Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T07:14:26.584Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Non-invasive assessment of positive affective state using infra-red thermography in rats

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 September 2023

Chanakarn Wongsaengchan*
Affiliation:
School of Biodiversity, One Health & Veterinary Medicine, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8QQ, UK School of Psychology & Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, KY16 9JP, UK
Dominic J McCafferty
Affiliation:
School of Biodiversity, One Health & Veterinary Medicine, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8QQ, UK
Katie Lennox
Affiliation:
School of Biodiversity, One Health & Veterinary Medicine, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8QQ, UK
Ruedi G Nager
Affiliation:
School of Biodiversity, One Health & Veterinary Medicine, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8QQ, UK
Dorothy EF McKeegan
Affiliation:
School of Biodiversity, One Health & Veterinary Medicine, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8QQ, UK
*
Corresponding author: Chanakarn Wongsaengchan; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

With recent increased focus on positive welfare in animal welfare science, there is demand for objective positive welfare indicators. It is unclear whether changes in body surface temperature can be used to non-invasively identify and quantify positive states in mammals. We recorded continuous measurements of tail surface temperature using infra-red thermography (IRT) and concurrent behavioural observations in male and female Wistar rats (Rattus norvegicus). If tail surface temperature can differentiate between positive and negative experiences, we expect a qualitatively different response compared to negative experiences. Three groups of rats were presented with increasing magnitudes of food rewards (neutral/none, one and three rewards). The rats were placed in an arena to which they were habituated and filmed for 30 s before and 30 min after exposure to different rewards. Tail temperature initially decreased from the pre-reward baseline and subsequently returned towards baseline temperature. The overall pattern of the change was the same as for rats subjected to negative stimuli in previous studies. Nevertheless, dynamic changes in tail temperature, specifically the rate of recovery and the behavioural response (exploration), differed between neutral and rewarded rats but failed to distinguish reward magnitude. Sex differences were found in both thermal and behavioural responses, unrelated to reward magnitudes. Female rats exhibited a greater initial response with a slower recovery than male rats, emphasising the value of using of both sexes in animal welfare research. This study improves our understanding of the effects of positive emotions induced by food reward on peripheral body temperature and behaviour.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

In animal welfare science, the focus is shifting from assessing and avoiding or reducing negative experiences and suffering in animals (Broom & Johnson Reference Broom and Johnson1993; Farm Animal Welfare Council [FAWC] 2009; Hawkins et al. Reference Hawkins, Morton, Burman, Dennison, Honess, Jennings, Lane, Middleton, Roughan, Wells and Westwood2011; Animals in Science Committee 2017) to also identifying and promoting positive welfare states (Boissy et al. Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007; Yeates & Main Reference Yeates and Main2008; Mellor Reference Mellor2016; Lawrence et al. Reference Lawrence, Vigors and Sandøe2019). Furthermore, to adequately address the complex nature of animal welfare, recent animal welfare concepts have incorporated the individual adaptability of animals and taken the dynamic nature of animal welfare over time into account (McMillan Reference McMillan2019; Arndt et al. Reference Arndt, Goerlich and van der Staay2022). Therefore, there is an increasing requirement for objective and reliable welfare indicators that are sensitive to the dynamics of positive affective states of animals.

Identifying an animal’s affective state is one of the biggest challenges in animal welfare science, the well-established approaches of identifying affective state using concomitant behavioural and physiological measures have been the basis of significant progress in animal welfare research (Möstl & Palme Reference Möstl and Palme2002; Olivier et al. Reference Olivier, Zethof, Pattij, van Boogaert, van Oorschot, Leahy, Oosting, Bouwknecht, Veening, van der Gugten and Groenink2003, Buynitsky & Mostofsky Reference Buynitsky and Mostofsky2009; Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Burman, Parker and Paul2009; Hubrecht & Kirkwood Reference Hubrecht and Kirkwood2010; Campos et al. Reference Campos, Fogaça, Aguiar and Guimarães2013). According to the dimensional perspective of affective states, the most commonly assumed dimensions are valence (negative or positive) and arousal (high or low) (Mauss & Robinson Reference Mauss and Robinson2009). The valence dimension contrasts states of pleasant (e.g. happy) with states of unpleasant (e.g. sad), and the arousal dimension contrasts states of low arousal (e.g. boredom) with states of high arousal (e.g. startle). Available physiological and behavioural measures of affective state differ in their sensitivity to arousal and valence, with behavioural responses being currently the main route to identify valence (Mellor Reference Mellor2015). Most available physiological measures lack valence and require invasive procedures (e.g. blood/tissue sampling, insertion of probe or surgical implantation of a data logger) that can impede the animal’s ability to freely express their behaviour and impact on affective state in itself (Burgdorf & Panksepp Reference Burgdorf and Panksepp2006; Boissy et al. Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007; Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Burman, Parker and Paul2009; Wöhr & Schwarting Reference Wöhr and Schwarting2009; Zupan et al. Reference Zupan, Buskas, Altimiras and Keeling2016; Duarte & Pinto-Gouveia Reference Duarte and Pinto-Gouveia2017; Alexander et al. Reference Alexander, Aragón, Bookwala, Cherbuin, Gatt, Kahrilas, Kästner, Lawrence, Lowe, Morrison, Mueller, Nusslock, Papadelis, Polnaszek, Helene Richter, Silton and Styliadis2021). Infra-red thermography (IRT) has recently emerged as a promising non-invasive physiological measure that may provide information on an animal’s affective state (Travain & Valsecchi Reference Travain and Valsecchi2021).

Changes in body temperature (a rise in core temperature and concurrent drop in body surface temperatures) reflect the stress-induced hyperthermia (SIH) phenomenon and is triggered via the sympathetic activation of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) (Diorio et al. Reference Diorio, Viau and Meaney1993; Oka et al. Reference Oka, Oka and Hori2001; Olivier et al. Reference Olivier, Zethof, Pattij, van Boogaert, van Oorschot, Leahy, Oosting, Bouwknecht, Veening, van der Gugten and Groenink2003; Bouwknecht et al. Reference Bouwknecht, Olivier and Paylor2007; Hänsel & von Känel Reference Hänsel and von Känel2008). Consequently, changes in body surface temperature measured with infra-red thermography have been used to non-invasively detect negative affective states in many endothermic species (for a review, see Travain & Valsecchi Reference Travain and Valsecchi2021). In systematic validation work, IRT has also been shown to be useful for quantifying arousal intensity of restraint stressors in hens (Gallus gallus domesticus) (Herborn et al. Reference Herborn, Graves, Jerem, Evans, Nager, McCafferty and McKeegan2015) and in laboratory Wistar rats (Rattus norvegicus) (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023). However, less is known about ANS responses to positive affective states, although some responses have been reported to be valence-specific (Kreibig Reference Kreibig2010; Shiota et al. Reference Shiota, Neufeld, Yeung, Moser and Perea2011; Wilhelm et al. Reference Wilhelm, Rattel, Wegerer, Liedlgruber, Schweighofer, Kreibig, Kolodyazhniy and Blechert2017; Ishii & Shinya Reference Ishii and Shinya2021), including skin temperature responses (Kreibig Reference Kreibig2010; Ioannou et al. Reference Ioannou, Gallese and Merla2014).

Few studies have used IRT to evaluate body surface temperature responses to positive stimuli. Studies in humans and non-human primates reported different directions (cooling/warming) of surface temperature of nose and fingers in response to negative and positive stimuli (Merla & Romani Reference Merla and Romani2007; Kreibig Reference Kreibig2010; Hahn et al. Reference Hahn, Whitehead, Albrecht, Lefevre and Perrett2012). In monkeys and apes, upper lip temperature rose after negative stimuli while nose temperature decreased, and eye temperature increased after positive stimuli (Chotard et al. Reference Chotard, Ioannou and Davila-Ross2018). This suggests that the direction of temperature change could differ according to the valence of the response, and that discrete emotions may induce different autonomic patterns as supported by some authors (Larsen et al. Reference Larsen, Berntson, Poehlmann, Ito and Cacioppo2008; Mauss & Robinson Reference Mauss and Robinson2009). In domestic dogs (Canis familiaris), a positive event (receiving food treats) led to an increase in eye temperature (Travain et al. Reference Travain, Colombo, Grandi, Heinzl, Pelosi, Prato Previde and Valsecchi2016). However, a pilot study found that a dog’s eye temperature also increased during a negative event (standardised veterinary examination) (Travain et al. Reference Travain, Colombo, Heinzl, Bellucci, Prato Previde and Valsecchi2015). Similarly, in chickens, a decrease in peripheral temperature was noted both when anticipating a positive event (Moe et al. Reference Moe, Stubsjøen, Bohlin, Flø and Bakken2012) and during a stressful situation (Edgar et al. Reference Edgar, Nicol, Pugh and Paul2013; Moe et al. Reference Moe, Bohlin, Flø, Vasdal and Stubsjøen2017). Nasal temperatures decreased in response to positive experiences in cows and non-human primates (Proctor & Carder Reference Proctor and Carder2015; Chotard et al. Reference Chotard, Ioannou and Davila-Ross2018), again the same as reported in negative experiences (Nakayama et al. Reference Nakayama, Goto, Kuraoka and Nakamura2005; Ioannou et al. Reference Ioannou, Chotard and Davila-Ross2015; Kano et al. Reference Kano, Hirata, Deschner, Behringer and Call2016; Heintz et al. Reference Heintz, Fuller and Allard2019). The extent to which changes in surface temperature may reflect changes in arousal (i.e. high or low) and/or valence (i.e. positive/pleasant or negative/unpleasant) is still not clear.

Rats have been used extensively in studies of emotions, where the focus has been on applying the results to humans, however this process has yielded a great amount of information on rat physiology, behaviour and welfare (Makowska & Weary Reference Makowska and Weary2013). Rat tails are well-vascularised with arterio-venous anastomoses and lack fur, thus providing a suitable region of interest (ROI) for IRT (Gemmell & Hales Reference Gemmell and Hales1977). Previous work in mice (Mus musculus) and rats including ours (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023) indicates that tail temperature should decrease in response to negative stimuli (Vianna & Carrive Reference Vianna and Carrive2005; Marks et al. Reference Marks, Vianna and Carrive2009; Reis et al. Reference Reis, Scopinho, Guimarães, Corrêa and Resstel2011; Fassini et al. Reference Fassini, Scopinho, Resstel and Correa2014, Reference Fassini, Scopinho, Alves, Fortaleza and Corrêa2017; Lecorps et al. Reference Lecorps, Rödel and Féron2016; Gjendal et al. Reference Gjendal, Franco, Ottesen, Sørensen and Olsson2018; Miyazono et al. Reference Miyazono, Hasegawa, Miyazaki, Sakakima, Konno, Meguro, Sasajima, Noguchi, Osada and Kashiwayanagi2018) and magnitude of stress is reflected in peripheral control of circulation in the tail (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023). There is no indication yet whether the experience of positive emotions would have a different effect on tail temperatures compared to negative emotions (valence), and whether different magnitudes of positive experiences (arousal) are reflected in differences in tail temperatures as is the case for negative experiences. While spontaneous behavioural tests, such as the open field test and elevated plus maze test, are commonly used in the study of animal negative emotion to standardise observational and stimulus techniques (Steimer Reference Steimer2011), the equivalent tests for positive emotions have not yet been validated. Play behaviour (Held & Špinka Reference Held and Špinka2011), anticipation of a reward (Spruijt et al. Reference Spruijt, Van den Bos and Pijlman2001), 50-kHz calls (especially when being tickled or during social play) (Boissy et al. Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007; Hinchcliffe et al. Reference Hinchcliffe, Mendl and Robinson2020), facial expression (Finlayson et al. Reference Finlayson, Lampe, Hintze, Würbel and Melotti2016) and optimistic cognitive bias (Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Burman, Parker and Paul2009), have all been linked to positive emotions in rats. In specific contexts where other indicators of positive emotions (e.g. play and anticipation) are not available, such as during open-field IRT filming, the simplest spontaneous approach and avoidance behaviour may be used to gauge the general valence (negative/positive) of a stimulus: while freezing, darting, attacking behaviours may reflect negative emotions, exploratory and consumptive behaviours may indicate specific, object-directed positive emotions (Paul et al. Reference Paul, Harding and Mendl2005). The use of spontaneous approach and avoidance behaviour was used in our previous study to validate IRT responses, cross-validated with hormonal and behavioural responses (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023).

In this study we used food reward to induce positive affective state and we aimed to assess whether tail surface temperature measured non-invasively with IRT can identify positive affective state (valence) in male and female rats in response to a food reward and whether IRT can also quantify increasing levels of reward magnitude (arousal). We used Honey Cheerios (Nestlé®, UK), a highly palatable sweet cereal, conventionally used as food reward in rats (Makowska Reference Makowska2016; Makowska & Weary Reference Makowska2016; Brydges & Hall Reference Brydges and Hall2017; Nip et al. Reference Nip, Adcock, Nazal, MacLellan, Niel, Choleris, Levison and Mason2019). We collected concurrent thermal and spontaneous behavioural responses for cross-validation. Since sex differences have been inconsistently reported in the responses of rats to food reward (Marshall et al. Reference Marshall, Liu, Murphy, Maidment and Ostlund2017; Sinclair et al. Reference Sinclair, Hildebrandt, Culbert, Klump and Sisk2017; Chowdhury et al. Reference Chowdhury, Wallin-Miller, Rear, Park, Diaz, Simon and Moghaddam2019), both sexes of rats were studied. We hypothesised that the tail surface temperature response to a food reward would differ between non-rewarded and rewarded rats and between rats exposed to different reward magnitudes as reflected by behavioural indicators of positive emotions in rats. Our objective was to provide evidence whether the body surface temperature can provide information on the valence and arousal of positive experiences in rats.

Materials and methods

Study animals and husbandry

All experimental procedures and data acquisition were carried out under UK Home Office authorisation (Project licence: PIFD5B3DB, Personal licence: I3D10B21C). The design and report of the study followed the ARRIVE (Animal Research: Reporting of In Vivo Experiments) guidelines 2.0 (Percie du Sert et al. Reference Percie du Sert, Hurst, Ahluwalia, Alam, Avey, Baker, Browne, Clark, Cuthill, Dirnagl, Emerson, Garner, Holgate, Howells, Karp, Lazic, Lidster, MacCallum, Macleod, Pearl, Petersen, Rawle, Reynolds, Rooney, Sena, Silberberg, Steckler and Würbel2020) and PREPARE (Planning Research and Experimental Procedures on Animals: Recommendations for Excellence) guideline (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Clutton, Lilley, Hansen and Brattelid2018) for reporting research in animals. Eighteen male and 18 female five week old dam-reared outbred albino Wistar rats (101–125 g on arrival, total n = 36) were acquired from Charles River (UK). By the time of testing, rats were 8 weeks old and had a mean (± SD) body mass of 257.97 (± 17.72) g (males) and 182.67 (± 10.21) g (females). Rats were housed in groups of three individuals of the same sex in a 48 × 37.5 × 21 cm (length × width × height) polycarbonate cage (Tecniplast, London, UK) and were maintained in a 12:12h light: dark cycle with lights on at 0700h. The mean (± SD) temperature and relative humidity of the room were 22.04 (± 1.95)°C and 55 (± 10)%, respectively. Animals had free access to ad libitum water and food (Maintenance and breeder pellets, CRM Special Diet Services, Witham, UK). In each cage, there was approximately 7-cm deep corn cob and sizzle nest bedding for burrowing, two cardboard tunnels and a 21.5 × 21.5 × 12.5 cm Sputnik rat house enrichment device (SAVIC nv®, Belgium). Rats were handled as part of normal husbandry with non-aversive handling tunnels (NC3Rs 2013) and were individually marked with a non-toxic animal marker (Stoelting Co, USA) at the end of the first habituation trial. All rats were inspected daily and found healthy. After the trials, the minority of rats were humanely euthanased and the majority retained and re-used under another Project Licence after veterinary certification of fitness.

Habituation phase

The experimental protocol consisted of three phases: acclimatisation; habituation; and testing (Figure 1). Upon arrival rats were put into home cages in groups of three of the same sex and left undisturbed for seven days to acclimatise to the housing unit. Following the acclimatisation, rats were habituated individually to the testing conditions. Since we recorded the surface temperature of rats using IRT and infra-red radiation cannot pass through the polycarbonate base of the home cages and the wire-mesh cover obstructs a clear view of the rat, we recorded individual thermal response to different treatments in a separate test arena. During the habituation and the testing periods, rats were transferred individually from their home cage to the test arena which was located in a separate room. The habituation period was designed to overcome, as far as possible, the stress response to temporary social isolation, the transportation, the novel testing arena and the novel food reward. Transfers were carried out using a transport cage (a white opaque polypropylene rat cage sized 56 × 38 × 17 cm; North Kent Plastic Cages, UK) covered with a raised wire lid, supplemented with a handful of the rats’ own cage bedding material. Rats were tunnel-handled between home cage, transport cage and testing arena. The familiar odour of the bedding material was intended to minimise novelty and maximise habituation (Wallace et al. Reference Wallace, Gorny and Whishaw2002; Burn Reference Burn2008). Rats were put into the test arena along with the bedding material from their home cage. The test arena was a grey, plastic, open-topped box (40 × 30 × 32.5 cm; Key Industrial Equipment™, Napa, CA, USA). To allow recording of infra-red radiation without any obstruction of the view, there was no lid on the testing arena. In cases where a rat jumped up to the top edge of the testing arena, it was immediately tunnel-handled back into the arena. The habituation trials mimicked the testing period except that the animal was not given a food reward and the duration of exposure to the test arena was gradually increased (5, 5, 10, 20, 30, 30-min duration represented as H1–6, respectively) across the six habituation trials and completed within six consecutive days for each rat (Figure 1). Each habituation trial included putting the experimenter’s hand into the arena once every trial to habituate rats to the experimenter’s hand which would be the mean to introduce food reward into the arena in the testing period. Two transport cages and test arenas were used alternately and were cleaned between rats and trials using tap water and alcohol disinfectant wipes (Medipal®, Pal International Ltd, UK). The day before the beginning of the habituation period and at the end of the habituation period, rats were given the food reward in their home cage in order to minimise the novelty fear of the food reward in the testing period. The habituation protocol was assessed as a separate study and significant reduction of thermal and behavioural responses to being exposed alone in the testing arena over the six trials of increasing duration were observed (Wongsaengchan Reference Wongsaengchan2022). All habituation was undertaken during the light phase from 0830–1730h, i.e. within 1.5 to 10.5 h after the onset of the light phase.

Figure 1. The three phases of the experimental protocol: acclimatisation; habituation; and testing. Rats were left undisturbed for the first week after arrival to acclimatise to the home cage. In the second week, six habituation trials of increasing duration of exposure to the test arena (5, 5, 10, 20, 30, 30-min duration represented as H1–6, respectively) were completed within six consecutive days; trials H1 and H2 were completed in one day and trials H5 and H6 were completed over three days. In the third week, each rat was tested by being put into the test arena and filmed with infra-red and video cameras for 30 s to record the baseline temperature and behaviour. Each rat was then exposed to one of three treatments (0, 1 or 3 Cheerios) and further filmed for 30 min.

Testing phase

Testing phase began one day after the habituation period was completed (Figure 1) and was conducted 2.5–7.5 h after the onset of the light phase. Systematic randomisation was used so that each of the three rats within each home cage were exposed to a different treatment. One- or three-jointed Cheerios (Honey Cheerios cereal, Nestlé®, UK) were used as two different reward magnitudes according to a previous report that rats preferred a reward which is higher in density and surface area (Wadhera et al. Reference Wadhera, Wilkie and Capaldi-Phillips2018). Rats that were offered no reward served as a neutral (control) group. The first 30 s in the test arena were used to obtain individual baseline measure for body surface temperature and behaviour. Then one of the three treatments either neutral (zero food reward), one piece of Cheerio or three-jointed pieces of Cheerios were placed to the bottom of the arena by hand. For the neutral group, the experimenter put an empty hand into the arena to control for the effect of rats’ exposure to a hand to be the same throughout the three groups. After the treatment was applied, the rat was filmed continuously for a further 30 min. The baseline tail temperature for each rat was averaged from three measurements every 10 s during the 30-s baseline filming. The 30-min response of the tail surface was then measured as the difference from each individual rat’s own baseline temperature (referred to as ‘difference from baseline’ hereafter). Rats were weighed once at the end of the experiment immediately after the test trial finished.

Imaging set-up

The rats were imaged with an infra-red thermal camera (FLIR A65, f = 25 mm, spatial resolution 0.68 mrad, thermal sensitivity < 0.05°C @ +30°C, FLIR Systems, Wilsonville, OR, USA) while in the testing arena. The thermal camera was mounted on a clamp stand which was positioned 55 cm above the floor of the test arena. The rats were also videoed with a GoPro HERO 7 Silver 4K Action Camera (GoPro Inc, San Mateo, USA) attached to the top of the arena with a mount (GorillaPod 500 Action, JOBY, CA, USA) for behavioural analysis. Both cameras were positioned such that the entire test arena was within their field of view. The thermal videos were recorded at a frame rate of 30 frames per min. Air temperature and relative humidity of the room were also measured at 5-min intervals during all trials with an EasyLog USB logger (Lascar Electronics Ltd, UK) attached to the camera stand.

Thermal data extraction

All thermal image sequences were extracted using FLIR ThermaCAM Researcher Pro 2.10™ (FLIR Systems Inc). The tail was the ROI used as it displays the most informative body surface temperatures in rats (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023). The emissivity of bare skin is 0.98 (McCafferty Reference McCafferty2007), the air temperature and relative humidity at the nearest 5 min and the distance between the object and the camera (55 cm) were inputted into the software. The accuracy of temperature readings was validated using black insulation tape attached to the EasyLog thermister (Wongsaengchan Reference Wongsaengchan2022). Thermal sequence images were viewed using the palette ‘rain’ (rainbow) and the most suitable thermal image (rat not sitting on its tail and their body and the head parallel with the floor) was selected every 10 s for the first 4 min after treatment exposure and every 60 s for the remaining video. For each selected thermal image, a line along the middle of the entire length of the tail was delineated manually using the ThermaCAM drawing tool ‘bendable line’ and (Figure 2[a]) eye temperature was also recorded and will be reported elsewhere. From the delineated tail we extracted the maximum temperature and then plotted the maximum temperature difference from baseline against time to produce the thermal response curve that we then compared between treatment groups and sexes (for a justification of sampling intervals and extracting maximum temperatures, see Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023). The tail thermal curve properties (Figure 2[b]) of each individual rat were also extracted for analysis as the dynamic response also exhibited the five curve properties and these were reported to be sensitive to arousal magnitude of negative stimuli, especially the rate of recovery (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023). If there was new defaecation or urination visible in the thermal images as an area on the substrate that was warmer than the surrounding at that sampling time-point, this was collectively termed ‘Elimination.’

Figure 2. Thermal data extraction. (a) Thermal image of a rat in the testing arena viewed in the ‘rain’ (rainbow) palette in ThermaCAM Researcher software. A bendable line (white arrow) was drawn manually to extract the maximum temperature of the whole length of the tail. Left and right eye temperatures (black arrow) were also recorded and will be reported elsewhere. (b) The schematic standardised tail surface temperature response to a stimulus, identifying five distinct curve properties. The amplitude of the initial decrease in individual tail temperature from baseline (Adrop, 1), defined as the minimum value of the temperature difference from baseline (T difference) before the first rise of temperature back towards the baseline, and the amplitude of the maximum recovery (Arecov, 2) was defined as the highest T difference value recorded after Adrop. The time elapsed (s) to reach Adrop was designated as Sdrop (3). The rate of change of temperature from Adrop to Arecov was represented by the slope Mrecov (4). The time elapsed (s) to reach Arecov was designated as Srecov (5).

Behavioural analysis

The ethogram we used in this study was adapted from Wongsaengchan et al. (Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023) and mutually exclusive behaviours observed and recorded are described in the ethogram (Supplementary Table 1). Behavioural data were collected using instantaneous sampling with a scan interval of 10 s based on continuous pilot observations of the behaviour of one female rat to find the optimum sampling interval (Martin & Bateson Reference Martin and Bateson2007). The observer was aware of the treatment allocation as it was visible in the videos but was blinded to the sex, cage and rat ID. Proportion of scans showing each behaviour was calculated per total scans per 10 min, excluding unidentified ‘Other’, and were then grouped into four behavioural groups using Principal Component Analysis (Supplementary Figure 1) with R package; ‘FactoMineR (Lê et al. Reference Lê, Josse and Husson2008) to reduce type I error from separate analysis of a large number of different behaviours. The four behavioural groups were: Explorative (Explore, Eat, Interact with object); Resting stationary (Rest, Stationary, Groom, Non-intake); Fear/Anxiety (Freeze, Dart); and Escape/Mobility (Escape, Rear, Wall grab, Climb, Walk). Latency to eat, the time from reward placement in the arena until the time rats started eating, was recorded for rats that received food rewards as the latency taken to approach food in a novel situation has been shown to be longer due to novelty fear or ‘bait shyness’ in anxious or stressed individuals to avoid the risks from food (Deacon Reference Deacon2011).

Statistical analysis

All analyses were completed in R version 4.1.1 (R Core Team 2019). The sample size of n = 6 rats per sex per treatment (either neutral, one Cheerio or three Cheerios) was based on the sample size of a similar study of surface temperature response to different arousal levels of acute restraint stress in laboratory rats (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023) calculated using 80% power and 0.9 smallest standardised effect size at the 5% significance level. One female rat which jumped out from the testing arena continuously for more than 10 min and one female rat that did not approach the food reward were excluded from the analyses as the first exhibited obvious stress and the second did not provide comparable data. Therefore, the female neutral and 3-Cheerios groups had only n = 5 while the other treatment groups had n = 6. The mean difference in each rat’s maximum tail temperature from their own baseline temperature for each time-point and the five tail thermal curve properties of each rat were used as response variables. These were analysed separately using general linear mixed models (GLMMs) with the ‘nlme’ package in R (Pinheiro et al. Reference Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy and Sarkar2018) with treatment, sex, time-point as a quadratic term to capture a non-linear relationship, time of day (TOD), environmental factors that can affect measured infra-red radiation (i.e. air temperature, humidity), animal factors that can affect measured infra-red radiation (i.e. location in the arena, posture, body mass) and possible interactions as explanatory variables and animal identity (Rat ID) as a random factor (see Supplementary Table 2). Behaviours were pooled into three time-blocks of 10 min each and the proportion of scans per 10 min showing each behavioural group used as response variables. ‘Elimination’ behaviour was observed from thermal images at a different interval than other behaviours and treated as counts per 10 min and analysed with a Poisson residual distribution. Correlations between behavioural response and thermal response were examined using GLMM and multivariate exploratory data analysis (see Supplementary Figure 3).

In all statistical models, non-significant terms were removed with backward-stepwise model simplification using the likelihood ratio test (LRT) at a significance level of 0.05. The post hoc test in the R package ‘lsmeans’ (Lenth Reference Lenth2016) was used to further examine statistically significant differences between groups where appropriate. Model assumptions were diagnosed with graphical tools and functions in R (Zuur et al. Reference Zuur, Ieno and Elphick2010) and the response variables were log- or square-root transformed where necessary to meet the normality of residuals, independence of residuals, co-linearity and homogeneity of variance assumptions suitable for GLMMs testing. When checking explanatory variables for co-linearity, body mass and sex were positively correlated (r = 0.80, df = 32) and had a high variance inflation factor (VIF = 8.83), therefore, ‘body mass’ was excluded from models that included ‘sex’ and was only included in models analysing male or female rats separately. The GLMMs models of tail temperature were examined for temporal autocorrelation using the ‘acf’ and ‘pacf’ plotting functions in R showed a significant autocorrelation at a lag of 1 and much lower spikes for the subsequent lags. Thus, the correlation correction ‘corAR1()’ was added to tail temperature GLMM models.

Results

Body surface temperature

Female rats had lower baseline tail temperatures (30.36 [± 1.70]°C, n = 16) than male rats (32.41 [± 0.92]°C, n = 18; LRT, 2∆LL = 82.93, df = 1; P < 0.001). After exposure to treatments, tail temperature initially decreased from the baseline in all groups and subsequently increased back towards baseline temperature and overshot the baseline (rewarded males and females) or continued to decrease (neutral females) (Figure 3). The tail thermal response curve was non-linear and differed between the three treatments (LRT, treatment-by-time: 2∆LL = 12.26, df = 2; P = 0.002; treatment-by-time2: 2∆LL = 14.44, df = 2; P < 0.001) and between the two sexes (LRT, sex-by-time: 2∆LL = 37.06, df = 1; P < 0.001; sex-by-time2: 2∆LL = 22.58, df = 1; P < 0.001; Supplementary Table 2).

Figure 3. Sex difference in tail temperature response to food reward with different magnitudes.The figure shows spline-fitted lines and 95% confidence interval (grey bands) of the maximum tail temperature responses of rats to either no reward (neutral), one Cheerio or three Cheerios (n = 6 for each response curve except female neutral and female 3-Cheerios groups have n = 5). The thermal response of the tail shown in the graph was of the rats being exposed to the treatment until 30 min post-treatment. The baseline temperature (dashed line) was calculated from three measurements every 10 s of the 30 s baseline filming immediately before treatment exposure. The yellow bands represent the range in reward consumption.

The tail temperature difference from baseline over time differed between the three treatments in both male and female rats (male, treatment-by-time: 2∆LL = 25.55, df = 2; P < 0.001, treatment-by-time2: 2∆LL = 20.23, df = 2; P < 0.001, female, treatment-by-time: 2∆LL = 18.11, df = 2; P < 0.001, treatment-by-time2: 2∆LL = 17.29, df = 2; P < 0.001; Supplementary Table 2). The tail cooled the most, started to recover later and remained below the baseline during the 30 min in males in the neutral group compared to the rewarded males. Tail temperature, however, changed similarly over time in the two rewarded groups, initially cooling and later overshooting the baseline, although the tail temperature of male rats rewarded with three Cheerios remained above baseline for longer than in males rewarded with one Cheerio (Figure 3). In females, the tail cooled initially and returned towards baseline but did not overshot above baseline during the 30 min of recording except the 3-Cheerios rewarded group (Figure 3). A separate GLMMs of only the food reward groups showed no significant difference in the tail thermal response between rats rewarded with one or three Cheerios (LRT, 2∆LL = 1.18, df = 1; P = 0.278) but between sexes where females showed greater decrease of tail temperature after exposure to rewards and lower recovery than males (LRT, 2∆LL = 31.42, df = 2; P < 0.001; Figure 3). Since tail temperature response over time differed between sexes in all models, each sex was then analysed separately with GLMMs (Supplementary Table 2). Furthermore, the tail temperature response in females differed between postures (Supplementary Table 2) where the tail temperature was cooler when female rats were inactive compared to when walking (LSM; P = 0.008) and eating (LSM; P = 0.041), but there were no significant differences or trends with other postures. The tail temperature response was not related to body mass, location, air temperature, humidity and time of day (see Supplementary Table 2).

To further investigate how the tail temperature response differed between the treatments and sexes, five specific components of the change in tail temperature (Figure 2[b]) were identified for each individual rat and analysed separately (Figure 4). Due to the non-significant treatment by sex interaction (Table 1), the specific curve components of the change in tail temperature were not analysed for each sex separately. Most of the curve components of the tail thermal response did not differ between treatments and between sexes (Table 1). Only the rate of recovery (Mrecov) differed between the three treatments (Table 1). Post hoc analysis showed that rats exposed to three Cheerios recovered faster than rats in the neutral groups (LSM; P =0.033) and a trend that rats exposed to three Cheerios recovered faster than rats exposed to one Cheerio (LSM; P = 0.054). Only the time to reach the lowest tail temperature (Sdrop) differed between the two sexes (Table 1), where males took more time to reach the lowest drop of tail temperature than females. None of the specific components of the change in tail temperature were related to air temperature, humidity and time of day.

Figure 4. Boxplots display the distribution of the amplitudes and the dynamics of specific properties of the tail temperature response to 0 (neutral), 1 and 3 Cheerios according to sex. The median for each dataset (n = 6 per box except female neutral and female 3-Cheerios groups have n = 5) is indicated by the black centre line, and the lower and upper hinges of the box are the inter-quartile range (IQR). The extreme values (within 1.5 times the IQR from the upper or lower quartile) are the ends of the lines extending from the IQR. Outliers are represented as filled circles outside the whiskers and whiskers are the standard deviations. The specific thermal response properties plotted are the amplitude of the drop of the temperature (Adrop: a) and the rise of the temperature (Arecover: b) and the time taken to reach Adrop (Sdrop: c) and the time taken to reach Arecover (Srecover: d) (s). The rate of change of temperature from Adrop to Arecover was represented by the slope (Mrecover: e).

Table 1. GLMM analysis of the amplitudes and the dynamics of specific properties of the tail temperature response to either no Cheerios, one Cheerio or three Cheerios (n = 35). The table shows the fixed effects included in the models. Individual rat identification is the random effect also included in the models but is not shown. The significant P-values of using log likelihood ratio tests are shown in bold italic font

The specific thermal response curve properties are the amplitude of the drop of the temperature (Adrop) and the rise of the temperature (Arecover) and the time taken to reach Adrop (Sdrop) and the time taken to reach Arecover (Srecover) in seconds. The rate of change of temperature from Adrop to Arecover was represented by the slope (Mrecover).

Behavioural response

The most performed behaviours by rats in this study were ‘Rest’, ‘Stationary’, ‘Wall grab’ and ‘Groom’ while the least performed behaviours were ‘Non-intake’, ‘Freeze’ and ‘Dart’ (Supplementary Figure 2). After behaviours were pooled into four groups (see Materials and methods: Behavioural analysis), the ‘Explorative’ and ‘Resting stationary’ behavioural groups were affected by treatment depending on time block (Table 2). ‘Explorative’ behaviours increased, while ‘Resting stationary’ behaviours decreased with a greater number of Cheerios, however, the difference with the neutral group disappeared over time (Figure 5[b], [d], Table 2). The two sexes behaved differently after exposure to treatments (Table 2): female rats performed more ‘Fear/Anxiety’ and ‘Escape/Mobility’ behaviours than male rats while male rats were exhibiting ‘Resting stationary’ behaviours more than female counterparts (Figure 5), but the sexes did not respond differently to reward. ‘Escape/Mobility’, ‘Fear/Anxiety’ and ‘Elimination’ behaviours decreased over time while changes over time in ‘Resting stationary’ and ‘Explorative’ behaviours differed between treatments (Table 2, Figure 5). The latency to eat ranged from 8–201 s (mean [± SE]: 42.54 [± 44.84]) after the food reward was introduced. Furthermore, longer latency to eat was associated with lower baseline temperature (LRT, 2∆LL = 6.43, df = 1; P = 0.011). PCA analysis was also performed to explore correlation between behaviours and body surface temperature (baseline tail temperature and rate of recovery of the tail temperature response curve; Supplementary Figure 3). Rats that performed more ‘Resting stationary’ behaviour were those with higher baseline tail temperature and later tail cooling while baseline temperature was negatively correlated with ‘Escape/Mobility’ behaviours. The rate of recovery of tail temperature (Mrecover) was negatively correlated with both ‘Explorative’ and ‘Fear/Anxiety’ behavioural groups.

Table 2. GLMM analysis of proportion of scans showing rat behaviours per 10 min during the 30-min filming after exposure to either no Cheerios, one Cheerio or three Cheerios (n = 35). The table shows the fixed effects included in the models. Individual rat identification is the random effect also included in the models but is not shown. The significant P-values of using log likelihood ratio tests are shown in bold italic font

Figure 5. Bar graphs indicating mean (± SE) of proportion of scans showing (a) ‘Escape/Mobility’, (b) ‘Explorative’, (c) ‘Fear/Anxiety’ and (d) ‘Resting stationary’ behaviours and eliminating (defaecating/urinating) behaviour counts per 10 min of rats during the 30-min time-period after treatment (neutral, one Cheerio or three Cheerios) exposure (n = 34).

Discussion

This study demonstrates, for the first time in rats, that the dynamic response of body surface temperature measured with IRT differs between rats with and without food reward and between the sexes. These effects were supported by the concurrent approach-avoidance behavioural response. The tail surface temperature decreased after exposure to food reward. Extracting individual curve properties of the tail temperature response gave insights that the treatment difference was seen in the rate of the recovery (Mrecover). The tail temperature of rats given the larger food reward recovered faster than rats without food reward and tail temperature of male rats took more time to reach the minimum temperature (Sdrop) than in female rats. However, the overall tail temperature response did not differ over time between the small and larger food reward groups.

The first objective of this study was to determine if IRT could indicate positive affective state (valence). We found that IRT could differentiate a rewarding experience from a neutral condition but not between the two magnitudes of food reward. After exposure to a food reward, the tail surface temperature decreased with the same direction of thermal change after an exposure to the neutral treatment as well as negative events such as acute restraint (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023) and foot shock (Vianna & Carrive Reference Vianna and Carrive2005). These findings are in accordance with previous work in non-human primates (Ioannou et al. Reference Ioannou, Chotard and Davila-Ross2015; Kano et al. Reference Kano, Hirata, Deschner, Behringer and Call2016; Chotard et al. Reference Chotard, Ioannou and Davila-Ross2018; Heintz et al. Reference Heintz, Fuller and Allard2019), cows (Proctor & Carder Reference Proctor and Carder2015, Reference Proctor and Carder2016) and hens (Moe et al. Reference Moe, Stubsjøen, Bohlin, Flø and Bakken2012) that positive emotional state may have the same effect on the peripheral temperatures of mammals as a negative state does. Our results suggest that the arrival and subsequent consumption of food reward elicited a positive, moderate arousal state such as excitement or reward anticipation (Gygax et al. Reference Gygax, Reefmann, Wolf and Langbein2013) as opposed to stimulus novelty since it has been ruled out by two exposures of these same rewards in the home cage (see Habituation phase). Similar effects have been found in chickens where the comb temperature drops in response to the conditioned positive anticipation and delivery of a favoured food (Moe et al. Reference Moe, Stubsjøen, Bohlin, Flø and Bakken2012). These findings suggest that the SIH phenomenon of the sympathetic activation may be influenced by both positive and negative emotional states. The positive emotion induced by feeding, however, would have involved parasympathetic co-activation and would have facilitated the recovery of the thermal response in this study. Whether a relation between emotion and the organisation of ANS activity exists is not well known and needs to be investigated with future works employing several ANS parameters including cardiovascular, electrodermal and respiratory measures. In our previous IRT study (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023), we found a slight increase in eye temperature after restraint, and after food reward in this study, but the results were not correlated with tail temperature response, corticosterone level and behaviours. Furthermore, the asymmetrical response of nostril temperature was reported to potentially reflect valence of emotions in dogs (Telkänranta Reference Telkänranta2016), according to the emotional lateralisation theory (Leliveld et al. Reference Leliveld, Langbein and Puppe2013). Although one more possibility for assessing valence using body surface temperature could lie in the lateralised temperature response due to emotional cerebral and behavioural lateralisation (Rogers Reference Rogers2010; Leliveld et al. Reference Leliveld, Langbein and Puppe2013, Goursot et al. Reference Goursot, Düpjan, Puppe and Leliveld2021), we did not find valence clues from changes in eye temperature in our previous stress study in rats.

The second objective of this study was to assess whether body surface temperature measured with IRT of male and female rats can also quantify increasing levels of reward magnitude (arousal). IRT was reported in rats (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023) and in hens (Herborn et al. Reference Herborn, Graves, Jerem, Evans, Nager, McCafferty and McKeegan2015) to allow quantification of stress from mild and brief restraints of different magnitude, however, we were unable to quantify positive affect from one and three food reward items used in this study. Possibly, the rewards used to induce positive affective states in this study were not sufficiently different in magnitude to be detectable by IRT. The degree of temperature change will depend on sympathetic nervous activation which may even lead to an activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and an increase in plasma corticosterone levels (Lowe et al. Reference Lowe, Cook, Ingram and Harris2005; Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, Webster, Schaefer, Cook and Scott2005; Ouyang et al. Reference Ouyang, Macaballug, Chen, Hodach, Tang and Francis2021). The reward magnitude difference in this study was based on a previous work measuring running speed in rats using a forced-choice maze paradigm where three Cheerios clumped horizontally together would induce a greater positive affect due to greater density and surface area (Wadhera et al. Reference Wadhera, Wilkie and Capaldi-Phillips2018). At the very least, the larger reward increased the duration of the positive event during imaging. This also raises questions regarding the triggers for SIH, which is often considered to be associated only with negative states and high arousal (Bouwknecht et al. Reference Bouwknecht, Olivier and Paylor2007; Kuraoka & Nakamura Reference Kuraoka and Nakamura2011). In addition, in laboratory rodents, most measures are reported to capture the extremes of the scale from negative to positive valence and there is a lack of established welfare measures for the range between neutral and positive valence (Jirkof et al. Reference Jirkof, Rudeck and Lewejohann2019). This should be investigated further using more pleasant stimuli or spontaneous positive events (e.g., play) with a greater difference in magnitude. Transparent, infra-red materials and add-on equipment have been developed recently and can be made into a temporary lid or attached to a home cage to create an infra-red inspection window (e.g., IR Material Window, Edmund Optics Inc, Barrington, USA; FLIR IR WINDOWS or FLIR IRW-XPC/XPS 2020© Teledyne FLIR, OR, USA). This may therefore allow simultaneous recording of surface temperature with other physiological and behavioural measurements in a home cage that may be useful for examining dynamics of positive affective states in future studies.

As we avoided measuring physiological welfare markers, especially invasive procedures which would confound the results, we only have the concurrent behavioural responses to validate our thermal responses. However, our behavioural analysis showed that, unlike behavioural response to acute stress in previous studies (Gregus et al. Reference Gregus, Wintink, Davis and Kalynchuk2005; Brenes et al. Reference Brenes, Padilla and Fornaguera2009; Wright et al. Reference Wright, Muir and Perrot2013; Jaisinghani & Rosenkranz Reference Jaisinghani and Rosenkranz2015; Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023), rats in this study performed mostly resting and stationary (not freezing/immobility) behaviours and exhibited substantially fewer anxiety/fear behaviours such as freezing and darting (Rodgers Reference Rodgers, Cao, Dalvi and Holmes1997; Vianna & Carrive Reference Vianna and Carrive2005; Barnum et al. Reference Barnum, Blandino and Deak2007; Nikaido & Nakashima Reference Nikaido and Nakashima2009; Brenes et al. Reference Brenes, Padilla and Fornaguera2009; Barker et al. Reference Barker, Sanabria, Lasswell, Thrailkill, Pawlak and Killeen2010; Seffer et al. Reference Seffer, Schwarting and Wöhr2014; Gruene et al. Reference Gruene, Flick, Stefano, Shea and Shansky2015; Magara et al. Reference Magara, Holst, Lundberg, Roman and Lindskog2015; Le Moëne & Ågmo Reference Le Moëne and Ågmo2018) (Supplementary Figure 2). This suggests that our experiment was not as stressful for the rats compared to other previous stress studies. Observations of behaviour indicating fear/anxiety still existed, even minimally, in this study of positive stimuli potentially because of the use of the open-field-like test arena for thermal image recording, although rats have gone through the habituation protocol to minimise effects of isolation in the test arena, the experiment lasting 30 min in the test arena may have become an ongoing mild stressor after the food reward was eaten.

Nevertheless, we found a food reward effect in the behavioural response, supporting the different thermal responses between rewarded and non-rewarded rats. The most performed resting and explorative behaviours differed between reward treatments depending on time-point. Explorative behaviours increased while resting stationary behaviours decreased with a greater number of food rewards, suggesting rat anticipation after food rewards exposure. Some rats were also seen looking for more rewards after they finished eating (CW personal observation 2020). Anticipation reflects the activation of the reward system and was one of the most documented behavioural measures for positive affective states which rats can express alone, as opposed to play or affiliative behaviours when in groups (Boissy et al. Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007; Makowska & Weary Reference Makowska and Weary2013). However, most studies observing anticipation use trained rats with regular presentation of rewards for rats to learn a condition that rats would expect reward and show anticipating behaviours (Van der Harst & Spruijt Reference Van der Harst and Spruijt2007; Zimmerman et al. Reference Zimmerman, Buijs, Bolhuis and Keeling2011; Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Von Keyserlingk, Lidfors and Weary2020). This study, on the other hand, only presented food reward once in the arena and some rats might not have expected that more food reward would be given. The explorative behaviours increased with the number of Cheerios during the first 10 min, possibly because the rats were less stressed by the arena due to the counter-effect of Cheerios in a dose-dependent manner. In humans, reward signalling in the brain was shown to be attenuated by aversive counter-conditioning (Kaag et al. Reference Kaag, Schluter, Karel, Homberg, van Den Brink, Reneman and van Wingen2016). Therefore, once the positive reward ceased, rats in this study reduced explorative behaviours over time compared to the unrewarded rats and instead rested more. The positive effect of Cheerios could be from the quality (visual appearance) and quantity (more surface area) of zero, one and three Cheerios once the rats saw their reward (Wadhera et al. Reference Wadhera, Wilkie and Capaldi-Phillips2018). The difference of the magnitude, however, was only strong enough to yield significant LSM post hoc comparisons between the neutral and the three-Cheerios group. This indicates that our treatments did not differ enough in reward magnitude, in agreement with the thermal responses.

Behavioural responses were analysed in relation to the thermal results and ‘Resting stationary’ behaviours were associated with higher baseline tail temperature and a more delayed tail cooling while baseline tail temperature was negatively correlated with escape behaviours. These results suggest that a lower baseline reflected a more anxious individual as was shown in small birds that eye surface temperature was also negatively correlated with baseline circulating glucocorticoid levels (Jerem et al. Reference Jerem, Jenni-Eiermann, Herborn, McKeegan, McCafferty and Nager2018). The rate of recovery of the tail temperature was found to be faster in rats that showed less fear/anxiety behaviours and, surprisingly, less explorative behaviours. However, the vectors of these variables covered only a few individual rats (Supplementary Figure 3), making the interpretation of these variables difficult. Furthermore, longer latency to eat was associated with lower baseline tail temperature. In anxious or stressed individuals, latency to approach food in a novel situation is longer due to neophobia or ‘bait shyness’ which is often used as a way of measuring anxiety (Deacon Reference Deacon2011). Therefore, the behavioural responses in this study suggested that rats expressed mostly non-stress behaviours after provision of a food reward and that a lower baseline and a longer latency to eat may be helpful in identifying more anxious individuals. Although most of the variation in the latency to eat was explained by baseline tail temperature of rats, different subjective experience of the prior exposures to food reward (two occasions in the habituation phase) between rats within the same cage could also contribute to some of the variation (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Batsche and Batsche1972; Galtress & Kirkpatrick Reference Galtress and Kirkpatrick2010). ‘Elimination’ (i.e. defaecation and urination) is a proxy measure of fear/anxiety which should be positively correlated with anxiety level and negatively correlated with locomotor activity (Wallace & Rosen Reference Wallace and Rosen2000; Brenes et al. Reference Brenes, Padilla and Fornaguera2009; Bowen et al. Reference Bowen, Keats, Kendig, Cakic, Callaghan and McGregor2012; Wright et al. Reference Wright, Muir and Perrot2013), could only be seen when rats were moving away from their faeces and urine or stood on their rear legs and was positively correlated with ‘Escape/Mobility’ behaviours, indicating that ‘Eliminate’ counts therefore effectively measured movement. The time effect could then also be explained that rats overall moved more initially after exposure to treatment and then reduced movement over time.

As in a previous stress experiment in rats (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023), female rats responded with a more prolonged duration than male rats. However, female rats given food rewards recovered better than female rats in the neutral group and after 1-min restraint (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023), suggesting that these Cheerios may be more rewarding to females than to males. Previous research has also suggested stronger preference for sweet reward in female compared to male rats (Valenstein et al. Reference Valenstein, Kakolewski and Cox1967; Sclafani et al. Reference Sclafani, Hertwig, Vigorito and Feigin1987) and that adult females exhibited greater initial consumption rates and stronger magnitude of neural responsiveness to high sugar food reward than adult males (Marshall et al. Reference Marshall, Liu, Murphy, Maidment and Ostlund2017; Sinclair et al. Reference Sinclair, Hildebrandt, Culbert, Klump and Sisk2017). Sex differences in opioid and dopaminergic signalling and autonomic nervous control of the cardiovascular system both prior to and during food intake may contribute to the enhanced responses to food reward in females (Gruene et al. Reference Gruene, Flick, Stefano, Shea and Shansky2015; Sinclair et al. Reference Sinclair, Hildebrandt, Culbert, Klump and Sisk2017). The more rats performed resting behaviours, the higher the baseline tail temperature. On the other hand, the lower the baseline temperature, the more escaping behaviours and longer latencies to eat were seen. The sex difference in tail baseline surface temperature also suggested that female rats were more anxious than male rats, with the SIH process already having started with tail cooling apparent even before testing while the eye temperature which was related to the core temperature (Kessel et al. Reference Kessel, Johnson, Arvidsson and Larsen2010) remained similar to the males (eye results will be reported elsewhere). Sex differences in the behavioural response was also found in this as well as in previous studies (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Lin, DeVries and Lambert2003; Dalla et al. Reference Dalla, Pitychoutis, Kokras and Papadopoulou-Daifoti2011; Colom-Lapetina et al. Reference Colom-Lapetina, Li, Pelegrina-Perez and Shansky2019; Knight et al. Reference Knight, Chellian, Wilson, Behnood-Rod, Panunzio and Bruijnzeel2021). Fear/anxiety-related behaviours were performed more by female than male rats whereas resting/stationary behaviours were performed more in male than female counterparts, but this was not affected by the treatments. The lack of sex-by-time interaction in this study could suggest that the sexes differed in their behavioural responses depending on the valence of stimuli driven by a fundamental difference in stress/reward processing (Mashoodh et al. Reference Mashoodh, Wright, Hébert and Perrot-Sinal2008; Marshall et al. Reference Marshall, Liu, Murphy, Maidment and Ostlund2017; Chowdhury et al. Reference Chowdhury, Wallin-Miller, Rear, Park, Diaz, Simon and Moghaddam2019). The main sex effect shown in this study, however, mainly reflected the different strategies to cope with the testing arena between the sexes reported in previous studies (Keating Reference Keating2010; Gruene et al. Reference Gruene, Flick, Stefano, Shea and Shansky2015; Bangasser & Wicks Reference Bangasser and Wicks2017; Le Moëne & Ågmo Reference Le Moëne and Ågmo2018). Several theories have been proposed to explain the cause of sex differences (Tamres et al. Reference Tamres, Janicki and Helgeson2002; Luine & Dohanich Reference Luine and Dohanich2008; Bangasser & Wicks Reference Bangasser and Wicks2017). For example, in the natural selection theory, different ethological demands and biological support of evolutionary explanations render that females may be more likely to survive a threat if they are active and able to detect and escape it, while males may benefit more by conserving energy and using more passive strategies (Jonasson Reference Jonasson2005; Colom-Lapetina et al. Reference Colom-Lapetina, Li, Pelegrina-Perez and Shansky2019).

The potential confounding factors in the present study were any physical activities that trigger the sympathetic nervous activation such as walking (Pavlidis et al. Reference Pavlidis, Levine and Baukol2000), food consumption (van Baak Reference van Baak2008; Ioannou et al. Reference Ioannou, Chotard and Davila-Ross2015) as well as the environmental temperature (Fernández-Cuevas et al. Reference Fernández-Cuevas, Bouzas Marins, Arnáiz Lastras, Gómez Carmona, Piñonosa Cano, García-Concepción and Sillero-Quintana2015; Tattersall Reference Tattersall2016; Nord & Folkow Reference Nord and Folkow2019), humidity (Fernández-Cuevas et al. Reference Fernández-Cuevas, Bouzas Marins, Arnáiz Lastras, Gómez Carmona, Piñonosa Cano, García-Concepción and Sillero-Quintana2015; Tattersall Reference Tattersall2016) and time of day (Koch et al. Reference Koch, Leinweber, Drengberg, Blaum and Oster2017; Oka Reference Oka2018) that the test was conducted. These were all included in analysis and only posture was found to affect body surface temperature. The tail temperature was cooler when female rats were inactive compared to when eating. Increased metabolism during eating produces extra heat and could be an explanation for this finding (van Baak Reference van Baak2008; Ioannou et al. Reference Ioannou, Chotard and Davila-Ross2015).

Animal welfare implications

Advancing our knowledge of positive welfare state in non-human animals is not only important for improving the welfare of animals but that of human caregivers and the quality of animal research. The thermal curve component analysis of the tail has revealed the ability of IRT to non-invasively compare valence between neutral and positive state relatively in rats of both sexes, but the ability of IRT to quantify positive states induced by different reward magnitudes was not supported. This could potentially be due to the rewards used in this study not differing sufficiently in their magnitude. Although tail temperature might not be able to reveal valence in discrete emotion terms due to the overall shape and direction of the tail thermal curve response being very similar during both negative and positive experiences, it can tell which event is relatively more positive than another by showing a higher rate of recovery. This could then be used to assess the continuum between negative and positive affective states that is part of the dynamic welfare of an animal. The validation of a surface temperature approach to assess, not only negative, but also positive events and their magnitude gives the possibility to provide a non-invasive, real-time, continuous means by which to assess dynamic welfare, contributing to refinement of research (3Rs: replacement, reduction, refinement). Furthermore, if home cage thermal imaging can be undertaken, this approach could also be used in rodent husbandry to monitor welfare throughout life. In addition, rats in this study appeared to recover better when receiving a food reward at the start of the procedure. The food reward could be used more often before or after procedures to encourage participation and recovery.

Conclusion

This study aimed to explore the use of IRT to measure the two dimensions of affective state (valence and arousal) to identify and quantify reward-induced positive affective state. The overall shape and direction of the tail temperature response curve cannot differentiate between valences as tail temperature dropped after exposure to neutral situation and rewards as it did when exposed to negative experiences (Wongsaengchan et al. Reference Wongsaengchan, McCafferty, Evans, McKeegan and Nager2023). However, the temperature response curve dynamic was still able to differentiate between neutral and reward-induced positive states; a higher rate of recovery (Mrecov) was found in rewarded rats as compared to unrewarded rats but was unable to quantify differences in reward magnitudes. Behavioural responses supported these thermal responses as explorative behaviours increased in the rewarded group compared to the neutral group in both sexes but did not differ between reward magnitudes. Sex differences were found in both thermal and behavioural measures, emphasising the need to consider both sexes in welfare research. To further explore the positive effects of reward and its magnitude on thermal and behavioural responses, we recommend firstly, to use stronger rewards which differ largely in their magnitude and secondly, to use non-invasive standardised tests avoiding confounding negative stimulation by thermal imaging in home cages using a permeable window to infra-red radiation. Future work could also explore the effects of sex, oestrus cycle, genetic strain, light/dark-phase, and age on surface temperature responses to negative and positive stimuli as well as to expand IRT applications to other endodermic species both in captive and wild contexts. This study contributes to our understanding of the dynamics of positive emotions induced by food reward and their effects on peripheral body temperature and behaviour.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2023.87.

Acknowledgements

The infra-red thermography equipment and software were funded by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council, UK, grant number BB/K002775/1. The authors would like to thank staff at the animal unit for their great help with animal husbandry and three anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments.

Data availability

Data are available at Mendeley Data: https://doi.org/10.17632/c6bm6ywnmv.1.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: CW, RN, DMcK, DMcC; Data curation: CW, DMcC, KL; Formal analysis: CW, RN; Funding acquisition: CW, DMcK; Investigation: CW, RN; Methodology: CW, RN, DMcK, DMcC; Project administration: CW, RN, DMcK, DMcC; Resources: CW, RN, DMcK, DMcC; Software: DMcC, RN, DMcK; Supervision: RN, DMcK, DMcC; Validation: RN, DMcK, DMcC; Visualisation: CW, RN, DMcK, DMcC; Writing – original draft: CW; Writing – review & editing: CW, RN, DMcK, DMcC.

References

Alexander, R, Aragón, OR, Bookwala, J, Cherbuin, N, Gatt, JM, Kahrilas, IJ, Kästner, N, Lawrence, A, Lowe, L, Morrison, RG, Mueller, SC, Nusslock, R, Papadelis, C, Polnaszek, KL, Helene Richter, S, Silton, RL and Styliadis, C 2021 The neuroscience of positive emotions and affect: Implications for cultivating happiness and wellbeing. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 121: 220249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, C, Von Keyserlingk, MAG, Lidfors, LM and Weary, DM 2020 Anticipatory behaviour in animals: A critical review. Animal Welfare 29: 231238. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.29.3.231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arndt, SS, Goerlich, VC and van der Staay, FJ 2022 A dynamic concept of animal welfare: The role of appetitive and adverse internal and external factors and the animal’s ability to adapt to them. Frontiers in Animal Science 3: 121. https://doi.org/10.3389/fanim.2022.908513CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bangasser, DA and Wicks, B 2017 Sex-specific mechanisms for responding to stress. Journal of Neuroscience Research 95: 7582. https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.23812CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, DJ, Sanabria, F, Lasswell, A, Thrailkill, EA, Pawlak, AP and Killeen, PR 2010 Brief light as a practical aversive stimulus for the albino rat. Behavioural Brain Research 214: 402408. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.06.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnum, CJ, Blandino, P and Deak, T 2007 Adaptation in the corticosterone and hyperthermic responses to stress following repeated stressor exposure. Journal of Neuroendocrinology 19: 632642. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2826.2007.01571.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boissy, A, Manteuffel, G, Jensen, MB, Moe, RO, Spruijt, B, Keeling, LJ, Winckler, C, Forkman, B, Dimitrov, I, Langbein, J, Bakken, M, Veissier, I and Aubert, A 2007 Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiology & Behavior 92: 375397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouwknecht, AJ, Olivier, B and Paylor, RE 2007 The stress-induced hyperthermia paradigm as a physiological animal model for anxiety: A review of pharmacological and genetic studies in the mouse. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 31: 4159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowen, MT, Keats, K, Kendig, MD, Cakic, V, Callaghan, PD and McGregor, IS 2012 Aggregation in quads but not pairs of rats exposed to cat odor or bright light. Behavioural Processes 90: 331336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2012.03.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brenes, JC, Padilla, M and Fornaguera, J 2009 A detailed analysis of open-field habituation and behavioral and neurochemical antidepressant-like effects in postweaning enriched rats. Behavioural Brain Research 197: 125137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broom, DM and Johnson, KG 1993 Stress and Animal Welfare. Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brydges, NM and Hall, L 2017 A shortened protocol for assessing cognitive bias in rats. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 286: 15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.05.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgdorf, J and Panksepp, J 2006 The neurobiology of positive emotions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 173187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.06.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burn, CC 2008 What is it like to be a rat? Rat sensory perception and its implications for experimental design and rat welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 112: 132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2008.02.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buynitsky, T and Mostofsky, DI 2009 Restraint stress in biobehavioral research: Recent developments. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33: 10891098. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.05.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, PE, Batsche, CJ and Batsche, GM 1972 Spaced-trials reward magnitude effects in the rat: Single versus multiple food pellets. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 81: 360364. https://doi.org/10.1037/H0033530CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, T, Lin, S, DeVries, C and Lambert, K 2003 Coping strategies in male and female rats exposed to multiple stressors. Physiology and Behavior 78: 495504. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00033-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campos, AC, Fogaça, MV, Aguiar, DC and Guimarães, FS 2013 Animal models of anxiety disorders and stress. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria 35: 101111. https://doi.org/10.1590/1516-4446-2013-1139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chotard, H, Ioannou, S and Davila-Ross, M 2018 Infrared thermal imaging: Positive and negative emotions modify the skin temperatures of monkey and ape faces. American Journal of Primatology 80: e22863. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22863CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chowdhury, TG, Wallin-Miller, KG, Rear, AA, Park, J, Diaz, V, Simon, NW and Moghaddam, B 2019 Sex differences in reward- and punishment-guided actions. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 19: 14041417. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00736-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colom-Lapetina, J, Li, AJ, Pelegrina-Perez, TC and Shansky, RM 2019 Behavioral diversity across classic rodent models is sex-dependent. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 13: 45. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalla, C, Pitychoutis, PM, Kokras, N and Papadopoulou-Daifoti, Z 2011 Sex differences in response to stress and expression of depressive-like behaviours in the rat. Biological basis of sex differences in psychopharmacology pp 97118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, RMJ 2011 Hyponeophagia: A measure of anxiety in the mouse. Journal of Visualized Experiments: JoVE: 2613. https://doi.org/10.3791/2613CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diorio, D, Viau, V and Meaney, MJ 1993 The role of the medial prefrontal cortex (cingulate gyrus) in the regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Journal of Neuroscience 13: 38393847. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.13-09-03839.1993CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duarte, J and Pinto-Gouveia, J 2017 Positive affect and parasympathetic activity: Evidence for a quadratic relationship between feeling safe and content and heart rate variability. Psychiatry Research 257: 284289. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYCHRES.2017.07.077CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edgar, JL, Nicol, CJ, Pugh, CA and Paul, ES 2013 Surface temperature changes in response to handling in domestic chickens. Physiology and Behavior 119: 195200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.06.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farm Animal Welfare Council (FAWC) 2009 Farm Animal Welfare in Great Britain. FAWC: London, UK.Google Scholar
Fassini, A, Scopinho, AA, Alves, FHF, Fortaleza, EAT and Corrêa, FMA 2017 The medial preoptic area modulates autonomic function under resting and stress conditions. Neuroscience 364: 164174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.09.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fassini, A, Scopinho, AA, Resstel, LBM and Correa, FMA 2014 Opioid receptors in the prelimbic cortex modulate restraint stress-induced cardiovascular responses in the rat. Neuropharmacology 85: 367374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.04.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Cuevas, I, Bouzas Marins, JC, Arnáiz Lastras, J, Gómez Carmona, PM, Piñonosa Cano, S, García-Concepción, and Sillero-Quintana, M 2015 Classification of factors influencing the use of infrared thermography in humans: A review. Infrared Physics and Technology 71: 2855. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infrared.2015.02.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finlayson, K, Lampe, JF, Hintze, S, Würbel, H and Melotti, L 2016 Facial indicators of positive emotions in rats. PLoS ONE 11: e0166446. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0166446CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galtress, T and Kirkpatrick, K 2010 Reward magnitude effects on temporal discrimination. Learning and Motivation 41: 108124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2010.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gemmell, RT and Hales, JR 1977 Cutaneous arteriovenous anastomoses present in the tail but absent from the ear of the rat. Journal of Anatomy 124: 355358.Google ScholarPubMed
Gjendal, K, Franco, NH, Ottesen, JL, Sørensen, DB and Olsson, IAS 2018 Eye, body or tail? Thermography as a measure of stress in mice. Physiology and Behavior 196: 135143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.08.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goursot, C, Düpjan, S, Puppe, B and Leliveld, LMC 2021 Affective styles and emotional lateralization: A promising framework for animal welfare research. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 237: 105279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregus, A, Wintink, AJ, Davis, AC and Kalynchuk, LE 2005 Effect of repeated corticosterone injections and restraint stress on anxiety and depression-like behavior in male rats. Behavioural Brain Research 156: 105114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2004.05.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruene, TM, Flick, K, Stefano, A, Shea, SD and Shansky, RM 2015 Sexually divergent expression of active and passive conditioned fear responses in rats. eLife 4. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11352.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gygax, L, Reefmann, N, Wolf, M and Langbein, J 2013 Prefrontal cortex activity, sympatho-vagal reaction and behaviour distinguish between situations of feed reward and frustration in dwarf goats. Behavioural Brain Research 239: 104114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.052CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, AC, Whitehead, RD, Albrecht, M, Lefevre, CE and Perrett, DI 2012 Hot or not? Thermal reactions to social contact. Biology Letters 8: 864867. https://doi.org/10.1098/RSBL.2012.0338CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hänsel, A and von Känel, R 2008 The ventro-medial prefrontal cortex: a major link between the autonomic nervous system, regulation of emotion, and stress reactivity? BioPsychoSocial Medicine 2: 21. https://doi.org/10.1186/1751-0759-2-21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, P, Morton, DB, Burman, O, Dennison, N, Honess, P, Jennings, M, Lane, S, Middleton, V, Roughan, JV, Wells, S and Westwood, K 2011 A guide to defining and implementing protocols for the welfare assessment of laboratory animals: Eleventh report of the BVAAWF/FRAME/RSPCA/UFAW Joint Working Group on Refinement. Laboratory Animals 45: 113. https://doi.org/10.1258/la.2010.010031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heintz, MR, Fuller, G and Allard, S 2019 Exploratory investigation of infrared thermography for measuring gorilla emotional responses to interactions with familiar humans. Animals 9: 604. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9090604CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Held, SDE and Špinka, M 2011 Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour 81: 891899. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ANBEHAV.2011.01.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herborn, KA, Graves, JL, Jerem, P, Evans, NP, Nager, R, McCafferty, DJ and McKeegan, DEF 2015 Skin temperature reveals the intensity of acute stress. Physiology and Behavior 152: 225230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.09.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinchcliffe, JK, Mendl, M and Robinson, ESJ 2020 Rat 50 kHz calls reflect graded tickling-induced positive emotion. Current Biology 30: R1034R1035. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.08.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubrecht, R and Kirkwood, J 2010 The UFAW Handbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory and Other Research Animals: Eighth Edition. Wiley: London, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ioannou, S, Chotard, H and Davila-Ross, M 2015 No strings attached: physiological monitoring of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) with thermal imaging. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9: 110. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ioannou, S, Gallese, V and Merla, A 2014 Thermal infrared imaging in psychophysiology: Potentialities and limits. Psychophysiology 51: 951963. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12243CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ishii, Y and Shinya, Y 2021 Positive emotions have different impacts on mood and sympathetic changes in crying from negative emotions. Motivation and Emotion 45: 530542. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-021-09887-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaisinghani, S and Rosenkranz, JA 2015 Repeated social defeat stress enhances the anxiogenic effect of bright light on operant reward-seeking behavior in rats. Behavioural Brain Research 290: 172179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jerem, P, Jenni-Eiermann, S, Herborn, K, McKeegan, D, McCafferty, DJ and Nager, RG 2018 Eye region surface temperature reflects both energy reserves and circulating glucocorticoids in a wild bird. Scientific Reports 8: 110. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-20240-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jirkof, P, Rudeck, J and Lewejohann, L 2019 Assessing affective state in laboratory rodents to promote animal welfare—What is the progress in applied refinement research? Animals 9: 1026. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9121026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonasson, Z 2005 Meta-analysis of sex differences in rodent models of learning and memory: a review of behavioral and biological data. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 28: 811825. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.10.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaag, AM, Schluter, RS, Karel, P, Homberg, J, van Den Brink, W, Reneman, L and van Wingen, GA 2016 Aversive counterconditioning attenuates reward signaling in the ventral striatum. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10: 418. https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2016.00418/BIBTEXCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kano, F, Hirata, S, Deschner, T, Behringer, V and Call, J 2016 Nasal temperature drop in response to a playback of conspecific fights in chimpanzees: A thermo-imaging study. Physiology & Behavior 155: 8394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keating, C 2010 Sex differences precipitating anorexia nervosa in females: The estrogen paradox and a novel framework for targeting sex-specific neurocircuits and behavior. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences pp 189207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessel, L, Johnson, L, Arvidsson, H and Larsen, M 2010 The relationship between body and ambient temperature and corneal temperature. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 51: 65936597. https://doi.org/10.1167/IOVS.10-5659CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knight, P, Chellian, R, Wilson, R, Behnood-Rod, A, Panunzio, S and Bruijnzeel, AW 2021 Sex differences in the elevated plus-maze test and large open field test in adult Wistar rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 204: 173168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2021.173168CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koch, CE, Leinweber, B, Drengberg, BC, Blaum, C and Oster, H 2017 Interaction between circadian rhythms and stress. Neurobiology of Stress 6: 5767. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.09.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreibig, SD 2010 Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology 84: 394421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuraoka, K and Nakamura, K 2011 The use of nasal skin temperature measurements in studying emotion in macaque monkeys. Physiology & Behavior 102: 347355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.11.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsen, JT, Berntson, GG, Poehlmann, KM, Ito, TA and Cacioppo, JT 2008 The Psychophysiology of Emotion. The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA.Google Scholar
Lawrence, AB, Vigors, B and Sandøe, P 2019 What is so positive about positive animal welfare?—A critical review of the literature. Animals 9: 783. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9100783CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, S, Josse, J and Husson, F 2008 FactoMineR: An R Package for Multivariate Analysis. Journal of Statistical Software 25. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v025.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lecorps, B, Rödel, HG and Féron, C 2016 Assessment of anxiety in open field and elevated plus maze using infrared thermography. Physiology and Behavior 157: 209216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.02.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leliveld, LMC, Langbein, J and Puppe, B 2013 The emergence of emotional lateralization: Evidence in non-human vertebrates and implications for farm animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 145: 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenth, R V 2016 Least-squares means: the R package lsmeans. Journal of Statistical Software 69: 133. http://doi:10.18637/jss.v069.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowe, TE, Cook, CJ, Ingram, JR and Harris, PJ 2005 Changes in ear-pinna temperature as a useful measure of stress in sheep (Ovis aries). Animal Welfare 14: 3542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luine, VN and Dohanich, G 2008 Sex Differences in the Brain: From Genes to Behavior pp 227252. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Magara, S, Holst, S, Lundberg, S, Roman, E and Lindskog, M 2015 Altered explorative strategies and reactive coping style in the FSL rat model of depression. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9: 89. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Makowska, IJ 2016 Understanding the welfare of rats living in standard versus semi-naturalistic laboratory environments. Doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia, Canada.Google Scholar
Makowska, IJ and Weary, DM 2013 Assessing the emotions of laboratory rats. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 148: 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.07.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marks, A, Vianna, DML and Carrive, P 2009 Nonshivering thermogenesis without interscapular brown adipose tissue involvement during conditioned fear in the rat. AJP: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 296: R1239R1247. https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpregu.90723.2008Google ScholarPubMed
Marshall, AT, Liu, AT, Murphy, NP, Maidment, NT and Ostlund, SB 2017 Sex-specific enhancement of palatability-driven feeding in adolescent rats. PLOS One 12: e0180907. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180907CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, P and Bateson, P 2007 Recording Methods. Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mashoodh, R, Wright, LD, Hébert, K and Perrot-Sinal, TS 2008 Investigation of sex differences in behavioural, endocrine, and neural measures following repeated psychological stressor exposure. Behavioural Brain Research 188: 368379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.11.018Google ScholarPubMed
Mauss, IB and Robinson, MD 2009 Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition & Emotion 23: 209237. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802204677CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCafferty, DJ 2007 The value of infrared thermography for research on mammals: Previous applications and future directions. Mammal Review 37: 207223. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2907.2007.00111.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMillan, FD 2019 Health and Well-Being in Animals. CABI Publishing: Boston, USA.Google Scholar
Mellor, DJ 2015 Positive animal welfare states and reference standards for welfare assessment. New Zealand Veterinary Journal 63: 1723. https://doi.org/10.1080/00480169.2014.926802CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellor, DJ 2016 Updating animal welfare thinking: Moving beyond the “Five Freedoms” towards “A Life Worth Living.” Animals: an Open Access Journal from MDPI 6: 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/ANI6030021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendl, M, Burman, OHP, Parker, RMA and Paul, ES 2009 Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 118: 161181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merla, A and Romani, GL 2007 Thermal signatures of emotional arousal: A functional infrared imaging study. 29th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society pp 247249. IEEE, Lyon, France.Google Scholar
Miyazono, S, Hasegawa, K, Miyazaki, S, Sakakima, H, Konno, S, Meguro, S, Sasajima, H, Noguchi, T, Osada, K and Kashiwayanagi, M 2018 Etizolam attenuates the reduction in cutaneous temperature induced in mice by exposure to synthetic predator odor. European Journal of Pharmacology 824: 157162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejphar.2018.02.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moe, RO, Bohlin, J, Flø, A, Vasdal, G and Stubsjøen, SM 2017 Hot chicks, cold feet. Physiology and Behavior 179: 4248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.05.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moe, RO, Stubsjøen, SM, Bohlin, J, Flø, A and Bakken, M 2012 Peripheral temperature drop in response to anticipation and consumption of a signaled palatable reward in laying hens (Gallus domesticus). Physiology and Behavior 106: 527533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Moëne, O and Ågmo, A 2018 Behavioral responses to emotional challenges in female rats living in a seminatural environment: The role of estrogen receptors. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2018.10.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Möstl, E and Palme, R 2002 Hormones as indicators of stress. Domestic Animal Endocrinology 23: 6774. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0739-7240(02)00146-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakayama, K, Goto, S, Kuraoka, K and Nakamura, K 2005 Decrease in nasal temperature of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) in negative emotional state. Physiology and Behavior 84: 783790. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.03.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nikaido, Y and Nakashima, T 2009 Effects of environmental novelty on fear-related behavior and stress responses of rats to emotionally relevant odors. Behavioural Brain Research 199: 241246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.11.043CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nip, E, Adcock, A, Nazal, B, MacLellan, A, Niel, L, Choleris, E, Levison, L and Mason, G 2019 Why are enriched mice nice? Investigating how environmental enrichment reduces agonism in female C57BL/6, DBA/2, and BALB/c mice. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 217: 7382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2019.05.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nord, A and Folkow, LP 2019 Ambient temperature effects on stress-induced hyperthermia in Svalbard ptarmigan. https://doi.org/10.1242/bio.043497CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oka, T 2018 Stress-induced hyperthermia and hypothermia. Handbook of Clinical Neurology 157: 599621. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-64074-1.00035-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oka, T, Oka, K and Hori, T 2001 Mechanisms and mediators of psychological stress-induced rise in core temperature. Psychosomatic Medicine 63: 476486. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006842-200105000-00018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olivier, B, Zethof, T, Pattij, T, van Boogaert, M, van Oorschot, R, Leahy, C, Oosting, R, Bouwknecht, A, Veening, J, van der Gugten, J and Groenink, L 2003 Stress-induced hyperthermia and anxiety: pharmacological validation. European Journal of Pharmacology 463: 117132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0014-2999(03)01326-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ouyang, JQ, Macaballug, P, Chen, H, Hodach, K, Tang, S and Francis, JS 2021 Infrared thermography is an effective, noninvasive measure of HPA activation. Stress 24: 584589. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2020.1868431CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, ES, Harding, EJ and Mendl, M 2005 Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 29: 469491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlidis, I, Levine, J and Baukol, P 2000 Thermal imaging for anxiety detection. Proceedings of the IEEE Workshop on Computer Vision Beyond the Visible Spectrum: Methods and Applications pp 104109. 16 June 2000, IEEE Computer Society, Hilton Head, SC, USA.Google Scholar
Percie du Sert, N, Hurst, V, Ahluwalia, A, Alam, S, Avey, MT, Baker, M, Browne, WJ, Clark, A, Cuthill, IC, Dirnagl, U, Emerson, M, Garner, P, Holgate, ST, Howells, DW, Karp, NA, Lazic, SE, Lidster, K, MacCallum, CJ, Macleod, M, Pearl, EJ, Petersen, OH, Rawle, F, Reynolds, P, Rooney, K, Sena, ES, Silberberg, SD, Steckler, T and Würbel, H 2020 The ARRIVE guidelines 2.0: Updated guidelines for reporting animal research. PLOS Biology 18: e3000410. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000410CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinheiro, J, Bates, D, DebRoy, S, Sarkar, D and R Core Team 2018 Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3: 1137. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlmeGoogle Scholar
Proctor, H and Carder, G 2016 Can changes in nasal temperature be used as an indicator of emotional state in cows? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 184: 16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.07.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proctor, HS and Carder, G 2015 Nasal temperatures in dairy cows are influenced by positive emotional state. Physiology and Behavior 138: 340344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team 2019 R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Reis, DG, Scopinho, AA, Guimarães, FS, Corrêa, FMA and Resstel, LBM 2011 Behavioral and autonomic responses to acute restraint stress are segregated within the lateral septal area of rats. PLoS One 6. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023171CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodgers, RJ, Cao, B-J, Dalvi, A and Holmes, A 1997 Animal models of anxiety: an ethological perspective. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research 30: 289304. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X1997000300002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, LJ 2010 Relevance of brain and behavioural lateralization to animal welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 127: 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2010.06.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sclafani, A, Hertwig, H, Vigorito, M and Feigin, MB 1987 Sex differences in polysaccharide and sugar preferences in rats. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 11: 241251. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(87)80032-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seffer, D, Schwarting, RKW and Wöhr, M 2014 Pro-social ultrasonic communication in rats: Insights from playback studies. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 234: 7381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.01.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiota, MN, Neufeld, SL, Yeung, WH, Moser, SE and Perea, EF 2011 Feeling good: Autonomic nervous system responding in five positive emotions. Emotion 11: 13681378. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0024278CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinclair, EB, Hildebrandt, BA, Culbert, KM, Klump, KL and Sisk, CL 2017 Preliminary evidence of sex differences in behavioral and neural responses to palatable food reward in rats. Physiology and Behavior 176: 165173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, AJ, Clutton, RE, Lilley, E, Hansen, KEA and Brattelid, T 2018 PREPARE: guidelines for planning animal research and testing. Laboratory Animals 52: 135141. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023677217724823CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spruijt, BM, Van den Bos, R and Pijlman, FTA 2001 A concept of welfare based on reward evaluating mechanisms in the brain: Anticipatory behaviour as an indicator for the state of reward systems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 72: 145171. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(00)00204-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steimer, T 2011 Animal models of anxiety disorders in rats and mice: Some conceptual issues. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 13: 495506. https://doi.org/10.31887/dcns.2011.13.4/tsteimerCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, M, Webster, J, Schaefer, A, Cook, N and Scott, S 2005 Infrared thermography as a non-invasive tool to study animal welfare. Animal Welfare 14: 319325. https://doi.org/10.1017/S096272860002964XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamres, LK, Janicki, D and Helgeson, VS 2002 Sex differences in coping behavior: A meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Personality and Social Psychology Review 6: 230. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0601_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tattersall, GJ 2016 Infrared thermography: A non-invasive window into thermal physiology. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology 202: 7898. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpa.2016.02.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Telkänranta, H 2016 Infrared thermography as a non-invasive tool for quantifying emotional arousal in rats, dogs and elephants: Recent findings and methodological aspects. University of Helsinki, Finland. https://researchportal.helsinki.fi/en/publications/infrared-thermography-as-a-non-invasive-tool-for-quantifying-emotGoogle Scholar
Travain, T, Colombo, ES, Grandi, LC, Heinzl, E, Pelosi, A, Prato Previde, E and Valsecchi, P 2016 How good is this food? A study on dogs’ emotional responses to a potentially pleasant event using infrared thermography. Physiology and Behavior 159: 8087. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Travain, T, Colombo, ES, Heinzl, E, Bellucci, D, Prato Previde, E and Valsecchi, P 2015 Hot dogs: Thermography in the assessment of stress in dogs (Canis familiaris): A pilot study. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 10: 1723. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2014.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Travain, T and Valsecchi, P 2021 Infrared thermography in the study of animals’ emotional responses: A critical review. Animals 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11092510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valenstein, ES, Kakolewski, JW and Cox, VC 1967 Sex differences in taste preference for glucose and saccharin solutions. Science 156: 942943. https://doi.org/10.1126/SCIENCE.156.3777.942CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Baak, MA 2008 Meal-induced activation of the sympathetic nervous system and its cardiovascular and thermogenic effects in man. Physiology & Behavior 94: 178186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.12.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Harst, JE and Spruijt, BM 2007 Tools to measure and improve animal welfare: Reward-related behaviour. Animal Welfare 16: 6773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vianna, DML and Carrive, P 2005 Changes in cutaneous and body temperature during and after conditioned fear to context in the rat. European Journal of Neuroscience 21: 25052512. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04073.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadhera, D, Wilkie, LM and Capaldi-Phillips, ED 2018 The rewarding effects of number and surface area of food in rats. Learning & Behavior 46: 242255. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-017-0305-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallace, DG, Gorny, B and Whishaw, IQ 2002 Rats can track odors, other rats, and themselves: Implications for the study of spatial behavior. Behavioural Brain Research 131: 185192. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00384-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, KJ and Rosen, JB 2000 Predator odor as an unconditioned fear stimulus in rats: Elicitation of freezing by trimethylthiazoline, a component of fox feces. Behavioral Neuroscience 114: 912922. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.114.5.912CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilhelm, FH, Rattel, JA, Wegerer, M, Liedlgruber, M, Schweighofer, S, Kreibig, SD, Kolodyazhniy, V and Blechert, J 2017 Attend or defend? Sex differences in behavioral, autonomic, and respiratory response patterns to emotion–eliciting films. Biological Psychology 130: 3040. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BIOPSYCHO.2017.10.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wöhr, M and Schwarting, RKW 2009 Ultrasonic communication in rats: Effects of morphine and naloxone on vocal and behavioral responses to playback of 50-kHz vocalizations. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 94: 285295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2009.09.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wongsaengchan, C 2022 Non-invasive welfare assessment in rats using infrared thermography. University of Glasgow, UK.Google Scholar
Wongsaengchan, C, McCafferty, DJ, Evans, NP, McKeegan, DEF and Nager, RG 2023 Body surface temperature of rats reveals both magnitude and sex differences in the acute stress response. Physiology & Behavior 264: 114138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2023.114138CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, LD, Muir, KE and Perrot, TS 2013 Stress responses of adolescent male and female rats exposed repeatedly to cat odor stimuli, and long-term enhancement of adult defensive behaviors. Developmental Psychobiology 55: 551567. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21060CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeates, JW and Main, DCJ 2008 Assessment of positive welfare: A review. Veterinary Journal 175: 293300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2007.05.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmerman, PH, Buijs, SAF, Bolhuis, JE and Keeling, LJ 2011 Behaviour of domestic fowl in anticipation of positive and negative stimuli. Animal Behaviour 81: 569577. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.11.028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zupan, M, Buskas, J, Altimiras, J and Keeling, LJ 2016 Assessing positive emotional states in dogs using heart rate and heart rate variability. Physiology and Behavior 155: 102111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuur, AF, Ieno, EN and Elphick, CS 2010 A protocol for data exploration to avoid common statistical problems. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 1: 314. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210x.2009.00001.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The three phases of the experimental protocol: acclimatisation; habituation; and testing. Rats were left undisturbed for the first week after arrival to acclimatise to the home cage. In the second week, six habituation trials of increasing duration of exposure to the test arena (5, 5, 10, 20, 30, 30-min duration represented as H1–6, respectively) were completed within six consecutive days; trials H1 and H2 were completed in one day and trials H5 and H6 were completed over three days. In the third week, each rat was tested by being put into the test arena and filmed with infra-red and video cameras for 30 s to record the baseline temperature and behaviour. Each rat was then exposed to one of three treatments (0, 1 or 3 Cheerios) and further filmed for 30 min.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Thermal data extraction. (a) Thermal image of a rat in the testing arena viewed in the ‘rain’ (rainbow) palette in ThermaCAM Researcher software. A bendable line (white arrow) was drawn manually to extract the maximum temperature of the whole length of the tail. Left and right eye temperatures (black arrow) were also recorded and will be reported elsewhere. (b) The schematic standardised tail surface temperature response to a stimulus, identifying five distinct curve properties. The amplitude of the initial decrease in individual tail temperature from baseline (Adrop, 1), defined as the minimum value of the temperature difference from baseline (T difference) before the first rise of temperature back towards the baseline, and the amplitude of the maximum recovery (Arecov, 2) was defined as the highest T difference value recorded after Adrop. The time elapsed (s) to reach Adrop was designated as Sdrop (3). The rate of change of temperature from Adrop to Arecov was represented by the slope Mrecov (4). The time elapsed (s) to reach Arecov was designated as Srecov (5).

Figure 2

Figure 3. Sex difference in tail temperature response to food reward with different magnitudes.The figure shows spline-fitted lines and 95% confidence interval (grey bands) of the maximum tail temperature responses of rats to either no reward (neutral), one Cheerio or three Cheerios (n = 6 for each response curve except female neutral and female 3-Cheerios groups have n = 5). The thermal response of the tail shown in the graph was of the rats being exposed to the treatment until 30 min post-treatment. The baseline temperature (dashed line) was calculated from three measurements every 10 s of the 30 s baseline filming immediately before treatment exposure. The yellow bands represent the range in reward consumption.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Boxplots display the distribution of the amplitudes and the dynamics of specific properties of the tail temperature response to 0 (neutral), 1 and 3 Cheerios according to sex. The median for each dataset (n = 6 per box except female neutral and female 3-Cheerios groups have n = 5) is indicated by the black centre line, and the lower and upper hinges of the box are the inter-quartile range (IQR). The extreme values (within 1.5 times the IQR from the upper or lower quartile) are the ends of the lines extending from the IQR. Outliers are represented as filled circles outside the whiskers and whiskers are the standard deviations. The specific thermal response properties plotted are the amplitude of the drop of the temperature (Adrop: a) and the rise of the temperature (Arecover: b) and the time taken to reach Adrop (Sdrop: c) and the time taken to reach Arecover (Srecover: d) (s). The rate of change of temperature from Adrop to Arecover was represented by the slope (Mrecover: e).

Figure 4

Table 1. GLMM analysis of the amplitudes and the dynamics of specific properties of the tail temperature response to either no Cheerios, one Cheerio or three Cheerios (n = 35). The table shows the fixed effects included in the models. Individual rat identification is the random effect also included in the models but is not shown. The significant P-values of using log likelihood ratio tests are shown in bold italic font

Figure 5

Table 2. GLMM analysis of proportion of scans showing rat behaviours per 10 min during the 30-min filming after exposure to either no Cheerios, one Cheerio or three Cheerios (n = 35). The table shows the fixed effects included in the models. Individual rat identification is the random effect also included in the models but is not shown. The significant P-values of using log likelihood ratio tests are shown in bold italic font

Figure 6

Figure 5. Bar graphs indicating mean (± SE) of proportion of scans showing (a) ‘Escape/Mobility’, (b) ‘Explorative’, (c) ‘Fear/Anxiety’ and (d) ‘Resting stationary’ behaviours and eliminating (defaecating/urinating) behaviour counts per 10 min of rats during the 30-min time-period after treatment (neutral, one Cheerio or three Cheerios) exposure (n = 34).

Supplementary material: File

Wongsaengchan et al. supplementary material 1
Download undefined(File)
File 177.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Wongsaengchan et al. supplementary material 2
Download undefined(File)
File 115.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Wongsaengchan et al. supplementary material 3
Download undefined(File)
File 117.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Wongsaengchan et al. supplementary material 4
Download undefined(File)
File 104.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Wongsaengchan et al. supplementary material 5
Download undefined(File)
File 148.8 KB