Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T05:20:58.178Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of sign language rehearsal on deaf subjects' immediate and delayed recall of English word lists

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

John D. Bonvillian*
Affiliation:
University of Virginia
Cathleen Althaus Rea
Affiliation:
University of Virginia
Michael D. Orlansky
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University
L. Allen Slade
Affiliation:
University of Delaware
*
John D. Bonvillian, Department of Psychology, Gilmer Hall, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22903-2477

Abstract

The relationship between sign language rehearsal and written free recall was examined by having deaf college students overtly rehearse the sign language equivalents of printed English words. In studies of both immediate and delayed memory, word recall was found to increase as a function of total rehearsal frequency and frequency of appearance in rehearsal sets. The serial recall curves in both memory experiments evidenced a primacy effect, which was interpreted as resulting from increased rehearsal of the words in the initial positions over the course of the list. In contrast to findings from previous short- and long-term memory studies with normally hearing subjects, neither a recency nor a negative recency effect was found. High imagery words were rehearsed and recalled slightly more frequently in immediate memory, but there was no effect resulting from the different imagery values of the stimuli in delayed recall. These results are discussed in relation to current conceptualizations of memory and of linguistic processing by deaf individuals.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1987

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bellugi, U. (1972). Studies in sign language. In O'Rourke, T. J. (Ed.), Psycholinguistics and total communication: The state of the art (pp. 6884). Washington, D.C.: American Annals of the Deaf.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U., & Fischer, S. (1972). A comparison of sign language and spoken language. Cognition, 1, 173200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U., Klima, E. S., & Siple, P. (1974/1975). Remembering in signs. Cognition, 3, 93125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U., & Siple, P. (1974). Remembering with and without words. In Bressen, F. (Ed.), Current problems in psycholinguistics (pp. 215235). Paris: CNRS.Google Scholar
Belmont, J. M., & Karchmer, M. A. (1978). Deaf people's memory: There are problems testing special populations. In Gruneberg, M. M., Morris, P. E., & Sykes, R. N. (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 581588). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Belmont, J. M., Karchmer, M. A., & Pilkonis, P. A. (1976). Instructed rehearsal strategies' influence on deaf memory processing. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 19, 3647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, F. X. (1957). A study of the visual memory of deaf and hearing children. American Annals of the Deaf, 102, 254265.Google Scholar
Bonvillian, J. D. (1983). Effects of signability and imagery on word recall of deaf and hearing students. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 56, 775791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonvillian, J. D., Charrow, V. R., & Nelson, K. E. (1973). Psycholinguistic and educational implications of deafness. Human Development, 16, 321345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, H., & Kannapell, B. (1969). New signs for instructional purposes. ERIC, No. ED-042-293.Google Scholar
Conlin, D., & Paivio, A. (1975). The associative learning of the deaf: The effects of word imagery and signability. Memory and Cognition, 3, 335340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornoldi, C., & Sanavio, E. (1980). Imagery value and recall in deaf children. Italian Journal of Psychology, 7, 3339.Google Scholar
Craig, E. M. (1973). Role of mental imagery in free recall of deaf, blind, and normal subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97, 249253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F., & Lockhart, R. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowder, R. G. (1976). Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dawson, E. H. (1983). Sign mediation in a word recognition task. In Kyle, J. G. & Woll, B. (Eds.), Language in sign: An international perspective on sign language (pp. 199225). London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Hanson, V. L. (1982). Short-term recall by deaf signers of American Sign Language: Implications of encoding strategy for order recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 572583.Google ScholarPubMed
Howard, D. V. (1983). Cognitive psychology: Memory, language, and thought. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kirchner, D. M., & Klatzky, R. L. (1985). Verbal rehearsal and memory in language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 556565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Krakow, R. A., & Hanson, V. (1985). Deaf signers and serial recall in the visual modality: Memory for signs, fingerspelling, and print. Memory and Cognition, 13, 265272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kyle, J. G. (1980). Sign language and internal representation. In Ahlgren, I. & Bergman, B. (Eds.), Papers from the first international symposium on sign language research (pp. 207221). Leksand: Swedish National Association of the Deaf.Google Scholar
Liben, L., Nowell, R., & Posnansky, C. (1978). Semantic and formational clustering in deaf and hearing subjects' free recall of signs. Memory and Cognition, 6, 599606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, S. (1983). Levels of development in the language of deaf children: ASL grammatical processes, signed English structures, and semantic features. Sign Language Studies, 40, 193286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, J. L., & Locke, V. L. (1971). Deaf children's phonetic, visual, and dactylic coding in a grapheme recall task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 142146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, J. L., & Scott, K. K. (1979). Phonetically mediated recall in the phonetically disordered child. Journal of Communication Disorders, 12, 125131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayberry, R., & Tuchman, S. (1985). Memory for sentences in American Sign Language: The influence of age on first sign learning. In Stokoe, W. & Volterra, V. (Eds.), SLR '83: Proceedings of the third international symposium on sign language research (pp. 120125). Silver Spring, MD: Linstok.Google Scholar
McCall, R., & Appelbaum, M. (1973). Bias in the analysis of repeated measures designs: Some alternative approaches. Child Development, 44, 401415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moores, D. F. (1981). Educating the deaf: Psychology, principles, and practices (2nd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Moulton, R., & Beasley, D. (1975). Verbal coding strategies used by hearing-impaired individuals. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 18, 559570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Jonides, J. (1984). Maintenance rehearsal: A two-component analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 369385.Google Scholar
Odom, P. B., Blanton, R. L., & McIntyre, C. K. (1970). Coding medium and word recall by deaf and hearing subjects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 13, 5458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph, 76 (1, Pt. 2).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parasnis, I. (1980). The effect of imagery on memory for signs and words (Doctoral dissertation, University of Rochester, 1980). Dissertation Abstracts International, 41, 2367B.Google Scholar
Postman, L., & Phillips, L. (1965). Short-term temporal changes in free recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 132138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riekehof, L. (1963). Talk to the deaf. Springfield, MO: Gospel Publishing House.Google Scholar
Rundus, D. (1977). Maintenance rehearsal and single-level processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 665681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rundus, D. (1980). Maintenance rehearsal and long-term recency. Memory and Cognition, 8, 226230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rundus, D., & Atkinson, R. (1970). Rehearsal processes in free recall: A procedure for direct observation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 99105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shand, M. A. (1980). Short-term coding processes of congenitally deaf signers of ASL: Natural language considerations. Dissertation Abstracts International, 41, 1572A–1573A. (University Microfilms No. 80-23115)Google Scholar
Shand, M. A. (1982). Sign-based short-term coding of American Sign Language signs and printed words by congenitally deaf signers. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shand, M. A., & Klima, E. S. (1981). Nonauditory suffix effects in congenitally deaf signers of American Sign Language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 464474.Google ScholarPubMed
Siple, P. (Ed.). (1978). Understanding language through sign language research. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Siple, P., & Brewer, L. (1985). Individual differences in coding strategies for short-term retention of signs. In Stokoe, W. C. & Volterra, V. (Eds.), SLR '83: Proceedings of the third international symposium on sign language research (pp. 109119). Silver Spring, MD: Linstok.Google Scholar
Siple, P., Fischer, S., & Bellugi, U. (1977). Memory for non-semantic attributes of ASL signs and English words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 561574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stokoe, W. C., Casterline, C., & Croneberg, C. (1965). A dictionary of American Sign Language on linguistic principles. Washington, DC: Gallaudet College Press.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L., & Lorge, I. (1944). The teacher's word book of 30,000 words. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1983). Silent reading: Insights from second-generation deaf readers. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 3965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed