Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T13:24:17.489Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is second language lexical access prosodically constrained? Processing of word stress by French Canadian second language learners of English

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2008

ANNIE TREMBLAY*
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Annie Tremblay, Department of French, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, 2090 Foreign Languages Building, 707 S. Mathews Avenue, Urbana, IL 61801. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The objectives of this study are (a) to determine if native speakers of Canadian French at different English proficiencies can use primary stress for recognizing English words and (b) to specify how the second language (L2) learners' (surface-level) knowledge of L2 stress placement influences their use of primary stress in L2 word recognition. Two experiments were conducted: a cross-modal word-identification task investigating (a) and a vocabulary production task investigating (b). The results show that several L2 learners can use primary stress for recognizing English words, but only the L2 learners with targetlike knowledge of stress placement can do so. The results also indicate that knowing where primary stress falls in English words is not sufficient for L2 learners to be able to use stress for L2 lexical access. This suggests that the problem that L2 word stress poses for many native speakers of (Canadian) French is at the level of lexical processing.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical database [CD]. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2007). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 4.5.18) [Computer program]. Retrieved March 30, 2007, from http://www.praat.org/Google Scholar
Booij, G. E. (1995). The phonology of Dutch. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Brown, J. D. (1980). Relative merits of four methods for scoring cloze tests. Modern Language Journal, 64, 311317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burzio, L. (1994). Principles of English stress. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charette, M. (1991). Conditions on phonological government. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, H.-C., & Cutler, A. (1997). Auditory priming in spoken and printed word recognition. In Chen, H.-C. (Ed.), The cognitive processing of Chinese and related Asian languages (pp. 7781). Hong Kong: Chinese University Press.Google Scholar
Clopper, C. G. (2002). Frequency of stress patterns in English: A computational analysis. Indiana University Linguistics Club Working Papers Online, 2. Retrieved November 15, 2005, from http://www.indiana.edu/iulcwpGoogle Scholar
Colantoni, L., & Steele, J. (2007). Acquiring /ʁ/ in context. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29, 381406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connine, C. M., Blasko, D. G., & Wang, J. (1994). Vertical similarity in spoken word recognition: Multiple lexical activation, individual differences, and the role of sentence context. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 624636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connine, C. M., Titone, D., Deelman, T., & Blasko, D. (1997). Similarity mapping in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 463480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, N., Cutler, A., & Wales, R. (2002). Constraints of lexical stress on lexical access in English: Evidence from native and non-native listeners. Language and Speech, 45, 207228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutler, A. (1986). Forbear is a homophone: Lexical prosody does not constrain lexical access. Language and Speech, 29, 201220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., & Butterfield, S. (1992). Rhythmic cues to speech segmentation: Evidence from juncture misperception. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 218236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., & Clifton, C. E. (1984). The use of prosodic information in word recognition. In Bouma, H. & Bouwhuis, D. G. (Eds.), Attention and performance X (pp. 183196). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cutler, A., & Donselaar, W. van (2001). Voornaam is not a homophone: Lexical prosody and lexical access in Dutch. Language and Speech, 44, 171195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., & Norris, D. G. (1988). The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 113121.Google Scholar
Cutler, A., & Otake, T. (1999). Pitch accent in spoken-word recognition in Japanese. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105, 18771888.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donselaar, W. van, Koster, M., & Cutler, A. (2005). Exploring the role of lexical stress in lexical recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 251273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, E., Pallier, C., Sebastian, N., & Mehler, J. (1997). A destressing “deafness” in French? Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 406421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, E., Peperkamp, S., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). A robust method to study stress “deafness.” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 110, 16061618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dupoux, E., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Navarrete, E., & Peperkamp, S. (2008). Persistent stress “deafness”: The case of French learners of Spanish. Cognition, 106, 682706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fear, B. D., Cutler, A., & Butterfield, S. (1995). The strong/weak syllable distinction in English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 18931904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frenck-Mestre, C. (2002). An on-line look at sentence processing in the second language. In Herrida, R. & Altarriba, J. (Eds.), Bilingual sentence processing (pp. 217236). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fry, D. B. (1958). Experiments in the perception of stress. Language and Speech, 1, 126152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaskell, M. G., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2002). Representation and competition in the perception of spoken words. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 220266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goad, H., & Buckley, M. (2006). Prosodic structure in child French: Evidence for the foot. Catalan Journal of Linguistics, 5, 109142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldinger, S. D., Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1989). Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: Effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 501518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimson, A. (1980). An introduction to the pronunciation of English (3rd ed.). London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Halle, M., & Vergnaud, J.-R. (1987). An essay on stress. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hammond, M. (1999). The phonology of English. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, J. W. (1983). Syllable structure and stress in Spanish: A non-linear analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jared, D., & Kroll, J. F. (2001). Do bilinguals activate phonological representations in one or both of their languages when naming words? Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jun, S.-A., & Fougeron, C. (2000). A phonological model of French intonation. In Botinis, A. (Ed.), Intonation: Analysis, modelling, and technology (pp. 209242). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D. M., & Newsome, M. (1999). The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 159207CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehiste, I. (1976). Influence of fundamental frequency pattern on the perception of duration. Journal of Phonetics, 4, 113117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003). Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 173193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1990). Activation, competition, and frequency. In Altmann, G. T. M. (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic computational perspectives (pp. 148172). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Warren, P. (1994). Levels of perceptual representation and process in lexical access: Words, phonemes, and features. Psychological Review, 101, 653675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, J., & Prince, A. (1993). Generalized alignment. In Booij, G. E. & van Marle, J. (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1993 (pp. 79153). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McQueen, J. M., Norris, D. G., & Cutler, A. (1994). Competition in spoken word recognition: Spotting words in other words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 621638.Google Scholar
McQueen, J. M., Norris, D. G., & Cutler, A. (1999). Lexical influence in phonetic decision making: Evidence from subcategorical mismatches. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 13631389.Google Scholar
Morgan, J. L., & Saffran, J. R. (1995). Emerging integration of sequential and suprasegmental information in preverbal speech segmentation. Child Development, 66, 911936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, J., Jusczyk, P. W., Kemler Nelson, D. G., Charles-Luce, J., Woodward, A. L., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1996). Infants' sensitivity to word boundaries in fluent speech. Journal of Child Language, 23, 130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norris, D. G., McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (1995). Competition and segmentation in spoken-word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 12091228.Google ScholarPubMed
Paradis, C., & Deshaies, D. (1990). Rules of stress assignment in Québec French: Evidence from perceptual data. Language Variation and Change, 2, 135154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sekiguchi, T., & Nakajima, Y. (1999). The use of lexical prosody for lexical access of the Japanese language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 439454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Small, L. H., Simon, S. D., & Goldberg, J. S. (1988). Lexical stress and lexical access: Homographs versus nonhomographs. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 272280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Cutler, A. (2001). Segmental and suprasegmental mismatch in lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 412432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A. (2007). Bridging the gap between theoretical linguistics and psycholinguistics in L2 phonology: Acquisition and processing of word stress by French Canadian L2 learners of English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hawaii, Honolulu, HI.Google Scholar
Turk, A., Nakai, S., & Sugahara, M. (2006). Acoustic segment durations in prosodic research: A practical guide. In Sudhoff, S., Lenertová, D., Meyer, R., Pappert, S., Augurzky, P., Mleinek, I., Richter, N., & Schließer, J. (Eds.), Methods in empirical prosody research (pp. 128). Berlin: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Walker, D. C. (1984). The pronunciation of Canadian French. Ottawa: University of Ottawa Press.Google Scholar
Weber, A., & Cutler, A. (2004). Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwitserlood, P. (1989). The locus of the effects of sentential-semantic context in spoken-word processing. Cognition, 32, 2564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed