No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
The emergent lexicon: The child's development of a linguistic vocabulary. Michael D. Smith and John L. Locke (Eds.). New York: Academic, 1988. Pp. 358.
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 28 November 2008
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Book Reviews
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990
References
REFERENCES
Barrett, M., Harris, M., Jones, D., & Brookes, S. (1986, September). The first words of the infant: Their characteristics and their relationship to maternal speech. Paper presented to the British Psychological Society Developmental Section Annual Conference, Exeter.Google Scholar
Beckwith, R. (1988). Learnability and psychological constraints. Doctoral dissertation, Teachers College, Columbia University.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1974). Talking, understanding and thinking: Developmental relationship between receptive and expressive language. In Schiefelbusch, R. & Lloyd, L. (Eds.), Language perspectives – Acquisition, retardation, and intervention (pp. 285–312). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1987). Developments in expression: Affect and speech. In Stein, N., Leventhal, B., & Trabasso, T. (Eds.) (1988), Psychological and biological approaches to emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (in press). Representation and expression. In Krasnegor, N., Rumbaugh, D., Schiefelbusch, R., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (Eds.), Biobehavioral foundations of language development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., & Beckwith, R. (1988). Intentionality and language development. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Lifter, K., & Broughton, J. (1985). The convergence of early cognition and language in the second year of life: Problems in conceptualization and measurement. In Barrett, M. (Ed.), Single word speech (pp. 149–180). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Miller, P., & Hood, L. (1975). Variation and reduction as aspects of competence in child language. In Pick, A. (Ed.), Minnesota symposia in child psychology (pp. 3–53). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G. (1985). Mental spaces: Aspects of meaning construction in natural language. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C., & Farwell, C. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 51, 419–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Inhelder, B. (1975). If you want to get ahead get a theory. Cognition, 3, 195–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lifter, K., & Bloom, L. (1989). Object play and the emergence of language. Journal of Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 395–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masur, E. (1982). Mothers' responses to infants' object-related gestures: Influences on lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 9, 23–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B., (Eds.). (1979). Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Roth, P. (1987, April). Longitudinal study of maternal verbal interaction styles. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore, MD.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1977). Mothers' speech research: From input to interaction. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (Eds.), Talking to children: Linguistic input and acquisition (pp. 31–49). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (1985). Philosophy and the human sciences: Philosophical papers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–1463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uzgiris, I., & Hunt, J. (1975). Assessment in infancy. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar