The current availability of 3 vaccines for COVID-19 in the United States is a remarkable scientific feat. Despite their proven efficacy and safety, these vaccines are no exception to the problem of vaccine hesitancy. Reference Polack, Thomas and Kitchin1–3
Healthcare worker (HCW) interactions with patients play a key role in vaccine confidence. Reference Pandolfi, Marino and Carloni4–Reference Brewer, Chapman, Rothman, Leask and Kempe7 As professionals in healthcare, trained to evaluate science with rational arguments, often our response to vaccine hesitancy is to repeat the evidence. Reference Hornsey and Fielding8 For example, an emotionally evocative news story about a nurse developing anaphylaxis after vaccination would typically be countered by describing the low rates of vaccine side effects and the greater risk of illness from infection. Clearly this rational approach has missed the mark for a substantial proportion of people. 3
In contrast, behavioral science theorizes that emotions and core beliefs play an essential role in health decisions, whereas interventions for vaccination hesitancy might rely on appeals to anticipated regret, fear of disease, or even social norms. Using behavioral science methods, we sought to better understand HCW beliefs about the COVID-19 vaccine in our network of hospitals and among our colleagues. We then sought to determine what messaging themes are most effective at increasing both HCW likelihood of vaccination against COVID-19 and vaccination recommendations to their patients.
Methods
The Duke Antimicrobial Stewardship Outreach Network (DASON) partnered with the Center for Advanced Hindsight (CAH) to assess HCW experience and beliefs around COVID-19 and the perceived efficacy of messaging themes aimed at increasing COVID-19 vaccination. An online survey was e-mailed through 8 hospital listservs in DASON as well as through the North Carolina Medical Society during the month of February 2021. We randomly assigned participants to see 1 of 3 thematic messages about COVID-19 vaccines or to a no-message control group:
-
(1) Process safety: Rapid development and testing of COVID-19 vaccines were made possible through a combined effort worldwide and vaccines are both safe and effective.
-
(2) Appeal to normalcy: What do you miss about a prepandemic life? How much would you pay to have it back? For life to return to normal, vaccination is the key.
-
(3) Risk assessment: Decisions involving uncertain outcomes create 2 ways to be wrong—What is the risk of vaccinating versus the risk of COVID-19?
Participants were then asked a series of questions regarding their perception of the passage (eg, helpful, believable) as well as their likelihood of sharing it with their patients. We used the χ2 test to compare willingness to share the passage by condition and linear regression to test passage ratings by condition. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Duke University (protocol no. 2021-0286). Full survey available online. Reference Rayburn-Reeves, Lindemans and Clark9
Results
Our final sample included 674 survey respondents, almost exclusively from North Carolina (98%) (Table 1). The sample comprised 84% women and 83.3% were of White race. The most common role represented was nursing (34%), and 72% of the respondents came into direct contact with COVID-19 patients. The largest portion of respondents worked in a hospital with bed capacity ranging from 100 to 199 beds.
Of the 98% of respondents who had been offered, 80% had already accepted the COVID-19 vaccine. Of the smaller cohort who had not received the vaccine (n = 127), the top 3 reasons for hesitancy dealt with safety concerns, such as “want to ensure the vaccine is safe,” “the vaccine was created too quickly,” and “wait to see the vaccine’s effectiveness.” Vaccine acceptance rates also differed by reported political affiliation, with Democrats showing the highest uptake (90%), followed by Independents (80%), and finally Republicans (75%). Democrats were also more likely to share the message they read to their patients (94%) compared with 87% of Republicans. Among those who had not been vaccinated, we did not detect a significant difference across messaging interventions regarding vaccine acceptance (all P values >.13).
Generally, HCWs were very comfortable recommending the COVID-19 vaccine to patients, as indicated by an average rating of 6.33 on a scale from 1 to 7 (1 = extremely unlikely to 7 = extremely likely). However, participants found the risk assessment message to be the most trustworthy, believable, and correct on a scale of 1–7 (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). When these 3 ratings are combined, the risk assessment message is rated significantly higher than either the appeal to normalcy message (P = .04) or the process safety message (P = .03) (Fig. 1). However, those assigned to the process safety message were more likely to say they would share their message with patients (75.8%) than those assigned to the normalcy condition (56.2%; P = .09), and the risk assessment condition fell between these 2 conditions (66.2%).
Discussion
Fortunately, HCWs generally had high COVID-19 vaccine acceptance rates and were likely to recommend the vaccine to patients. Regarding those who were hesitant, there was no difference across messages on increasing their acceptance, but the small size of the cohort makes it difficult to draw any conclusions.
Although HCWs generally gave all 3 passages high ratings, the risk assessment message had the highest ratings as trustworthy, believable, and correct; however, the process safety message ranked higher when asked whether they would share this passage with a patient. This finding suggests rhetoric that appeals on a personal level is not necessarily what HCWs would recommend to their patients. In other words, an HCW may prefer a message that feels credible to them over one that they feel is more likely to change patient behavior.
The qualitative data we gathered provides some insight into this difference. HCWs considered managing vaccine discussions in both ideological and practical terms. On an ideological level, patient autonomy was cited as an issue: HCWs “want to educate without adding opinions.” In a practical sense, HCWs were concerned with avoiding patient conflict, and also about their own ability to discuss this new technology: Vaccines are “not in my scope of practice”; I’m “not sure if I can inform clearly”; and “I’m not knowledgeable enough to recommend the vaccine to others.” These beliefs may explain the appeal of a process safety message because it was succinct and easy to memorize and was very similar to public health messaging at that time. In contrast, the risk assessment message requires one to understand certain statistical facts and discuss relative risk in order to share. When there is novel health technology we are tasked to endorse, bullet points are an efficient way to learn facts to discuss and create a familiar tone as they are reiterated.
This study had several limitations. These results may not be generalizable across space or time because the study captured opinions from February 2021 among HCWs concentrated in North Carolina. However, our findings suggest that HCWs are as likely to show similar concerns about vaccine safety as those captured in the general population. Additionally, our finding that HCWs may prefer to share a message they find credible over one they find persuasive suggests that the rational model of thinking may not even apply to us, let alone our patients. Physicians and other professionals should consider adopting methods from behavioral science to determine the true reasons why patients fail to comply with directions or take advantage of preventative services. Understanding human behavior is the first step to changing it.
Supplementary material
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/ash.2022.50
Acknowledgments
The authors thank the participants at the DASON hospitals for their efforts in improving COVID-19 vaccination rates in their patients.
Financial support
No financial support was provided relevant to this article.
Conflicts of interest
All authors report no conflicts of interest relevant to this article.