Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T16:00:31.045Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The contributions of child and caregiver to referential and expressive language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Beverly A. Goldfield*
Affiliation:
Harvard University
*
Beverly A. Goldfield, Department of Psychology, Harvard University, William James Hall, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, MA 02138

Abstract

Twelve infants were observed longitudinally to examine the relationship of children's object play and social behavior and maternal object and social-centered talk to referential and expressive language. Measures of child behavior and maternal language were coded from play sessions videotaped in the home at 12, 15, and 18 months of age; mothers recorded their babies' first 50 words. Most children acquired a balanced distribution of object labels and social-centered words and phrases; a few children evidenced a more extreme “style.” More object labels were acquired by children who more often used a toy to engage mother and who had mothers who more often talked about toys. More social-centered speech was associated with children's nontoy-mediated social attention and more maternal behavior-focused speech.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1987

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bloom, L., Lightbown, P., & Hood, L. (1975). Structure and variation in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 40(2), Serial No. 160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bretherton, I., McNew, S., Snyder, L., Bates, E. (1983). Individual differences at 20 months: Analytic and holistic strategies in language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 10, 293320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1978). Learning how to do things with words. In Bruner, J. & Carton, A. (Eds.), Human growth and development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Collis, G. M. (1977). Visual co-orientation and maternal speech. In Schaffer, H. R. (Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction. London: Academic.Google Scholar
Cross, T. G. (1978). Mothers' speech and its association with rate of linguistic development in young children. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (Eds.), The development of communication. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
DellaCorte, M., Benedict, H., & Klein, D. (1983). The relationship of pragmatic dimensions of mothers' speech to the referential-expressive distinction. Journal of Child Language, 10, 3543.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1974). A pragmatic description of early language development. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 343351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, C. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 51(2).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furrow, D., & Nelson, K. (1984). Environmental correlates of individual differences in language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 11, 523534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldfield, B. A. (1985/1986). Referential and expressive language: A study of two mother-child dyads. First Language, 6, 119131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldfield, B. A., & Snow, C. E. (1985). Individual differences in language acquisition. In Gleason, J. B. (Ed.), The development of language. Columbus: Merrill.Google Scholar
Horgan, D. (1981). Rate of language acquisition and noun emphasis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 10(6), 629640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E. M. (1978). Conversations between mothers and young children: Individual differences and their possible implications for study of language learning. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (Eds.), The development of communication: Social and pragmatic factors in language acquisition. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lieven, E. M. (1980). Language development in young children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F. (1982). Mothers' responses to infants' object-related gestures: Influences on lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 9, 2330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(1–2), Serial No. 149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1981). Individual differences in language development: Implications for development and language. Developmental Psychology, 17(2), 170187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. E., & Bonvillian, J. D. (1978). Early language development: Conceptual growth and related processes between 2 and 4½ years of age. In Nelson, K. E. (Ed.), Language development. New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. (1980). The estensive definition in vocabulary teaching. Journal of Child Language, 7, 565573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A., & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling. Journal of Child Language, 5, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, A. M. (1983). The units of language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenblatt, D. (1977). Developmental trends in infant play. In Tizard, B. & Harvey, D. (Eds.), Biology of play. London: William Heinemann Medical Books.Google Scholar
Snyder, L. S., Bates, E., & Bretherton, I. (1981). Content and context in early lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 8, 565582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starr, S. (1975). The relationship of single words to two-word sentences. Child Development, 46, 701708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, D. (1977). The first relationship: Infant and mother. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C., & Cooper, J. (1981). Individual differences in early lexical and phonological acquisition. Paper presented at the meeting of the Second International Congress for the Study of Child Language,Vancouver, B.C.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4, 197212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, C. G., & Robinson, W. P. (1982). The role of adult speech in language development. In Fraser, C. & Scherer, K. (Eds.), Advances in the social psychology of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wolf, D., & Gardner, H. (1979). Style and sequence in symbolic play. In Franklin, M. & Smith, N. (Eds.), Early symbolization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar