Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T16:15:20.614Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A systematic approach to defining and verifying descriptors used in the Qualitative Behavioural Assessment of sows

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 February 2024

Sarah Ibach
Affiliation:
Swine Teaching and Research Center, University of Pennsylvania School of Veterinary Medicine, Kennett Square, PA, USA
Jen-Yun Chou
Affiliation:
Pig Development Department, Animal & Grassland Research and Innovation Centre, Teagasc, Moorepark, Ireland Institute of Animal Welfare Science, University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna, Austria
Monica Battini*
Affiliation:
Department of Agricultural and Environmental Sciences – Production, Landscape, Agroenergy. University of Milan, Via G. Celoria 2, 20133, Milan, Italy
Thomas D Parsons
Affiliation:
Swine Teaching and Research Center, University of Pennsylvania School of Veterinary Medicine, Kennett Square, PA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Monica Battini; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Qualitative Behaviour Assessment (QBA) is a welfare evaluation tool that uses a holistic approach to capturing an animal’s emotional state. Lists of QBA descriptors validated to assess pig welfare exist, but their definitions are often not described in peer-reviewed literature and the processes used to develop definitions are lacking. The objective of this study is to detail a systematic approach to creating clear definitions for a pre-existing fixed list of QBA descriptors and test their application. A fixed list of 20 descriptors from the EU Welfare Quality® assessment protocol for pigs was modified, and ten pig experts were recruited to assist with defining these descriptors in a focus group-style discussion. Half of the experts involved in creating descriptor definitions partook in a subsequent step, where the newly developed definitions were tested by implementing QBA on a video library of post-weaned sows selected to capture the breadth of sow behaviour. Experts displayed excellent agreement in identifying a PCA dimension interpreted as the valence of descriptors and good agreement for another reflecting arousal. Inter-observer reliability was also measured for each descriptor. Only two descriptors exhibited less than moderate agreement between experts whereas half of the descriptors evoked substantial agreement or better. These findings support our process to delineate clear definitions for a fixed list of QBA descriptors in pigs. This study is the first of its kind detailing the in-depth process of creating and verifying descriptor definitions for future use in sow welfare assessment.

Type
Technical Contribution
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Qualitative Behaviour Assessment (QBA) is a versatile animal welfare assessment tool that has been used in several farm species such as pigs (Morgan et al. Reference Morgan, Pluske, Miller, Collins, Barnes, Wemelsfelder and Fleming2014; Carreras et al. Reference Carreras, Mainau, Arroyo, Moles, González, Bassols, Dalmau, Faucitano, Manteca and Velarde2016; Schmitt et al. Reference Schmitt, O’Driscoll, Baxter and Boyle2019), dairy cattle (de Boyer des Roches et al. Reference de Boyer des Roches, Lussert, Marion, Vincent, Pascal, Denys, Wemelsfelder and Foucras2018; Vindevoghel et al. Reference Vindevoghel, Fleming, Hyndman, Musk, Laurence and Collins2019), beef cattle (Stockman et al. Reference Stockman, McGilchrist, Collins, Barnes, Miller, Wickham, Greenwood, Cafe, Blache, Wemelsfelder and Fleming2012), dairy buffalo (Napolitano et al. Reference Napolitano, De Rosa, Grasso and Wemelsfelder2012), goats (Grosso et al. Reference Grosso, Battini, Wemelsfelder, Barbieri, Minero, Dalla Costa and Mattiello2016; Battini et al. Reference Battini, Barbieri, Vieira, Can, Stilwell and Mattiello2018) and sheep (Phythian et al. Reference Phythian, Michalopoulou, Duncan and Wemelsfelder2013; Willis et al. Reference Willis, Fleming, Dunston-Clarke, Barnes, Miller and Collins2021). QBA facilitates capturing how individuals interact with their environment by recording “how the animal is behaving” instead of “what the animal is doing” (Wemelsfelder et al. Reference Wemelsfelder, Haskell, Mendl, Calvert and Lawrence2000). It employs a holistic approach and enables an evaluation of animals’ presumed emotional state (Wemelsfelder et al. Reference Wemelsfelder, Hunter, Mendl and Lawrence2001). Today, good welfare is recognised as being more complex than the mere absence of negative experiences (Boissy et al. Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007) thus increasing the need to develop techniques used to identify presumed positive emotional states in animals. QBA’s value lies particularly in the technique’s ability to identify positive emotional states.

Welfare is assessed with QBA by scoring observed animals using various descriptors; descriptive terms such as ‘confident’ or ‘calm’ that detail an animal’s manner of interacting with its surroundings (Wemelsfelder et al. Reference Wemelsfelder, Hunter, Mendl and Lawrence2001). Two approaches can be employed in QBA studies: free choice profiling (FCP) and fixed list (FL) (Clarke et al. Reference Clarke, Pluske and Fleming2016). During FCP, assessors create their own list of QBA descriptors after observing an animal, while the FL approach provides assessors with a pre-determined list of descriptors. FLs of descriptors already exist for many farm animal species that have been developed, validated and published by experts (Welfare Quality® 2009; AWIN 2015). Currently, the most commonly used list of descriptors for pigs was published in the Welfare Quality® Assessment for Pigs (Welfare Quality® 2009). The Welfare Quality® project was a pan-European research project initiated in 2004. It aimed to develop standardised welfare assessment tools for farm animals that both supported animal management as well as offered benefits downstream in the value chain (Blokhuis et al. Reference Blokhuis, Veissier, Miele and Jones2010). Their principles of good welfare include good feeding, housing, health, and appropriate behaviour (e.g. Welfare Quality® 2009). In the Welfare Quality® protocols, there are a range of various measurements to assess each principle with QBA being the technique used to assess the appropriate behaviour of animals. However, within these protocols, there are only instructions on how to conduct QBA and a list of descriptors. The process from which the descriptors were generated and defined, and more importantly, the definitions themselves, were never published or made available to the public. A lack of definitions can create difficulties in the understanding, training, and actual practice of QBA using FL to assess welfare, as the descriptors may be misinterpreted or misunderstood. Some studies have touched on the process to generate FL of descriptors in different species, such as donkeys (Minero et al. Reference Minero, Dalla Costa, Dai, Murray, Canali and Wemelsfelder2016), goats (Grosso et al. Reference Grosso, Battini, Wemelsfelder, Barbieri, Minero, Dalla Costa and Mattiello2016), horses (Minero et al. Reference Minero, Dalla Costa, Dai, Canali, Barbieri, Zanella, Pascuzzo and Wemelsfelder2018) and shelter dogs (Stubsjøen et al. Reference Stubsjøen, Moe, Bruland, Lien and Muri2020). The processes used to develop descriptor definitions have never been detailed for a FL of QBA descriptors in pigs.

The intention of this paper is to describe a detailed, systematic procedure for generating reliable and meaningful definitions for a pre-existing FL of QBA descriptors in pigs. The procedure consisted of six steps, starting with the modification of a pre-existing FL, recruiting an expert panel, generating definitions for the descriptors, voting on agreement, and later testing and verifying the newly developed definitions using QBA with subsequent statistical analyses. This systematic approach aims to decrease the ambiguity in interpretation of descriptors, allow for efficient training of assessors, and increase the standardisation of QBA findings both within and between studies.

Materials and methods

Ethical statement

The expert panel was recruited and consented to allow the use of their intellectual contributions for research purposes in exchange for an agreed upon monetary compensation. All data presented hereafter are anonymised and no personal information is provided. Video recordings were taken during a preceding study conducted in accordance with University of Pennsylvania’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee protocol #804656.

The systematic approach

We developed a six-step process for creating clear and concise definitions of our FL QBA descriptors: Modify, Recruit, Define, Vote, Test, and Verify. These steps were created to breakdown defining descriptors into an easy-to-follow, transparent process.

Modify: Adaptation of fixed-list descriptors

A pre-existing fixed list of 20 descriptors was modified from the Welfare Quality® assessment protocol for pigs (Welfare Quality® 2009) (Table 1). This fixed list of descriptors was chosen to facilitate longitudinal comparison across subsequent studies implementing the same descriptors. However, the original Welfare Quality® assessment protocol descriptor ‘social’ was removed and replaced with ‘curious’ (Duijvesteijn et al. Reference Duijvesteijn, Benard, Reimert and Camerlink2014) as the current study focused on assessing pigs in an isolated context where social behaviour could not be expressed.

Table 1. A final list of descriptors modified from the Welfare Quality® assessment protocol for pigs, including groupings used during the focus group discussion based on word similarity

Recruit: Gathering the experts

Ten pig experts (researchers, veterinarians, and farmers, including three co-authors of this manuscript) were recruited to assist with the defining of descriptors and participate in a focus group-style discussion in May 2022. Experts with experience in different sectors of swine care, medicine, and research were selected to ensure diversity of perspectives when creating definitions.

Define: Preparations

Prior to the focus group, experts were provided with a 6-min video to familiarise them with the process of QBA. The video content consisted of an introduction on the history and development of QBA, what QBA is, practical applications of QBA, and details on the differences between FCP and FL. After viewing the introductory materials, experts were instructed to submit preliminary definitions for each QBA descriptor based solely on their prior experience working with pigs. Descriptor definitions were to begin with the phrase, “A *descriptor* sow is…” to increase continuity between definitions. Experts were not given any information about the sows within the experiment prior to the focus group to maximise the likelihood of developing definitions with broad applicability beyond the scope of current study. The ten experts submitted one definition for each of the 20 descriptors. Definitions were compiled by the research team using R (R Core Team 2021) to analyse responses for common trends in words and phrases. A representative definition for each descriptor was created based on these trends in responses. In cases where submitted definitions included outliers from representative definitions, the outliers were collected and presented alongside representative definitions during the focus group, ensuring all points of view were reflected during discussion. Outliers were recognised as either definitions with keywords and themes minimally represented in trends or submissions that expressed concerns about the descriptor rather than a definition.

Representative definitions were provided to the expert panel one day prior to the focus group to allow the expert panel time to familiarise themselves with the definitions and serve as a starting point to facilitate discussion during the focus group. Experts were instructed to have the provided list of representative definitions readily available during the focus group.

Define: Creating the definitions with the expert panel

Discussion was moderated by a co-author with previous experience training others in QBA. The focus group began with a brief introduction to the research project, presenting the experts with the background of the research team, the goals of the study, and the timeline for the focus group. Then, experts were read each of the 20 representative definitions one-by-one with an accompanying slide displaying the descriptor and definition. Once presented with the definitions, experts participated in a mock QBA session, where they scored four randomly selected, 1 min 40 s videos of sows within a novel arena taken from an ongoing study (Chou & Parsons Reference J-Y and Parsons2022). Experts were instructed to complete QBA based on the representative definitions to test their efficacy.

Experts began their discussion of descriptor definitions following the mock QBA session. Experts were once again presented the 20 representative definitions, this time in six groupings based on descriptor similarities (Table 1). These groupings were created only as an aid to facilitate discussion and served no purpose beyond the focus group. Outlier definitions, including submissions expressing concerns rather than definitions, were presented to the experts prior to discussion of that descriptor. Experts were instructed to freely discuss each descriptor led by the moderator and reach a final consensus definition. For descriptors that did not reach a final consensus due to time limitations, definitions were formulated by the research team based on main discussion points from the experts.

Vote: Quantifying agreement

Two weeks after the focus group, definitions as agreed upon during the focus group or formulated by the research team based on discussion were distributed to the experts. Anonymous voting forms were distributed online via Google Forms to quantify agreement for each final consensus definition. Experts were instructed to indicate whether they agreed or disagreed with the consensus definition. If they did not agree, experts could provide feedback through a subsequent short answer question on what they did not agree with. Descriptors were modified based on feedback as needed and re-voted on until 80% agreement was reached for each descriptor.

Test: Trying out the definitions

Five of the original ten experts (excluding the three co-authors) volunteered to commit additional time to participate in the second phase to verify descriptor definitions (hereafter referred to as ‘verifying experts’). Verifying expert identities were anonymised so researchers would be blinded to the results. An identification code was self-created by prompting verifying experts to answer three questions the same way at the beginning of each form: What is your favourite animal? What is your favourite colour? And what is your favourite food? These responses generated a unique code used to track the responses of individual assessors while keeping their identities anonymous.

Verifying experts were provided with 12 pre-selected videos and instructed to view each one and complete the QBA. Videos used for QBA scoring were chosen from a library of videos compiled from the same previously mentioned study and were selected to represent a wide range of behaviours exhibited by sows based on previous ethological coding. For each video, verifying experts scored the 20 descriptors listed in alphabetical order using a visual analogue scale (VAS) ranging from 0 (minimum expression of the descriptor) to 125 (maximum expression of the descriptor), administered online via Zoho Forms (Zoho Corporation 2022), and were instructed to complete assessments based on the developed descriptor definitions. Verifying experts dragged a slider along each VAS to a point they felt appropriately represented the level of each descriptor displayed. QBA results were collected for statistical analysis.

Verify: Statistical analysis

QBA scores from all five verifying experts were combined and analysed using R (R Core Team 2021). Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was applied to reduce the dimensionality of the QBA scores using the PCA function in the FactoMineR package with a correlation matrix and no rotation. The first two principal components with Eigenvalues greater than 1 that contributed to most of the variation were selected. The appropriateness of employing PCA to our data set was verified in two ways. Barlett’s Test of Sphericity (cortest.bartlett function in the psych package) was implemented to suggest sufficient correlation between variables. Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin factor adequacy analysis (KMO function in the psych package) was used to test the sampling adequacy of the model. Main factors or principal components (PCs) identified by PCA as well as individual descriptor scores were tested for inter-observer reliability using Kendall Correlation Coefficient W (KendallW function in DescTools library). Kendall’s W values can vary from 0 (no agreement at all) to 1 (complete agreement), with values higher than 0.6 showing substantial agreement (Landis & Koch Reference Landis and Koch1977). Two linear mixed models were performed using the lmer function in the lme4 package with video as a fixed effect and verifying expert as a random factor to test the impact of each video on PC1 and PC2 scores. A one-way ANOVA was performed to test significant effects of each video.

Results

‘Define’ step: Qualitative description of generating the definitions

Experts deliberated and agreed upon definitions for 17 out of 20 descriptors over the 4-h long focus group. The process started with the review of initial representative definitions of the descriptors to identify the parts of the representative definitions that would be included in final definitions.

Many of the initial representative definitions contained another descriptor. For example, the most common word in the initial definition of ‘happy’ was ‘relaxed’ and created issues in differentiating descriptors. The panel focused upon maintaining separation between descriptor definitions by not including descriptors in other definitions. Discussion was heavily focused on differentiations between positively valenced descriptors, specifically the differentiation between descriptors in Group 4 (‘calm’ and ‘relaxed’) and Group 5 (‘content’, ‘happy’, and ‘enjoying’). Consideration was given to combining descriptors from each grouping into one but was ultimately decided against. Definitions for the descriptors, ‘content’, ‘happy’, and ‘enjoying’ were not agreed upon due to time limitations on discussion. ‘Happy’ generated a large amount of discussion due to difficulty articulating what happiness looks like in a sow. For these descriptors, consensus definitions were created by the research team based on notes taken during the discussion of each descriptor and presented after the expert panel for subsequent voting to establish a consensus.

Experts also discussed when physical actions (behaviours) were needed in a definition versus instances in which describing how an animal is interacting with its environment sufficed. For example, the final definition for ‘curious’ states a sow “is inquisitive and interested in her environment [and] may actively approach objects and situations of interest or be investigating all aspects of where she is.” This definition contains descriptions of both how the sow acts (“inquisitive and interested in her environment”) and what the sow is doing (“actively approaches objects and situations of interest”, “investigating all aspects of where she is”). Careful consideration was given to the wording of these types of definitions to ensure the listing of actions was not criteria for fitting into a descriptor. The final list of descriptor definitions is presented below (Table 2).

Table 2. Final consensus definitions as agreed upon by the expert panel

Quantitative analysis of descriptor definitions

The first two principal components together explained 57.89% of the variation between videos (35.80 and 22.09% for PC1 and PC2, respectively). Table 3 shows loadings of each descriptor on the first two PCs.

Table 3. PCA of the QBA descriptors

Loadings with positive or negative values higher than 0.6 are shown in bold.

Many of the descriptors loaded strongly on the first PC and ranged from ‘enjoying/relaxed’ to ‘tense/frustrated’, suggesting that this component may describe the valence of sows’ affective states. The second PC seems more related to the level of arousal and ranges from ‘active/lively’ to ‘indifferent/calm’. The distribution of the descriptors across the first two PCs is shown in a loading plot (Figure 1). The valence and arousal of each descriptor are clearly defined along the axes, with negatively valenced descriptors near the left of the plot, positively valenced descriptors near the right, lower arousal descriptors near the bottom, and higher arousal descriptors near the top. The positioning of each descriptor on the loading plot is intuitive and lines up with the interpreted valence and arousal of each descriptor.

Figure 1. PC loadings for each descriptor. The colouring of the descriptors is representative of the strength of descriptors’ loadings, as determined by the factoextra package.

The appropriateness of employing PCA to our data set was verified in two ways. Barlett’s Test of Sphericity was highly significant (c 2 = 1,106, df = 190; P < 0.0001) suggesting sufficient correlation between variables. Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) factor adequacy analysis yielded an overall measure of sample adequacy of 0.8 on a scale of 0 (no sampling adequacy) to 1 (perfect sample adequacy). The lowest KMO score for an individual descriptor was 0.65 with 17 of the 20 scores being 0.7 or higher.

Agreement among the verifying experts ranged from almost perfect on PC1 (Kendall’s W = 0.91) to substantial agreement for PC2 (Kendall’s W = 0.66). High inter-observer reliability was also found between assessors for the individual QBA descriptor scores (Table 4) as only two out of the twenty descriptors exhibited less than moderate agreement (Kendall’s W < 0.4).

Table 4. Kendall’s W values for each of the QBA descriptors separately

Descriptors with Kendall’s W reflecting substantial agreement (W > 0.6) or better are shown in bold.

Sows in the different videos were perceived by the observers as being in different emotional states. The PC dimension combinations are unique for each video and distributed across all four of the valence-by-arousal quadrants (Figure 2). Subsequent analysis with a linear mixed model revealed a significant effect of videos on both PC1 and PC2 (P < 0.001, respectfully) demonstrating the effectiveness of these definitions in differentiating emotional state in sows.

Figure 2. Mean PC values for each of the 12 selected videos. Videos are distributed along all four quadrants, indicating sows within each video were perceived as being in a different emotional state from one another.

Discussion

The objective of the present study was to document a transparent and understandable process for generating and then verifying reliable and meaningful definitions for QBA descriptors starting from a pre-existing fixed list of descriptors for pigs. Previous research into FL QBA has focused upon how the FL of descriptors were generated, which was typically carried out via a literature review and discussions among experts, but they did not provide specific details on how experts discussed the definitions and reached consensus (Grosso et al. Reference Grosso, Battini, Wemelsfelder, Barbieri, Minero, Dalla Costa and Mattiello2016; Minero et al. Reference Minero, Dalla Costa, Dai, Canali, Barbieri, Zanella, Pascuzzo and Wemelsfelder2018; Stubsjøen et al. Reference Stubsjøen, Moe, Bruland, Lien and Muri2020). This study is the first ever attempt to fully describe a systematic process for how a FL of QBA descriptors can be soundly defined. Further elucidating the process of defining the descriptors can prevent misunderstanding, improve the quality of assessor training and agreement, and strengthen the robustness of QBA as a measure of animal welfare.

QBA is a holistic approach that provides insight into the emotionality of an animal by assessing how an animal behaves, rather than simply what it is doing (Wemelsfelder et al. Reference Wemelsfelder, Hunter, Mendl and Lawrence2001; Andreasen et al. Reference Andreasen, Wemelsfelder, Sandøe and Forkman2013). Given QBA’s holistic nature, the identification of commonalities in scoring patterns as identified through PCA is of more relevance than the use of individual QBA descriptors (Clarke et al. Reference Clarke, Pluske and Fleming2016). Our study shows an excellent agreement of verifying experts on PC1 that is more related to the valence of emotions, meaning that the experts consistently agreed on sows’ expression of negative and positive emotions. A good agreement was reached on PC2 that is more related to the arousal of emotions. The valence-arousal interpretation of emotional affect is frequently used in animal welfare and behaviour research (Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Burman and Paul2010), and common across QBA studies in both pigs (Wemelsfelder & Millard Reference Wemelsfelder, Millard, Forkman and Keeling2009; Temple et al. Reference Temple, Manteca, Dalmau and Velarde2013; Oldham et al. Reference Oldham, Arnott, Camerlink, Doeschl-Wilson, Farish, Wemelsfelder and Turner2021) and other species (Diaz-Lundahl et al. Reference Diaz-Lundahl, Hellestveit, Stubsjøen, J. Phythian, Oppermann Moe and Muri2019; Cooke et al. Reference Cooke, Mullan, Morten, Hockenhull, Lee, Cardenas and Rivero2022; Travnik et al. Reference Travnik, Machado and Sant’Anna2022). In this study, the experts were able to distinguish whether the sows were in a positive or negative emotional state but were less likely to agree on arousal. A previous study investigating the implementation of FL descriptors in pigs, although without descriptor definitions, also reported better consensus for valence than arousal (Wemelsfelder & Millard Reference Wemelsfelder, Millard, Forkman and Keeling2009). However, our results showed higher inter-observer agreement compared to Wemelsfelder and Millard (Reference Wemelsfelder, Millard, Forkman and Keeling2009) for both valence (W = 0.91 > W = 0.82), and arousal (W = 0.66 > W = 0.56).

We explored the agreement on each descriptor to better understand the implementation of our definitions. For half of the descriptors (ten of 20), the agreement of verifying experts was high and ranged from a low of W = 0.68 for ‘active’, ‘content’ and ‘distressed’ to a high of W = 0.88 for ‘playful’. Wemelsfelder and Millard (Reference Wemelsfelder, Millard, Forkman and Keeling2009) also found high agreement in 22 out of 33 FL descriptors though it is important to note that only 16 of their 33 descriptors were included in our modified FL. Other studies implementing FL descriptors in other species, namely sheep and dairy cattle, found high agreement in six out of 13 descriptors (Muri & Stubsjøen Reference Muri and Stubsjøen2017) and 15 out of 20 descriptors (Bokkers et al. Reference Bokkers, de Vries, Antonissen and de Boer2012), respectively. Though good agreement was shown overall, our verifying experts showed lower agreement on certain individual descriptors. ‘Aimless’ is an example of this low agreement (W = 0.39). Difficulties in defining this descriptor already occurred during the focus group. Similar low agreement was found for ‘fearful’ (W = 0.39). Lower disagreement could also be due to the absence of certain emotional expressions that were not expressed by animals within the selected videos.

Pig experts were chosen for this study due to their familiarity with pigs and similar expertise, as previous studies investigating behaviour in shelter dogs found that welfare assessors with similar levels of experience working with a species assessed welfare in a similar manner (Munch et al. Reference Munch, Wapstra, Thomas, Fisher and Sinn2019). Since our newly developed definitions will be used to train a wide range of assessors in subsequent studies, we considered it coherent for the swine experts from the panel to complete the verification of these definitions. This also allowed for the possibility to fine tune the definitions, if necessary, before applying them to train other demographic groups to assess welfare, as the main task for our verifying experts was to test the definitions of the descriptors in QBA, rather than conduct a welfare assessment. However, it also is possible that this subset of observers who were also involved in the process of creating descriptor definitions might exhibit a more nuanced interpretation of the QBA descriptor definitions and, despite their similar backgrounds, yield some of the response variation we observed (aimless’, ‘fearful’, etc).

QBA is unique from traditional, ethological-based welfare assessments as it considers more than just an animal’s physical actions. During focus group discussion, experts spent sufficient time delineating physical actions a sow may be performing. Definitions containing physical actions were worded very carefully, particularly via inclusion of the word ‘may’ prior to any physical actions described. Concerns with the inclusion of physical actions arose when experts were worried that future QBA assessors using these descriptor definitions would believe a sow must be performing these actions to be considered as fitting into a certain descriptor, i.e. a curious sow must be actively approaching objects or situations of interest and investigating all aspects of where she is to be considered curious. We also acknowledge that as emotions are difficult to express in animals, especially when they can only be described using ‘human’ vocabularies resemblant of feelings (Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Neville and Paul2022), diction plays an important role in the creation of descriptor definitions. It may appear that, in some instances, the definitions could lead to circularity when it comes to their interpretation (i.e. a ‘fearful sow is defined as being afraid, but it is also true that an afraid sow could be described as ‘fearful’). The circularity of the vocabulary surrounding emotional expression has been a long-standing discussion in semantics (Storm & Storm Reference Storm and Storm1987), and it is argued that this debate can extend into discussion pertaining to animal emotion as well. Unlike defining an ethogram, where it is possible to use neutral terms to describe behaviours (Bateson & Martin Reference Bateson and Martin2021), it is not possible to define emotions using neutral terms. Therefore, definitions carefully deliberated upon by a group of pig experts using our systematic approach aim to serve as a guide to future assessors and help to ensure assessors would not think a sow must be performing an action to have a descriptor potentially apply to her during assessment.

Historically, animal welfare research has tended to focus upon studying negative affect (Yeates & Main Reference Yeates and Main2008) since negative experiences are typically more profound and therefore easier to perceive and study than positive experiences (Boissy et al. Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007). However, QBA has been identified as being a promising technique for identifying positive affect (Temple et al. Reference Temple, Manteca, Velarde and Dalmau2011; de Boyer des Roches et al. Reference de Boyer des Roches, Lussert, Marion, Vincent, Pascal, Denys, Wemelsfelder and Foucras2018; Schmitt et al. Reference Schmitt, O’Driscoll, Baxter and Boyle2019) and is the only validated method for recording positive emotion in EU animal welfare assessment protocols (Welfare Quality® 2009; AWIN 2015). During the focus group, experts were able to agree upon all but three descriptor definitions: ‘content’, ‘happy’, and ‘enjoying’. Lack of consensus could be caused in part by the time limitations of the focus group and the similarity of the three descriptors, but also the difficulty in defining and recognising positively valenced emotions. Experts, indeed, spent most of their discussion focused on defining positively valenced descriptors. Although positive affect can be recognised with QBA, there remain challenges to expressing it in explicit terms, even by pig experts. Despite the potential difficulties, verifying experts displayed good agreement in the use of all three descriptors when implementing QBA (‘content’, W = 0.68; ‘happy’, W = 0.74; ‘enjoying’, W = 0.81), suggesting that definitions developed were sufficient. This also highlights the benefit of a systematic approach, as descriptors that may be harder to comprehend and easier to confuse were identified through the focus group discussion, which can be strengthened during the actual assessor training.

Two pairs of descriptors (‘agitated/irritable’ and ‘playful/positively occupied’) showed nearly identical PC loadings on PCs 1 and 2, causing almost complete overlap visually on the loading plot. This can be explained by the perceived similarity of the descriptors. During the expert panel, ‘agitated/irritable’ and ‘playful/positively occupied’ were identified by the research team and experts as being similar. During verification, ‘irritable’ displayed only moderate agreement between verifying experts (W = 0.49), while all other descriptors had favourable agreement (‘agitated’, W = 0.76; ‘playful’, W = 0.88; ‘positively occupied’, W = 0.79). While ‘agitated’ could be easily recognised in sows, it is possible this is not the case for ‘irritable’, a much more subtle emotion. Additionally, the barren environment of the novel arena may impact the sows’ ability to express the full repertoire of their behaviours and emotions, hence making certain emotions more difficult to assess (Haskell et al. Reference Haskell, Wemelsfelder, Mendl, Calvert and Lawrence1996). We used videos of sows taken in a novel arena, in the absence of any cues revealing how they were housed, to avoid the possibility of observer bias as previously reported for behaviour observation and QBA (Tuyttens et al. Reference Tuyttens, de Graaf, Heerkens, Jacobs, Nalon, Ott, Stadig, Van Laer and Ampe2014). Standardising the development of QBA definitions promises to be an essential first step toward a more detailed understanding of the possible role of observer bias in QBA studies. As we intended to use the WQ list without major alterations to enable subsequent cross-study comparison or meta-analysis, these four similar descriptors were not removed from the FL, though future studies could investigate the potential modification of these descriptors to further appropriate them based upon the assessed conditions or consider removing or adding descriptors.

Animal welfare implications

QBA is an already well-studied and heavily used welfare assessment tool in many species. QBA’s value lies particularly in its ability to identify positive emotional states, as is often difficult to do with other methods of welfare evaluation. By refining and making clear the processes that go into the defining of FL descriptors in pigs, QBA as a welfare assessment tool will be better suited to assess positive aspects of pig welfare and increase the transparency and standardisation of the process.

Conclusion

Our study set out to clearly define the procedures for generating reliable and meaningful definitions for a pre-existing set of fixed list QBA descriptors for sows. Our process detailed a systematic procedure used for creating and verifying descriptor definitions and is the first of its kind detailing this information in pigs. The results of this study promise a stronger, more reliable use of FL QBA for sow welfare assessment in the future.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Pennsylvania Center for Poultry and Livestock Excellence and the Pennsylvania Department of Agriculture. Special thanks to Marisol Parada Sarmiento for her contributions to data visualisation and Maria Camila Ceballos Betancourt for informative discussions about QBA.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: MB, TDP; Data curation: SI; Formal analysis: SI, MB, TDP; Funding acquisition: J-YC, TDP; Methodology: J-YC, MB; Supervision: TDP; Writing (original draft): SI, J-YC, MB; Writing (review & editing): TDP.

References

Andreasen, SN, Wemelsfelder, F, Sandøe, P and Forkman, B 2013 The correlation of Qualitative Behavior Assessments with Welfare Quality® protocol outcomes in on-farm welfare assessment of dairy cattle. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 143: 917. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.11.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
AWIN 2015 AWIN Welfare assessment protocol for goats. https://doi.org/10.13130/AWIN_GOATS_2015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, M and Martin, P 2021 Measuring Behavior: An Introductory Guide, 4th Edition. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Battini, M, Barbieri, S, Vieira, A, Can, E, Stilwell, G and Mattiello, S 2018 The use of Qualitative Behaviour Assessment for the on-farm welfare assessment of dairy goats. Animals 8: 123. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8070123CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blokhuis, HJ, Veissier, I, Miele, M and Jones, B 2010 The Welfare Quality® project and beyond: Safeguarding farm animal well-being. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica, Section A Animal Science 60: 129140. https://doi.org/10.1080/09064702.2010.523480Google Scholar
Boissy, A, Manteuffel, G, Jensen, MB, Moe, RO, Spruijt, B, Keeling, LJ, Winckler, C, Forkman, B, Dimitrov, I, Langbein, J, Bakken, M, Veissier, I and Aubert, A 2007 Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiology & Behavior 92: 375397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bokkers, EAM, de Vries, M, Antonissen, ICMA and de Boer, IJM 2012 Inter- and intra-observer reliability of experienced and inexperienced observers for the Qualitative Behaviour Assessment in dairy cattle. Animal Welfare 21: 307318. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.21.3.307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carreras, R, Mainau, E, Arroyo, L, Moles, X, González, J, Bassols, A, Dalmau, A, Faucitano, L, Manteca, X and Velarde, A 2016 Housing conditions do not alter cognitive bias but affect serum cortisol, qualitative behaviour assessment and wounds on the carcass in pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 185: 3944. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.09.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
J-Y, Chou and Parsons, TD 2022 Tell me how you feel – using a modified novel arena test (MNAT) to assess sow welfare in three different post-weaning housings. Proceedings of the Advancing Animal Welfare Science – UFAW International Conference. June 2022, Edinburgh, UK.Google Scholar
Clarke, T, Pluske, JR and Fleming, PA 2016 Are observer ratings influenced by prescription? A comparison of Free Choice Profiling and Fixed List methods of Qualitative Behavioural Assessment. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 177: 7783. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.01.022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooke, AS, Mullan, SM, Morten, C, Hockenhull, J, Lee, MRF, Cardenas, LM and Rivero, MJ 2022 V-QBA vs. QBA—How do video and live analysis compare for Qualitative Behaviour Assessment? Frontiers in Veterinary Science 9: 832239. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2022.832239CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Boyer des Roches, A, Lussert, A, Marion, Faure, Vincent, Herry, Pascal, Rainard, Denys, Durand, Wemelsfelder, F and Foucras, G 2018 Dairy cows under experimentally-induced Escherichia coli mastitis show negative emotional states assessed through Qualitative Behaviour Assessment. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 206: 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.06.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaz-Lundahl, S, Hellestveit, S, Stubsjøen, SM, J. Phythian, C, Oppermann Moe, R and Muri, K 2019 Intra- and inter-observer reliability of Qualitative Behaviour Assessments of housed sheep in Norway. Animals 9: 569. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9080569CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duijvesteijn, N, Benard, M, Reimert, I and Camerlink, I 2014 Same pig, different conclusions: Stakeholders differ in Qualitative Behaviour Assessment. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics 27: 10191047. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10806-014-9513-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosso, L, Battini, M, Wemelsfelder, F, Barbieri, S, Minero, M, Dalla Costa, E and Mattiello, S 2016 On-farm Qualitative Behaviour Assessment of dairy goats in different housing conditions. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 180: 5157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.04.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haskell, M, Wemelsfelder, F, Mendl, MT, Calvert, S and Lawrence, AB 1996 The effect of substrate-enriched and substrate-impoverished housing environments on the diversity of behaviour in pigs. Behaviour 133: 741761. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853996X00459CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landis, JR and Koch, GG 1977 The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33: 159174. https://doi.org/10.2307/2529310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendl, M, Burman, OHP and Paul, ES 2010 An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277: 28952904. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendl, M, Neville, V and Paul, ES 2022 Bridging the gap: Human emotions and animal emotions. Affective Science 3: 703712. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42761-022-00125-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minero, M, Dalla Costa, E, Dai, F, Canali, E, Barbieri, S, Zanella, A, Pascuzzo, R and Wemelsfelder, F 2018 Using qualitative behaviour assessment (QBA) to explore the emotional state of horses and its association with human-animal relationship. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 204: 5359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.04.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minero, M, Dalla Costa, E, Dai, F, Murray, LAM, Canali, E and Wemelsfelder, F 2016 Use of Qualitative Behaviour Assessment as an indicator of welfare in donkeys. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 174: 147153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2015.10.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, T, Pluske, J, Miller, D, Collins, T, Barnes, AL, Wemelsfelder, F and Fleming, PA 2014 Socialising piglets in lactation positively affects their post-weaning behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 158: 2333. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2014.06.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munch, KL, Wapstra, E, Thomas, S, Fisher, M and Sinn, DL 2019 What are we measuring? Novices agree amongst themselves (but not always with experts) in their assessment of dog behaviour. Ethology 125: 203211. https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12846CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muri, K and Stubsjøen, S 2017 Inter-observer reliability of Qualitative Behavioural Assessments (QBA) of housed sheep in Norway using fixed lists of descriptors. Animal Welfare 26: 427435. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.26.4.427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Napolitano, F, De Rosa, G, Grasso, F and Wemelsfelder, F 2012 Qualitative behaviour assessment of dairy buffaloes (Bubalus bubalis). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 141: 91100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.08.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oldham, L, Arnott, G, Camerlink, I, Doeschl-Wilson, A, Farish, M, Wemelsfelder, F and Turner, SP 2021 Once bitten, twice shy: Aggressive and defeated pigs begin agonistic encounters with more negative emotions. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 244: 105488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105488CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phythian, C, Michalopoulou, E, Duncan, J and Wemelsfelder, F 2013 Inter-observer reliability of Qualitative Behavioural Assessments of sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 144: 7379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.11.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team 2021 R: A language and environment for statistal computing. R foundation for statistical computing: Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Schmitt, O, O’Driscoll, K, Baxter, E and Boyle, L 2019 Artificial rearing affects the emotional state and reactivity of pigs post-weaning. Animal Welfare 28: 433442. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.28.4.433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stockman, CA, McGilchrist, P, Collins, T, Barnes, AL, Miller, D, Wickham, SL, Greenwood, PL, Cafe, LM, Blache, D, Wemelsfelder, F and Fleming, PA 2012 Qualitative Behavioural Assessment of Angus steers during pre-slaughter handling and relationship with temperament and physiological responses. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 142: 125133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.10.016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storm, C and Storm, T 1987 A taxonomic study of the vocabulary of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53: 805816. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.4.805CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stubsjøen, SM, Moe, RO, Bruland, K, Lien, T and Muri, K 2020 Reliability of observer ratings: Qualitative behaviour assessments of shelter dogs using a fixed list of descriptors. Veterinary and Animal Science 10: 100145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vas.2020.100145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Temple, D, Manteca, X, Dalmau, A and Velarde, A 2013 Assessment of test–retest reliability of animal-based measures on growing pig farms. Livestock Science 151: 3545. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.livsci.2012.10.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Temple, D, Manteca, X, Velarde, A and Dalmau, A 2011 Assessment of animal welfare through behavioural parameters in Iberian pigs in intensive and extensive conditions. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 131: 2939. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.01.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Travnik, I, Machado, D and Sant’Anna, A 2022 Do you see the same cat that I see? Inter- and intra-observer reliability for Qualitative Behaviour Assessment as temperament indicator in domestic cats. Animal Welfare 31: 319327. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.31.3.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tuyttens, FAM, de Graaf, S, Heerkens, JLT, Jacobs, L, Nalon, E, Ott, S, Stadig, L, Van Laer, E and Ampe, B 2014 Observer bias in animal behaviour research: can we believe what we score, if we score what we believe? Animal Behaviour 90: 273280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vindevoghel, TV, Fleming, PA, Hyndman, TH, Musk, GC, Laurence, M and Collins, T 2019 Qualitative Behavioural Assessment of Bos indicus cattle after surgical castration. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 211: 95102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.11.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welfare Quality® 2009 Welfare Quality® assessment protocol for pigs (sows and piglets, growing and finishing pigs). Welfare Quality® Consortium: Lelystad, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Wemelsfelder, F, Haskell, M, Mendl, MT, Calvert, S and Lawrence, AB 2000 Diversity of behaviour during novel object tests is reduced in pigs housed in substrate-impoverished conditions. Animal Behaviour 60: 385394. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1466CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wemelsfelder, F, Hunter, TEA, Mendl, MT and Lawrence, AB 2001 Assessing the ‘whole animal’: a free choice profiling approach. Animal Behaviour 62: 209220. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1741CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wemelsfelder, F and Millard, F 2009 Qualitative behaviour assessment. In: Forkman, B and Keeling, L (Eds.) Welfare Quality® Reports No 10: Assessment of Animal Welfare Measures for Sows, Piglets, and Fattening Pigs. Welfare Quality® Consortium: Lelystad, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Willis, RS, Fleming, PA, Dunston-Clarke, EJ, Barnes, AL, Miller, DW and Collins, T 2021 Animal welfare indicators for sheep during sea transport: The effect of voyage day and time of day. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 238: 105304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeates, JW and Main, DCJ 2008 Assessment of positive welfare: A review. The Veterinary Journal 175: 293300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2007.05.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zoho Corporation 2022 Zoho Forms. Zoho Corporation: Chennai, Tamil Nadu, India.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. A final list of descriptors modified from the Welfare Quality® assessment protocol for pigs, including groupings used during the focus group discussion based on word similarity

Figure 1

Table 2. Final consensus definitions as agreed upon by the expert panel

Figure 2

Table 3. PCA of the QBA descriptors

Figure 3

Figure 1. PC loadings for each descriptor. The colouring of the descriptors is representative of the strength of descriptors’ loadings, as determined by the factoextra package.

Figure 4

Table 4. Kendall’s W values for each of the QBA descriptors separately

Figure 5

Figure 2. Mean PC values for each of the 12 selected videos. Videos are distributed along all four quadrants, indicating sows within each video were perceived as being in a different emotional state from one another.