, spelling and rhyme: How spelling supports written word and picture rhyming skills in deaf subjects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 771788.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. M., Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., & Stevenson, J. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in pre-school children. Developmental Psychology, 39, 913923.Google Scholar
Castles, A., Homes, V. M., Neath, J., & Kinoshita, S. (2003). How does orthographic knowledge influence performance on phonological awareness tasks? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 445467.Google Scholar
Connor, C. M., & Zwolan, T. A. (2004). Examining multiple sources of influence on the reading comprehension skills of children who use cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 509526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conrad, R. (1979). The deaf school child. London: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Crosson, J., & Geers, A. (2001). Analysis of narrative ability in children with cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing, 22, 381394.Google Scholar
De Cara, B., & Goswami, U. (2002). Similarity relations among spoken words: The special status of rimes in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 34, 416423.Google Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Poe, M. D. (2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 465481.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1982). The British Picture Vocabulary Scales. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In Grough, P. B., Ehri, L. C., & Treiman, R. (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Elliott, C. D. (1996). British Ability Scales II. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Fowler, A. E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In Brady, S. & Shankweiler, D. (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy. A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 97117). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. (2006). Changes in letter sound knowledge are associated with development of phonological awareness in pre-school children. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 143161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In Patterson, K., Coltheart, M., & Marshall, J. (Eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301330). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Geers, A., Brenner, C., Nicholas, J., Uchanski, R., Tye-Murray, N., & Tobey, E. (2002). Rehabilitation factors contributing to implant benefit in children. Annals of Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngology, 111, 127130.Google Scholar
Geers, A. E. (2002). Factors affecting the development of speech, language, and literacy in children with early cochlear implantation. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 172183.Google Scholar
Geers, A. E. (2003). Predictors of reading skill development in children with early cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 24, 59S68S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., & Richardson, U. (2005). The effects of spelling consistency on phonological awareness: A comparison of English and German. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 345365.Google Scholar
Goswami, U. C., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Harris, M., & Beech, J. R. (1998). Implicit phonological awareness and early reading development in prelingually deaf children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 3, 205216.Google Scholar
Hatcher, P., Hulme, C., & Ellis, A. W. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure by integrating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: The phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Development, 65, 4157.Google Scholar
Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., Miles, J. N. V., Carroll, J. M., Hatcher, J., Gibbs, S., et al. (2006). Efficacy of small group reading intervention for beginning readers with reading-delay: A randomised controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 820827.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, D., Rajput, K., Brinton, J., & Goswami, U. C. (2008). Phonological awareness, vocabulary, and word reading in children with cochlear implants: Does age of implantation explain individual variability in performance outcomes and growth? Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13, 117137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, D., Rajput, K., Brown, T., Sirimanna, T., Brinton, J., & Goswami, U. (2005). Phonological awareness in deaf children who use cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 15111528.Google Scholar
Kyle, F., & Harris, M. (2006). Concurrent correlates and predictors of reading and spelling achievement in deaf and hearing children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 11, 273288.Google Scholar
LaSasso, C., & Davey, B. (1987). The relationship between lexical knowledge and reading comprehension for prelingually, profoundly hearing-impaired students. Volta Review, 89, 211220.Google Scholar
Leybaert, J. (1993). Reading in the deaf: The roles of phonological codes. In Marschark, M. & Clarke, D. (Eds.), Psychological perspectives in deafness (pp. 269309). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Marschark, M., & Harris, M. (1996). Success and failure in learning to read: The special (?) case of deaf children. In Cornoldi, C. & Oakhill, J. (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Metsala, J. L. (1997). An examination of word frequency and neighborhood density in the development of spoken word recognition. Memory and Cognition, 25, 4756.Google Scholar
Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children's phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metsala, J., & Walley, A. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors of phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In Metsala, J. L. & Ehri, L. (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning reading. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Meyer, T. A., Svirsky, M. A., Kirk, K. I., & Miyamoto, R. T. (1998). Improvements in speech perception by children with profound prelingual hearing loss: Effects of device, communication mode, and chronological age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 846858.Google Scholar
Miller, P. (1997). The effect of mode of communication on the development of phonemic awareness in prelingually deaf students. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 11511163.Google Scholar
Moores, D. F., & Sweet, C. (1990). Relationships of English grammar and communicative fluency to reading in deaf adolescents. Exceptionality, 1, 97106.Google Scholar
Pammer, K., & Kevan, A. (2007). The contribution of visual sensitivity, phonological processing, and nonverbal I.Q. to children's reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 3353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, L. J., Brittan, B. A., & Tomblin, J. B. (2003). Exploring the language and literacy outcomes of pediatric cochlear implant users. Ear and Hearing, 24, 236247.Google Scholar
Spencer, L. J., Gantz, B. J., & Knutson, J. F. (2004). Outcomes and achievements of students who grew up with access to cochlear implants. Larynoscope, 114, 15761581.Google Scholar
Sterne, A., & Goswami, U. (2000). Phonological awareness of syllables, rhymes and phonemes in deaf children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 609625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swan, D., & Goswami, U. (1997) Picture naming deficits in developmental dyslexia: The phonological representations hypothesis. Brain and Language, 56, 334353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomblin, J. B., Spencer, L., & Gantz, B. J. (2000). Language and reading acquisition in children with and without cochlear implants. Advances in Otorhinolaryngology, 57, 300304.Google Scholar
Transler, C., Leybaert, J., & Gombert, J. (1999). Do deaf children use phonological syllables as reading units? Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 4, 124143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vermeulen, A. M., van Bon, W., Schreuder, R., Knoors, H., & Snik, A. (2007). Reading comprehension of deaf children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12, 283302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziegler, J. C., Ferrand, L., & Montant, M. (2004). Visual phonology: The effects of orthographic consistency on different auditory word recognition tasks. Memory and Cognition, 32, 732741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. C. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 329.Google Scholar