Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:40:42.209Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Spontaneous Activities of Captive Performing Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops Truncatus)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

L Galhardo*
Affiliation:
Institute of Ecology and Resource Management, University of Edinburgh, West Mains Road, Edinburgh EH9 3JG, UK
M C Appleby
Affiliation:
Institute of Ecology and Resource Management, University of Edinburgh, West Mains Road, Edinburgh EH9 3JG, UK
N K Waran
Affiliation:
Institute of Ecology and Resource Management, University of Edinburgh, West Mains Road, Edinburgh EH9 3JG, UK
M E dos Santos
Affiliation:
Unidade de Investigação em Eco-Etologia, Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada, Rua Jardim do Tabaco, 44, 1000 Lisboa, Portugal
*
Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints: Travessa do Possolo, 17, 4° piso, 1350 Lisboa, Portugal
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Despite the number of dolphins that have been kept in dolphinaria, and the many behavioural studies that have been conducted on captive dolphins, few have focused on their welfare. Some behaviours have been described in detail, but insufficient attention has been paid to the diurnal variations in their occurrence.

Behavioural observations were conducted upon two groups (two and six individuals each) of captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncates) over a period of six weeks. Behavioural descriptions were produced and the amount of time the animals spent engaged in different behaviours was sampled. General trends of both groups and differences in patterns of variation throughout the day were also analysed. The use of area by the dolphins and their degrees of association were recorded. Considerable individual variation and differences between the two groups were observed.

In comparing the behaviour of different dolphins and in considering their welfare it is necessary to take into account their marked degree of individuality. The observations have shown that behavioural data such as variability of behaviour patterns, spontaneous variations in the daily activities and frequency of playing and exploration may constitute good welfare indicators. It is argued that social diversity, appropriate physical characteristics of the pools, existence of play objects in the pools, easy access to visual contact with people, and frequent interactions with the trainers throughout the day at unscheduled times may be important ways of improving environmental stimulation.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 1996 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Altmann, J 1974 Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227266CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begon, M, Harper, J L and Townsend, C R 1991 Ökologie, Individuen - Populationen - Lebensgemeinschaften. Birkhauser: Basel, Switzerland (Cited by Gygax 1993)Google Scholar
Brown, D H and Norris, K S 1956 Observations of captive and wild cetaceans. Journal of Mammalogy 37: 311326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, M C and Caldwell, D K 1972 Behavior of marine mammals. In: Ridgway, S H (ed) Mammals of the Sea: Biology and Medicine pp 419459. Springfield Illinois: Thomas, USAGoogle Scholar
Defran, R H and Pryor, K 1980 The behavior and training of cetaceans in captivity. In: Herman, L M (ed) Cetacean Behavior, Mechanisms and Functions pp 319362. John Wiley and Sons: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Evans, P G H 1987 The Natural History of Whales and Dolphins. Christopher Helm: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Galhardo, L 1993 Behaviour of Bottlenose Dolphins in Two Dolphinaria. MSc Thesis, University of Edinburgh, ScotlandGoogle Scholar
Greenwood, A G 1977 A stereotyped behaviour pattern in dolphins. Aquatic Mammals 5: 1517Google Scholar
Gygax, L 1993 Spatial movement patterns and behaviour of two captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): absence of stereotyped behaviour or lack of definition? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 38: 337344CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordão, P 1992 Descrição dos Padrões de Comportamento Estereotipados de um Grupo de Golfinhos Tursiops truncatus no Jardim Zoológico de Lisboa. Graduation Report in Marine Biology and Fisheries, Universidade do Algarve, PortugalGoogle Scholar
Klinowska, M and Brown, S 1986 A Review of Dolphinaria. Department of the Environment: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Leatherwood, S and Reeves, R R 1983 The Sierra Club Handbook of Whales and Dolphins. Sierra Club Books: San Francisco, USAGoogle Scholar
Lilly, J C 1963 Distress call of the bottlenose dolphin: stimuli and evoked behavioral responses. Science 139: 116118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manteca, X and Deag, J M 1993a Individual differences in temperament of domestic animals: a review of methodology. Animal Welfare 2: 247268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manteca, X and Deag, J M 1993b Social roles in cattle: a pica for interchange of ideas between primatologists and applied ethologists. Animal Welfare 2: 339346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, P and Batcson, P 1986 Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UKGoogle Scholar
McBride, A F and Kritzler, H 1951 Observations on pregnancy, parturition, and post-natal behavior in the bottlenose dolphin. Journal of Mammalogy 32: 251266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puente, A E and Dewsbury, D A 1976 Courtship and copulatory behavior of bottlenoscd dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Cetology 21: 19Google Scholar
Saayman, G S, Tayler, C K and Bower, D 1973 Diurnal activity cycles in captive and free-ranging Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus ehrenburg). Behaviour 44: 212233CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, M D, Wells, R S and Irvine, A B 1990 A long-term study of bottlenose dolphin on the West Coast of Florida. In: Leatherwood, S and Reeves, R R (eds) The Bottlenose Dolphin pp 235244. Acadcmic Press: San Diego, USACrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, S and Castellan, N J 1988 Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition. McGraw-Hill International Editions: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Steering Group 1988 Dolphinaria. Department of the Environment: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Sweeney, J C 1990 Marine mammal behavioral diagnostics. In: Dierauf, L A (ed) CRC Handbook of Marine Mammal Medicine: Health, Disease and Rehabilitation pp 5372. CRC Press: Florida, USAGoogle Scholar
Tavolga, M C 1966 Behavior of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): social interactions in a captive colony. In: Norris K S (ed) Whales, Dolphins and Porpoises pp 718-730. University of California Press: Berkeley, USACrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tavolga, M C and Essapian, F S 1957 The behavior of the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): mating, pregnancy, parturition and mother-infant behavior. Zoologica 42: 1131Google Scholar
Tayler, C K and Saayman, G S 1973 Imitative behaviour by Indian ocean bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) in captivity. Behaviour 44: 286298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weaver, A C 1987 An Ethogram of Naturally Occurring Behavior of Bottlenose Dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, in Southern California Waters. MSc Thesis, University of San Diego, USAGoogle Scholar
Wemclsfelder, F 1993 Animal Boredom - Towards an Empirical Approach of Animal Subjectivity. PhD Thesis, University of Leiden, The NetherlandsGoogle Scholar