Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T14:10:47.707Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Morphological awareness and word reading in English language learners: Evidence from Spanish- and Chinese-speaking children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2011

GLORIA RAMIREZ*
Affiliation:
Thompson Rivers University
XI CHEN
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
ESTHER GEVA
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
YANG LUO
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Gloria Ramirez, Faculty of Human, Social and Educational Development, Thompson Rivers University, Kamloops, BC, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study examined the effects of first language characteristics on the development of two aspects of English morphological awareness: derivational and compound awareness in English language learners (ELLs) with Chinese or Spanish as their first language. It also assessed the contribution of derivational and compound awareness to word reading in the two groups of ELLs as well as in monolingual English-speaking children. Participants included 89 Spanish-speaking ELLs, 77 Chinese-speaking ELLs, and 78 monolingual English-speaking children from Grade 4 and Grade 7. Results showed that Chinese-speaking ELLs performed similarly to monolingual English speakers on English compound awareness, and monolingual English speakers outperformed Spanish-speaking ELLs. Spanish-speaking ELLs and monolingual children, in contrast, both outperformed Chinese-speaking ELLs on derivational awareness. Another key finding was that in all three groups of children, morphological awareness made a unique contribution to word reading after controlling for nonverbal ability, maternal education, and other reading related variables. These results underscore the influence of first language structure on the development of second language morphological awareness, and the similar contribution of morphological awareness to word reading across monolinguals and ELLs.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Akamatsu, N. (1999). The effects of first language orthographic features on word recognition processing in English as a second language. Reading and Writing, 11, 381403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
August, D., Carlo, M., Dressler, C., & Snow, C. (2005). The critical role of vocabulary development for English language learners. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 20, 5057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
August, D., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Synthesis: Instruction and professional development. In August, D. & Shanahan, T. (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth (pp. 351364). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., McBride-Chang, C., & Luk, G. (2005). Bilingualism, language proficiency, and learning to read in two writing systems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 580590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruck, M., & Genesee, F. (1995). Phonological awareness in young second language learners. Journal of Child Language, 22, 307324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12, 169190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2003). Morphology matters in learning to read: A commentary. Reading Psychology, 24, 291322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F., & Fleming, J. (2003). Lexical processing of morphologically complex words in the elementary years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 239253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Li, W., Hao, M., Wu, X., & Shu, H. (2004). Phonological awareness of bilingual and monolingual Chinese children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 142151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiappe, P., & Siegel, L. S. (2006). A longitudinal study of reading development of Canadian children from diverse linguistic backgrounds. Elementary School Journal, 107, 135152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Wade-Woolley, L., & Kirby, J. (2007). Crossover: The role of morphological awareness in French immersion children's reading. Developmental Psychology, 43, 732746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographic depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 104115.Google ScholarPubMed
Geva, E., & Siegel, L. S. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing, 12, 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geva, E., Zadeh Yaghoub, Z., & Schuster, B. (2000). Part IV: Reading and foreign language learning: Understanding individual differences in word recognition skills of ESL children. Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 121154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, N., Lu, W., & Ju, D. (1994). Reading Chinese and reading English: Similarities, differences, and second-language reading. Neuropsychology and Cognition, 8, 73109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In Frost, R. & Katz, L. (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 6784). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koda, K. (1999). Development of L2 intraword orthographic sensitivity and decoding skills. Modern Language Journal, 83, 5164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koda, K. (2000). Cross-linguistic variations in L2 morphological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 297320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lesaux, N., & Geva, E. (2006). Synthesis: Development of literacy in language-minority students. In August, D. & Shanahan, T. (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth (pp. 5374). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lesaux, N. K., Rupp, A. A., & Siegel, L. S. (2007). Growth in reading skills of children from diverse linguistic backgrounds: Findings from a 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 821834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lipka, O., Siegel, L., & Vukovic, R. (2005). The literacy skills of English language learners in Canada. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 20, 3949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., & Kail, R. (2002). Cross-cultural similarities in the predictors of reading acquisition. Child Development, 73, 13921407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J. R., Liu, H. Y., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., & Zhou, A. (2005). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 140160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagy, W., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second-grade readers and at-risk fourth-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 134147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Center for Education Statistics. (2006). National Assessment of Educational Progress, 2006, reading assessments. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences.Google Scholar
Raven, J. (1958). Standard progressive matrices. London: H. K. Lewis & Co. Ltd.Google Scholar
Raven, J. (2000). Raven manual research supplement 3: American norms. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press Ltd.Google Scholar
Reed, D. (2008). A synthesis of morphology interventions and effects on reading outcomes for students in grades K–12. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 23, 3649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Hadadd, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English–Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 21, 481504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiff, R., & Calif, S. (2007). Role of phonological and morphological awareness in L2 oral word reading. Language Learning, 57, 271298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, M., Share, D., Leikin, M., & Kozminsky, E. (2008). On the benefits of bi-literacy: Just a head start in reading or specific orthographic insights? Reading and Writing, 21, 905927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, L. (2008). Morphological awareness skills of English language learners and children with dyslexia. Topics in Language Disorders, 28, 1527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K., Cunningham, A. E., & Feeman, D. J. (1984). Intelligence, cognitive skills, and early reading progress. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 278303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, I., & Taylor, M. M. (1995). Writing and literacy in Chinese, Korean, and Japanese. New York: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turvey, M. T., Feldman, L. B., & Lukatela, G. (1984). The Serbo-Croatian orthography constrains the reader to a phonologically analysis strategy. In Henderson, L. (Ed.), Orthographies and reading (pp. 8189). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R., Torgesen, J., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Wang, M., Cheng, C., & Chen, S. (2006). Contribution of morphological awareness to Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 542553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., & Koda, K. (2007). Commonalities and differences in word identification skills among learners of English as a second language. Language Learning, 57, 201222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., Koda, K., & Perfetti, C. (2003). Alphabetic and nonalphabetic L1 effects in English word identification: A comparison of Korean and Chinese English L2 learners. Cognition, 8, 129149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodcock, R. W. (1984). Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery. Allen, TX: DLM.Google Scholar