Introduction
The modern idea of purebred dogs has come under increasing critical scrutiny over recent decades, first by academics, and later in the public and the media. What is here referred to as purebred dog breeds are, as described later, the effects of man-made selection to create groups of dogs of similar appearance with a defined pedigree (a known recorded lineage), and their pedigreed or non-pedigreed descendants. A main concern has been that many dogs from high-profile breeds, despite many good intentions, end up suffering from a high burden of inherited disorders due to limited founder populations and inbreeding as well as disabilities resulting from deliberate selection for extreme conformation traits that negatively affect both their welfare and longevity (ICECDogs 2024). In light of this critical focus, some new trends in how companion dogs are bred and acquired have emerged in many countries across the globe. This means a diminishing influence from traditional kennel clubs with more purebred dogs sold without pedigrees, stricter legal restrictions on dog breeding (LAGECDogs 2024a), growing popularity of deliberate crosses of established breeds (i.e. so-called designer breeds) and growing hype around potential health benefits in mixed-breed dogs (O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, McMillan, Church and Brodbelt2023). In this Horizon paper, we aim to (a) give an overview of these trends, and (b) discuss to what extent they will serve to promote future dogs that are healthy, have good welfare and also function well in their various expected roles in today’s world.
The rise and fall of the modern pedigree dog
From the wolf to early breed-like types of dogs
The domestic dog originates from a close relative of today’s grey wolf through a domestication process that occurred between 40,000 and 15,000 years ago somewhere on the Eurasian continent and perhaps in more than one location (Frantz et al. Reference Frantz, Mullin, Pionnier-Capitan, Lebrasseur, Ollivier, Perri, Linderholm, Mattiangeli, Teasdale, Dimopoulos, Tresset, Duffraisse, McCormick, Bartosiewicz, Gál, Nyerges, Sablin, Bréhard, Mashkour, Bălăşescu, Gillet, Hughes, Chassaing, Hitte, Vigne, Dobney, Hänni, Bradley and Larson2016; Leeb Reference Leeb2023). Currently, there are two predominant “origin stories” describing this process (Serpell Reference Serpell2021).
The dominant story for many years has been the commensal scavenger hypothesis, proposing that wolves essentially domesticated themselves by staying close to Paleolithic human settlements in search of food scraps (Coppinger & Coppinger Reference Coppinger and Coppinger2001). However, this hypothesis has recently been challenged by several arguments. One is that late Pleistocene humans were unlikely to have generated enough waste food to sustain a population of scavenging wolves (Jung & Pörtl Reference Jung and Pörtl2018). Another counter-argument is that, for safety reasons, Pleistocene humans would have been unlikely to tolerate unsocialised wolves in such proximity to their settlements (Serpell Reference Serpell2021).
The alternative theory is known as the pet-keeping or cross-species adoption hypothesis. This theory derives legitimacy from observations of pet-keeping among contemporary societies of hunter-gatherers. According to this theory, habitual nurturing behaviour motivates humans to capture, adopt and rear infant mammals, such as wolf pups. During the Pleistocene epoch, most of these early pets would probably have reverted to living independently when they reached maturity, but a minority – those possessing socially desirable traits, such as tameability, trainability, and sociability – might have received favoured treatment and hence been more likely to remain under human control and survive to give rise to domestic descendants with similar characteristics (Serpell Reference Serpell2021).
Either way, selective pressures from the need to survive in a human environment have strongly shaped the modern dog’s genome and studies have identified genetic changes associated with dog domestication such as tameness, coat colour (Ostrander & Wayne Reference Ostrander and Wayne2005), and adaptation to a diet influenced by human food sources (Axelsson et al. Reference Axelsson, Ratnakumar, Arendt, Maqbool, Webster, Perloski, Liberg, Arnemo, Hedhammar and Lindblad-Toh2013; Arendt et al. Reference Arendt, Cairns, Ballard, Savolainen and Axelsson2016).
Archaeological findings demonstrate that the relationship between humans and their dogs from these early days of domestication already extended beyond just the practical utility of dogs. Burials of dogs alongside humans 14,000 years ago in Germany (Janssens et al. Reference Janssens, Giemsch, Schmitz, Street, Van Dongen and Crombé2018) and 12,000 years ago in Israel (Davis & Valla Reference Davis and Valla1978) suggest that strong emotional connections existed between humans and dogs that are also documented in numerous other prehistorical findings and by modern ethnographic evidence (Morey Reference Morey2006; Serpell Reference Serpell2017; pp 300–315). The Susa ceramics from 4,000 BC Iran seem to represent some of the oldest documentation of a recognisable breed type, with images of dogs that greatly resemble modern sighthounds, such as salukis and Afghan hounds (Hole & Wyllie Reference Hole and Wyllie2007). Aligned with this, genetic studies have reported an ancient origin of sighthound breeds and other breed types (Parker et al. Reference Parker, Kim, Sutter, Carlson, Lorentzen, Malek, Johnson, DeFrance, Ostrander and Kruglyak2004; Dutrow et al. Reference Dutrow, Serpell and Ostrander2022).
Development of modern dog breeding
While phenotypically diverse types of dogs – so-called blood lines – have existed for thousands of years, the modern notion of a ‘dog breed’ is a much more recent invention. This happened in Britain in the Victorian era in the second half of the 19th century. It was a time of great power and wealth for Britain, with expansion of the empire across the globe. The era was also characterised by industrial development, as well as rapid developments in science (Worboys et al. Reference Worboys, J-M and Pemberton2018).
The first documented dog show was held in Newcastle-upon-Tyne in 1859, the same year that Charles Darwin published The Origin of Species. The so-called dog fancy grew out of the earlier poultry and pigeon fancies, and the hobby breeding of prize-winning livestock that emerged in England during the late 18th and early 19th centuries (Ritvo Reference Ritvo1987; pp 97–99). Consequently, breeding of dogs became closely related to dog-showing competitions and was linked to social status. As stated by the historian, Harriet Ritvo: “The institutions that defined the dog fancy projected an obsessively detailed vision of a stratified order which sorted animals and, by implication, people into snug and appropriate niches” (Ritvo Reference Ritvo1987; p 93).
The foundation of the pedigree history of a breed was the stud book and later the breed registry. All members of a certain breed should either belong to a group of founder animals or should be offspring of these. In the beginning, the breed registry was open to allow entry of new founder animals with desirable phenotypes, but this practice ceased little-by-little. For each breed, an ideal conformation called a breed standard was described toward which breeders directed their selection efforts and based on which the judges at shows awarded their prizes (Skipper Reference Skipper, Packer and O’Neill2022a). Selection was required to maintain or improve the desired appearance, with ‘improve’ being taken to mean conforming more closely to these breed standards.
As already mentioned, some dog breeds formally recognised in the 1800s were preceded by breed-like types (or varieties or classes) of dogs. Earlier publications like Johannes Caius’s De Canibus Britannicis in Latin from 1570 and translated to English in 1576 described 17 varieties of dogs categorised by function and including names like ‘setters’ and ‘spaniels’ (Caius Reference Caius1576). Despite bearing names prefiguring those of nineteenth-century breeds, these varieties were not necessarily similar in appearance to the modern dogs called setters or spaniels. It was not until the eighteenth century that the term ‘breed’ defined as “a subspecies with definable physical characteristics that would reliably reproduce itself if its members were crossed with each other” was invented, beginning with foxhounds (Ritvo Reference Ritvo1987; p 93).
During the early years of the dog fancy, impartial and objective evaluation by show judges was an ideal that was hard to achieve in practice. Competition for prizes – some of which were pecuniary – was intense, and genuine fraud consequently took place behind the scenes of the dog shows. Coats of dogs were dyed to cover faults, ears and tails were surgically ‘corrected’, and false information was published in catalogues (Skipper Reference Skipper2020). To counteract the diminishing reputation of dog shows and dog breeding, after much political wrangling, a group describing themselves as “true sportsmen” formed the English Kennel Club – or just the Kennel Club (about to be renamed as the Royal Kennel Club) – in 1873. The main initial task of this new organisation was to combat fraud by establishing the identity and descent of pedigree dogs. At the same time, a system was developed that limited participation in dog shows, and thereby competition, to a carefully screened segment of the canine population (Ritvo Reference Ritvo1987; p 102). From then – and continuing today – all pedigree dogs were required to have a recorded parentage documented in a breed registry. However, many breed registers were still open to non-pedigree individuals all the way into the 1950s and the overall registry of the Kennel Club was not officially closed until 1 January 1971 (Skipper Reference Skipper2022b).
Many other European countries followed this model. The French Societé Centrale Canine was established in 1882, and in Scandinavia, Sweden and Finland established national kennel clubs in 1889, Denmark in 1897 and Norway in 1898. The international umbrella organisation for dog breeding, the Fédération Cynologique Internationale (FCI), was founded in 1911 and includes kennel clubs from most of the world except the UK and US (Sandøe et al. Reference Sandøe, Corr and Palmer2015; p 15). The American Kennel Club was founded in 1884 by a group of American members of elite families who pledged “to do everything to advance the study, breeding, exhibiting, running and maintenance of purity of thoroughbred dogs” (Lemonick Reference Lemonick2001).
The first studbook of the English Kennel Club identified 40 different dog breeds. This number has grown substantially over the years and in 2023, the year of the 150th anniversary, more than 200 breeds were recognised (The Kennel Club 2024). Worldwide, around 400 different breeds are described by kennel clubs with over 800 breeds described by owners in the wider general public (O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, McMillan, Church and Brodbelt2023). Some of these breeds, as mentioned, resemble and derive from types of dogs that previously existed as geographic varieties (or landraces) used for hunting, herding, guarding and other functions. Others result from creative efforts of passionate and entrepreneurial individuals. The English golden retriever, for instance, was created by hybridising a yellow wavy-coated retriever over several generations with a spaniel, a setter, a Labrador retriever, and a bloodhound. Many modern breeds are, therefore, essentially early examples of what we currently call designer crossbreeding and represent the product of deliberate breeding involving a range of different breeds or types of dogs (Sampson & Binns Reference Sampson and Binns2006).
The adoption of structured pedigree dog breeding in combination with adoption of the English model of a national kennel club overseeing the process led to relatively uniform structures for dog fancier activities across Europe, North America, Australasia, and other parts of the world. Within individual countries, dog populations were divided into pedigree dogs and ‘the rest’. The founders of the kennel clubs articulated this division as a beneficial and necessary step towards promoting their contemporary concept of the healthier pure subset of the overall populations at a time where mainstream beliefs around dog health centred around upholding an aristocracy of ‘pure’ bloodlines and avoiding adulteration from inferior non-pedigree blood lines (Worboys et al. Reference Worboys, J-M and Pemberton2018). However, even then, some far-sighted individuals already predicted later problems related to inbreeding (Walsh Reference Walsh1879; p 265).
Documentation and growing awareness of negative effects on health-related welfare of many breeds
More recent concerns regarding health disorders in purebred dogs have emerged gradually since the 1960s and are based on three dominant issues: (a) the unintended consequences of inbreeding; (b) selection for extreme conformation traits such as short muzzles (brachycephaly) and; (c) inadequate selection against disease-predisposing gene variants and phenotypes.
Inbreeding
The build-up of heritable diseases is a predictable result of increased homozygosity and progressive loss of genetic variation over time in closed breeding populations, no matter the species, due to selection and genetic drift. In dogs, however, some additional factors seem to have fueled the process. Many breeds were established with a limited number of founders so when the studbooks were closed – an event which occurred at variable times in different breeds and countries – the genetic variation was already small. Also, only a small proportion of each generation of dog breeds generally contributes to the next generation. Extensive use of popular sires (often dogs that have achieved extraordinary results at dog shows, utility tests or other kinds of dog sports) increases the frequency of the genetic variants they carry – including deleterious mutations – in the population. The widely promoted practice of deliberate inbreeding and line breeding – at least in the past – increased the risk of homozygosity for these variants in subsequent generations leading to the current situation with high levels of inherited disorders (Oliehoek et al. Reference Oliehoek, Bijma and van der Meijden2009; Leroy & Baumung Reference Leroy and Baumung2011; Marsden et al. Reference Marsden, Ortega-Del Vecchyo, O’Brien, Taylor, Ramirez, Vilà, Marques-Bonet, Schnabel, Wayne and Lohmueller2016; Broeckx Reference Broeckx2020; Ikolo et al. Reference Ikolo, Maity, Finn, Abdullah, Tajic, Cameron, Maj, Ikolo, Maity, Finn, Abdullah, Tajic, Cameron and Maj2023).
Some examples of these canine inherited disorders, include the eye disease ‘progressive rod-cone degeneration’ recorded in several breeds (Zangerl et al. Reference Zangerl, Goldstein, Philp, Lindauer, Pearce-Kelling, Mullins, Graphodatsky, Ripoll, Felix and Stone2006), disc herniation in dachshunds (Jensen & Christensen Reference Jensen and Christensen2000) and the cardiac disease myxomatous mitral valve disease in cavalier King Charles spaniels (O’Brien et al. Reference O’Brien, Beijerink and Wade2021). It should be noted that selection against disease-predisposing variants can result in homozygosity but of a desirable kind.
Breeding for extreme phenotypes
Extreme conformation in dogs describes a physical appearance that has been so significantly altered by selective breeding away from the ancestral natural canine appearance that affected dogs commonly suffer from poor health and welfare, with negative impacts on their quality and quantity of life (ICECDogs 2024). For example, the short muzzled (brachycephalic) breeds, such as French bulldog, English bulldog, and pug commonly have breathing difficulties, and dental, and eye problems (Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks, Tivers and Burn2015; O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, Pegram, Crocker, Brodbelt, Church and Packer2020; Packer & O’Neill Reference Packer and O’Neill2022), the short, rounded skull of the cavalier King Charles spaniel and the Chihuahua can cause syringomyelia, a neurological disease, due to compression of the brain tissue (Chandler et al. Reference Chandler, Volk, Rusbridge and Jeffery2008; Kiviranta et al. Reference Kiviranta, Rusbridge, Laitinen-Vapaavuori, Hielm-Björkman, Lappalainen, Knowler and Jokinen2017), and excessive skin relative to the skeleton in several breeds increases the risk of skin-fold dermatitis (O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, Rowe, Brodbelt, Pegram and Hendricks2022a).
Within FCI, the breed standards, by which the show judges are required to evaluate dogs, are written and managed by the country of origin for each breed (FCI 2023). This also holds true for the British breeds even though the Kennel Club is not a member of FCI. In the US, the process is more decentralised, and the breed standards are written and maintained by the individual breed clubs (AKC 2024). The wording of some breed standards has unfortunately supported progressive exaggeration of certain breed characteristics over time. For example, the American Kennel Club breed standard for the English bulldog states that: “The face, measured from the front of the cheekbone to the tip of the nose, should be extremely short”, and later: “The nose should be large, broad and black, its tip set back deeply between the eyes. The distance from the bottom of the stop, between the eyes, to the tip of the nose should be as short as possible and not exceed the length from the tip of the nose to the edge of underlip” (AKC 2016). The equivalent standards from The Kennel Club and the FCI are slightly less extreme stating that the face of a bulldog should be “relatively short, muzzle broad, blunt, and inclined slightly upwards, although not excessively so. Viewed from side, head appears very high and moderately short from back to point of nose” (FCI 2011; KC 2023). Furthermore, these breed-defining phenotypes have tended to become exaggerated in some breeds over time – seldom because the wording of the breed standards has changed but more often because their interpretation by breeders and judges tends to drift.
Consequently, over the years, the muzzles of many dogs of brachycephalic breeds have become shorter, the ears of many basset hounds have become longer and the downward lumbar slope of the back of a German shepherd dog described in the standard as “slightly downwards” has over time become steeper in many dogs. The motivations behind these progressive exaggerations are intriguing but could be examples of what are termed supernormal stimuli in ethology (Tinbergen Reference Tinbergen1951). Here, exaggerated versions of a stimulus elicit a stronger response than the stimulus for which a specific behaviour evolved (Ghirlanda & Enquist Reference Ghirlanda and Enquist2003). In other words, when judging a group of quite uniform dogs, the one that stands out due to its more exaggerated features gets the most attention – and therefore wins the prize, thereby promoting even greater moves towards exaggeration in the future. This may also have affected across-breed comparisons which contribute to the final competitions for ‘Best in Show’ titles in dog shows (Markarian Reference Markarian2023). Regardless of cause, comparisons of historically old and new images of the same breed suggest that the exterior characteristics of many breeds have, and continue to, become more extreme over time (Sandøe et al. Reference Sandøe, Corr and Palmer2015; Serpell Reference Serpell2019).
Inadequate selection against disease-predisposing gene variants and phenotypes
All domestic animals have changed over time because of selection for different purposes. Today, livestock breeding is concentrated on relatively few, large international and commercial companies which operate within well-defined selection parameters related to yield, growth or other economically important traits. Dog breeding on the contrary is a decentral and often hobby-based activity, where breeding decisions are influenced by more individuals with far less objective selection criteria. Health-based breeding, or direct selection against disease-predisposing gene variants and phenotypes in specific breeds, has been prioritised to some extent (FCI 2020) but prize-winning at dog shows and other competitions as well as individual breeder’s preferences for type, and colour are also in play. The most common health-screening programmes include radiographic evaluations for hip and elbow dysplasia (FCI 2006; IEWG 2024), clinical eye examinations to detect inherited diseases like PRA (Progressive Retinal Atrophy) or cataract (ECVO 2024) and heart scans to screen for diseases like MMVD (Myxomatous Mitral Valve Disease) (Birkegård et al. Reference Birkegård, Reimann, Martinussen, Häggström, Pedersen and Olsen2016), DCM (Dilated Cardiomyopathy) or SAS (Sub-Aortal Stenosis) (ESVC 2024). An increasing number of DNA tests for inherited diseases have become available for dog breeders in recent years (IPFD 2024) and estimated breeding values, which have been a highly effective tool in livestock breeding, have also been introduced to a limited extent (Lewis et al. Reference Lewis, Woolliams and Blott2010). Some kennel clubs and breed clubs have breed-specific health criteria which must be met for puppies to be registered and assigned a pedigree, but the level of regulation and administrative practice for screening results varies greatly between countries and from breed-to-breed. In addition, this regulation only applies to the dogs within each breed which have a pedigree.
Growing public awareness of welfare problems linked to dog breeding
The BBC1 television documentary, Pedigree Dogs Exposed, aired in August 2008, is widely considered as the tipping point for public awareness and concern about many deleterious effects of organised dog breeding on dog health and welfare (Nicholas Reference Nicholas2011; Lawler Reference Lawler2012). Similar concerns had been expressed and documented earlier but had failed to gain widespread public traction. One of the earliest investigations into physical defects in purebred dogs was carried out at the request of the Kennel Club in 1963 by the British Small Animal Veterinary Association (BSAVA) (Hodgman Reference Hodgman1963). Many subsequent studies from a wide range of academic researchers (Peyer & Steiger Reference Peyer and Steiger1998; McGreevy & Nicholas Reference McGreevy and Nicholas1999; McGreevy Reference McGreevy2007; Asher et al. Reference Asher, Diesel, Summers, McGreevy and Collins2009) addressed similar concerns. While veterinarians initially focused internationally on issues such as hip dysplasia in popular breeds of the day (e.g. German shepherd dogs and Labrador retrievers), concerns about brachycephaly were not prominent from the start, partly because at that time many of the seriously affected dog breeds were much less popular than they are today (Packer & O’Neill Reference Packer and O’Neill2022; pp 127–151).
In parallel with growing concerns about health issues related to purebred dog breeding, the documentation of population-based evidence on key breed-related health and welfare issues also underwent rapid development based on improved access to reliable and sufficient clinical data (O′Neill et al. Reference O′Neill, Church, McGreevy, Thomson and Brodbelt2014; Gough et al. Reference Gough, Thomas and O’Neill2018). The development of systematic programmes of research that apply Big Data approaches with a specific focus on breed-related disorders now provide animal welfare scientists with sufficient evidence to support both calls for active change and the design of policy reforms. For example, the VetCompassTM Research Programme at the Royal Veterinary College, with its stated focus on welfare issues of dogs since its inception in 2010, has generated over 100 publications on a wide range of topics related to canine breed health and welfare between 2012 and 2024 (VetCompass 2024). Unfortunately, to date, neither scientific evidence nor public awareness from media have proven overly effective in changing the dominant behaviours of many breed organisations or the public towards prioritising health over looks for popular breeds.
Slow and insufficient actions from kennel clubs
During the past 25 years, the national kennel clubs, and the international collaborations they belong to, have undertaken a number of initiatives, typically in the form of internal debates nationally and internationally and seminars for show judges, in efforts to respond to the major problems linked to the concept and practice of breeding pedigree dogs (Hedhammar & Indrebø Reference Hedhammar and Indrebø2011). Despite these well-intentioned initiatives, very little real-world change in the health, conformation, or welfare issues of problematic dog breeds appears to have been achieved.
As stated previously, many of today’s problems of pedigree dogs can be traced back to how these dog breeds were established, selected, and maintained through breeding in partially or fully closed populations or in the pursuit of exaggerated conformation traits. The way selective breeding is currently practiced for pedigree dog breeding generally results in progressively diminishing genetic diversity over time which impacts negatively on the health and welfare of the dogs (Leroy & Baumung Reference Leroy and Baumung2011; Kraus et al. Reference Kraus, Snyder-Mackler and Promislow2023). In efforts to redress this loss of diversity, some breed clubs have established outcrossing projects to attempt to rescue breeds with low genetic diversity and/or high incidence of genetic disease, such as the Norwegian lundehund (Melis et al. Reference Melis, Pertoldi, Ludington, Beuchat, Qvigstad and Stronen2022; Powell Reference Powell2011), and the Irish red and white setter (Irish Red and White Setter Club 2024). In 2023, the Swedish Kennel Klub approved a crossbreeding project for cavalier King Charles spaniel (SKC 2023) and the Finnish Kennel Club has done the same for both cavalier King Charles spaniel and French bulldogs (FKC 2023). As professor in veterinary neurology, Clare Rusbridge, stated in The Guardian in June 2024: “Dog breeds must be “rebooted” through careful crossbreeding to save them from ingrained health problems” (Davis Reference Davis2024).
These outcrossing projects are usually time-limited and include a well-defined and limited number of dogs. To our knowledge, maintaining an open breed registry within the framework of a national kennel club is rare, but nevertheless exists. The Danish Swedish farmdog was reconstructed in the 1980s and received FCI-recognition in 2019. All the way through the reconstruction process, the breed register was kept open, and still remains open by the Scandinavian kennel clubs. This means that owners of dogs showing phenotypic resemblance with the Danish Swedish farmdog can have their dog evaluated by an authorised judge and – if approved – the dog is enrolled in the breed register with a blank pedigree (DSGK 2024). The Finnish kennel club has an open breed registry for a number of other breeds, including the Jack Russell terrier, Lapponian herder, Norbottenspitz, Pyrenean sheepdogs, and Finnish lapdog. Apart from the phenotypic evaluation, entry into the breed also requires a DNA profile and testing for breed-specific hereditary diseases (FKC 2024). As a way to obtain better health, the Danish kennel club decided to open the breed registry for the English bulldog, French bulldog and pug by 1 May 2024. Entry into the registry requires a phenotypic evaluation and a maximum grade for BOAS (Brachycephalic Obstructive Airway Syndrome) of 0 or 1 (DKC 2024a).
Some of the former utility breeds, such as the German shepherd dog, border collie and Labrador retriever, have become popular as show and family dogs. This has resulted in show lines where selection is based almost entirely on meeting the current preferences of dog show judges, and in lines of family dogs produced on a commercial basis to physically represent the common public perception of the breeds. In both cases, there has been a switch away from selecting for the original functional behavioural traits, such as hunting or herding ability, that once characterised these breeds. Consequently, separate ‘breeds within the breed’ have emerged consisting of utility (working) lines, show lines and family dogs that can show quite remarkable differences in phenotype as well as basic behaviours and activity levels (Duffy et al. Reference Duffy, Hsu and Serpell2008; Fadel et al. Reference Fadel, Driscoll, Pilot, Wright, Zulch and Mills2016). Often, there is practically no exchange of genetic material between the two groups resulting in stratification and reduced effective population sizes within each (Chang et al. Reference Chang, Yokoyama, Branson, Dyer, Hitte, Overall and Hamilton2009).
The likelihood of achieving dog owner’s satisfaction increases when the needs and expectations of the owner are compatible with the behaviour of the dog (Curb et al. Reference Curb, Abramson, Grice and Kennison2013). Thus, mismatches between expectations of owners and the behaviours of dogs with respect to within-breed lines may increase the risk of a problematic dog-owner relationship.
The typical human behaviour of choosing breeding animals based on show champions and sometimes even because some influencers promote extreme variants of dogs, e.g. the ‘tea-cup’ Chihuahua (Redmalm Reference Redmalm2014), has resulted in other serious issues. What Harriet Ritvo wrote about the first dog shows in the late 18th century sadly still holds true: “When judges selected the prizewinner, they were not simply recognizing a particular outstanding animal. At the same time, they were identifying the strain to which the prizewinner belonged as promising breeding material, and they were endorsing a type toward which other breeders should aspire” (Ritvo Reference Ritvo1987; p 101).
Even though many breed standards of the FCI and other kennel clubs have been revised numerous times since their first versions right up to the present day, and all FCI standards now contain the general phrase that “any departure from the foregoing points should be considered a fault and the seriousness with which the fault should be regarded should be in exact proportion to its degree and its effect upon the health and welfare of the dog”, there is still room for improvement of the health focus in the standards and in their application. The Nordic countries have formulated so-called “Breed Specific Instructions” (NKU 2023) in which judges at dog shows are informed about relevant health issues in selected breeds. However, despite all this, prizes are still awarded to dogs that show extreme conformation (Burke Reference Burke2024). Sadly, therefore, limited progress in practice has been made based on the well-intentioned initiatives from the established breeding organisations.
Reactions from civil society and regulators
For decades, many Western countries concentrated their animal welfare policies and regulatory initiatives primarily on production and laboratory animals. Regulatory initiatives aimed at protecting the welfare of companion animals have been scarce, and there has been little legislation until recently specifically aimed at dogs over and above general formulations in some, but far from all, anti-cruelty and animal welfare statutes (Andersen et al. Reference Andersen, Meyer, Forkman, Nielsen and Sandøe2021). However, due to growing negative publicity and pressure from animal welfare non-governmental organisations (NGOs), governments and courts in some countries seem to have lost faith in the ability of the dog-breeding fraternity to reform itself from the inside. Consequently, national legislation is starting to be passed and/or more effectively enforced in a few countries (LAGECDogs 2024a).
In The Netherlands, the keeping and breeding of pets is regulated by Article 3.4 of the 2014 Dutch Animal Keepers Decree (Overheid 2024). According to this, the breeding of companion animals in ways that have negative effects on their welfare and health is prohibited. Hereditary defects and conformational traits with negative effects on welfare and health should be avoided as well as the passing of behavioural abnormalities to offspring. Animal protection organisations in The Netherlands have been particularly active in highlighting the impaired welfare of purebred dogs with extreme phenotypes, especially the brachycephalic breeds.
The Expertise Centre on the Genetics of Companion Animals at Utrecht University was asked by the Dutch authorities to provide guidelines for the enforcement of the decree and decided to focus initially on welfare issues of brachycephalic dogs. Based on knowledge about the health issues related to brachycephaly, a list of criteria was defined that all dogs used for breeding should comply with. The background and the criteria are described in a report commissioned by the Dutch Ministry of Agriculture, Nature and Food Quality in 2020 (van Hagen Reference van Hagen2020).
As of May 2020, Dutch breeders are therefore required to have physical measurements carried out on their breeding dogs by a veterinary practitioner prior to breeding, following the criteria outlined by the ministry. A central, though debated, criterion is the calculation of a craniofacial ratio (CFR) which is the ratio between the length of the muzzle and the cranial length. The CFR should preferably be above 0.5. Based on the level of fulfilment of the criteria, a traffic light system is used to indicate whether the animals can be used for breeding (green), can be accepted during a transition period (yellow), or are unacceptable (red). However, very few individuals of some breeds can meet this yellow or green criterion (Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks, Tivers and Burn2015; Liu et al. Reference Liu, Troconis, Kalmar, Price, Wright, Adams, Sargan and Ladlow2017) so in practice this amounts to a ban on breeding a number of flat-faced dog breeds. The legal actions in The Netherlands originally focused on breeders associated with the national kennel club, neglecting the fact that many individuals from the breeds in question are imported from outside the country in which the legislation had no effect. To address this issue, the Dutch Minister of Agriculture, Nature and Food Quality has announced that a general ban on the keeping of pets with “harmful external characteristics”, is being considered (FECAVA 2023).
An example of a legal action in relation to the welfare problems in purebred dogs comes from a lawsuit in Norway from 2020 to 2023. The Norwegian Society for Protection of Animals (NSPA) sued the Norwegian Kennel Club (NKC), the Norwegian Bulldog Club, the Norwegian Cavalier Club, three breeders of cavalier King Charles spaniels, and three breeders of English bulldogs for non-compliance with the Norwegian Animal Welfare Act § 25 which states that: “breeding shall encourage characteristics resulting in robust animals that function well and have good health”. The legislation goes on to specify that breeding cannot be carried out if it: “reduces the animals’ ability to practice natural behaviour or gives rise to general ethical reactions” (Lovdata 2009) (authors’ translation). The case was brought to the Oslo District Court, then to the Oslo Court of Appeal and finally to the Supreme Court which concluded that further breeding of cavalier King Charles spaniels with the current genetic pool is in violation of the Animal Welfare Act. The Supreme Court also ruled that English bulldogs must be bred under a breeding programme aimed at reducing the occurrence of diseases such as Brachycephalic Obstructive Airway Syndrome (BOAS) caused by extreme conformation (Supreme Court of Norway 2023).
Similarly, § 11b of the German Animal Welfare Act outlaws the breeding of animals where it is expected that, due to heredity, the animals themselves, or their offspring, will lack body parts or organs for appropriate use, or have body parts that are unsuitable, or reshaped in such a way that pain, suffering, or damage may occur as a result. It is also forbidden to breed animals if it is expected that the offspring will develop hereditary behaviour that may cause suffering, e.g. aggressive behaviour (Bundesministerium der Justiz Reference der Justiz2023). In order to make the Animal Welfare Act in Germany clearer and more operational, an expert group appointed by the Federal Ministry of Food and Agriculture has produced the document “Expert opinion on the interpretation of § 11b of the Animal Welfare Act” (Bundesministerium für Ernährung und Landwirtschaft 2023). In addition, new, sharpened rules on dog shows came into force in Germany in January 2022. It is now prohibited for dogs to participate in shows and field trials if they have evidence of any of a range of hereditary conditions.
In England and Wales, the basis for the legal protection of dog health and welfare is provided by the Animal Welfare Act (AWA) (Legislation 2006). Legal safeguarding of dog welfare in England under the AWA was strengthened in 2018 when the government passed the Licensing of Activities Involving Animals (LAIA) Regulations in 2018 (Legislation 2018). The LAIA regulations govern a variety of activities involving animals, including that commercial dog breeders are legally required to apply for a licence from their local authority to breed dogs and, if successful, must abide by the provisions in the LAIA regulations. These state that “no dog may be kept for breeding if it can reasonably be expected, on the basis of its genotype, phenotype or state of health, that the breeding from it could have a detrimental effect on its health or welfare or the health or welfare of its offspring” (Legislation 2018). To this end, licence holders “must take all reasonable steps” to check that the dogs kept for breeding – male and female – have good physical and genetic health, good temperament, can see, breathe normally, are physically fit, can exercise freely, and “must be aware of any health risks that may be specific to the particular type or breed of dog” (GOV.UK 2024). Any person who fails to meet these criteria will be in breach of their licence and may face a penalty notice imposing an on-the-spot fine of up to £5,000; revocation of the licence; or in the case of a serious breach, court proceedings potentially resulting in an unlimited fine and a criminal record (LAGECDogs 2024b).
Decreasing popularity of pedigree dogs
It is unknown how many dogs were excluded during the first years of the various national kennel clubs in countries where the breed registries were closed nor the size of the total population of dogs in those days. However, it is known that the total number of dogs in many countries and the distribution of individual breeds have fluctuated dramatically over time for many reasons, including times of economic boom and recession, changes in human demography and ‘fashion’ both at the national and international level (Herzog Reference Herzog2006). Current knowledge about the structure and numbers of the wider dog populations has also improved during the last 25 years for several reasons, including mandatory microchip marking, licensing, and registration of dogs in several countries and the advent of big data research programmes (O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, McMillan, Church and Brodbelt2023; McMillan et al. Reference McMillan, Bielby, Williams, Upjohn, Casey and Christley2024).
For example, in Denmark, ID-marking and registration in the Danish Dog Registry (DDR) has been mandatory since 1993 (Retsinformation 2018). Analyses of data from the Danish Dog Registry and the registration figures provided by the Danish Kennel Club (DKC), show that the Danish dog population currently falls into three categories: (a) dogs with a pedigree from the DKC (around 35%); (b) dogs that are registered as a stated breed but without a DKC pedigree (around 50%); and (c) dogs that are registered as mixed breed (around 15%) (DKC 2024b).
These Danish proportional registrations of pedigree purebreds, purebreds without a pedigree, and mixed breeds are similar to those reported for the UK (O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, McMillan, Church and Brodbelt2023; KC 2024) and Germany (TASSO 2023) even though the legal basis and registration procedures for dogs differ between the countries. In Germany, microchip identification of dogs is regulated locally in the 16 different regions (‘Bundesländer’). In the UK, it became compulsory for owners to ensure that their dog is microchipped in 2016. However, several different microchip databases are in use that are not inter-linked, and there is no single overall UK national dog registry. In Scandinavia, Sweden introduced legislation like Denmark’s in 2001, but it is not possible to distinguish breeds to the same level of precision (Jordbruksverket 2001). Raad van Beheer, the kennel club of The Netherlands, has extended even beyond a requirement for microchipping with mandatory genetic profiling or ‘DNA fingerprinting’ (Raad van Beheer Reference van Beheer2024).
Data from the Danish Dog Registry reveal that the proportion of the Danish dog population that is kennel club registered has been falling over time. When mandatory microchipping was first enforced in 1993, the Danish Kennel Club (DKC) registered two-thirds of the total number of new puppies entering the register. This proportion decreased progressively over the following years until a cross-over point in 2002 where 51,035 new puppies were registered, 23,359 with a DKC pedigree (45.8%) and 27,676 without a DKC pedigree (54.2%) (Danish Dog Registry 2024; DKC 2024b; Figure 1).
In parallel with strong economic growth in the first years of the current millennium, demand for dogs as domestic pets in Denmark also increased. However, this demand was met mainly by suppliers from outside the DKC (Sandøe et al. Reference Sandøe, Lund, Nielsen, Proschowsky, Ralund and Romberg2022). Inability or even reluctance among the DKC breeders to scale-up dog production to meet increasing demand offers a possible explanation for this development. Globalisation of all markets for products and production, including that for puppies, and increasing sales from international online platforms that offer cheaper puppies and more convenient sales processes could be another. The financial crisis and subsequent recession in 2008 resulted in a plateau and then reduction in the annual number of dog registrations in Denmark. From 2018, the number of registrations increased again, a development which further accelerated during the global COVID-19 pandemic (Figure 1).
The popularity of different breeds also fluctuates over time. Urbanisation, changes in family constellations, and influence from movies and television series, social media, and celebrity dog owners along with many other factors contribute to a constant reshaping of breed ownership levels (Ghirlanda et al. Reference Ghirlanda, Acerbi, Herzog and Serpell2013, Reference Ghirlanda, Acerbi and Herzog2014). In the past decade, many countries have experienced a shift in the top five most common breeds, away from larger breeds such as German shepherd dogs and Labrador retrievers, towards smaller breeds such as the Chihuahua and Havanese, and brachycephalic breeds such as the pug and French bulldog (KC 2019; Haid Reference Haid2023; TASSO 2023).
There are also differences when it comes to the relative number of dogs with a pedigree across the spectrum of breeds. In the Danish Dog Registry, utility breeds, originally selected and used for hunting, sporting and other physical activities and competitions, tend to include a high pedigree proportion maybe because the pedigree is a prerequisite for participation in some activities and competitions. The proportion of individuals with a DKC pedigree is lower in breeds that are largely kept as companion animals (Figure 2). Thus, the link between the DKC and the ownership of many of the popular companion breeds has reduced dramatically. Presumably, this situation is similar across other countries and kennel clubs and implies that the kennel clubs are losing control of several breeds where most of the breeding is taking place outside traditional dog-breeding organisations.
Developments outside traditional dog breeding organisations
Besides declining proportions of purebred dogs who have a pedigree, several other developments in the last two to three decades have also affected how people in large parts of the world perceive and categorise ‘breed’ in dogs. So-called designer breeds and various other kinds of mixed breed dogs have become prominent contributors to widespread changes in dog-breed structures.
Designer breeds
History and development of designer breeds
The emergence of the so-called ‘designer dog breeds’ (also known as ‘designer-crossbred dogs’) has provided a new twist to the concept of ‘breed’. Designer dog breeds are deliberate crosses between two or more different pure breeds and have gained great popularity over the past two decades. A study of the demography of 2.25 million UK dogs in 2019 within the VetCompass™ Programme estimated that designer crossbreeds made up 6.7% of the UK dog population, with poodle heritage being very common in these dogs (O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, McMillan, Church and Brodbelt2023).
Wally Conron, who was then working at the Royal Guide Dogs Association of Australia, is often credited with the invention of the first of these modern designer breeds, the labradoodle, in the 1980s. He crossed one of his best performing Labrador retrievers with a poodle to create a guide dog to help a blind woman whose husband was allergic to dogs. His approach appeared to be successful in terms of a reduced allergic reaction from the husband and this led to an assumption that still prevails widely today that the non-shedding fur of the poodle – in purebreds as well as in F1-hybrids – could reduce or prevent allergic reactions (Burnett et al. Reference Burnett, Brand, O’Neill, Pegram, Belshaw, Stevens and Packer2022).
This assumption of hypo-allergenicity has since been shown to be more complex than originally thought (Nicholas et al. Reference Nicholas, Wegienka, Havstad, Zoratti, Ownby and Johnson2011; Vredegoor et al. Reference Vredegoor, Willemse, Chapman, Heederik and Krop2012) but is nevertheless often repeated in sales advertisements for many designer breeds that include poodle parentage. As well as the original labradoodle, these include cockapoo (cocker spaniel × poodle), cavapoo (cavalier King Charles spaniel × poodle), schnoodle (schnauzer × poodle) and maltipoo (Maltese × poodle). Mr Conron later expressed his regrets about triggering the whole phenomenon of designer breeds because the new trend encouraged unscrupulous people to set up puppy mills (Coren Reference Coren2014). Unfortunately, once Pandora’s box had been opened, the list of creative combinations kept getting longer with ‘breeds’ such as puggle (pug × beagle), pomsky (Pomeranian × husky), chorkie (Chihuahua × Yorkshire terrier) and many others being added.
Interviews with owners of designer dogs suggest that they expect their dogs to have stable temperaments, be healthier than other dogs, and have hypoallergenic and non-shedding coats. Also, these owners have the overall perception that their designer dogs somehow fit better into domestic life compared to other dogs (Power Reference Power2012; Powell et al. Reference Powell, Chia, McGreevy, Podberscek, Edwards, Neilly, Guastella, Lee and Stamatakis2018).
According to a recent study comparing owner expectations and reality associated with owning purebred dogs, mixed breeds, and designer breeds, owners generally appear to be happy with their choice of dog regardless of category. However, one area where the designer dogs apparently do not meet their owner’s expectations concerns the need for grooming. Among owners of ‘doodles’ (offspring resulting from the intentional mix of at least two purebreds, one of which was either a miniature, toy, or standard poodle), 18.2% lamented the cost and frequency of grooming required while the corresponding figures were lower for owners of mixed breeds (3.1%) and purebreds (5.2%) (Hladky-Krage & Hoffman Reference Hladky-Krage and Hoffman2022). That study also reported the decision to acquire a designer breed had been driven largely by the appearance of the breeds and by a perception that these dogs are healthier than purebred dogs (Hladky-Krage & Hoffman Reference Hladky-Krage and Hoffman2022). The popularity of designer breeds can thus to some extent be interpreted as a backlash to the perceived poor health condition of the traditional pedigree breeds. However, the validity of this perception of improved health in cross-bred dogs is still uncertain. The overall health and behaviour of designer crossbreeds may be no more than a regression to the mean of the values for these attributes in the parental breeds, as was suggested to explain the largely equivalent disorder risks overall between general crossbred dogs compared to purebred dogs in the UK (O′Neill et al. Reference O′Neill, Church, McGreevy, Thomson and Brodbelt2014).
Are designer breeds healthier than purebred dogs?
The general expectation of improved health in crossbreeds compared to purebred dogs has its roots in the concept of hybrid vigour or heterosis. This is the extent to which the average performance of first-cross (F1) individuals for a specified trait is superior to the average performance of their parental strains/breeds for that same trait (Nicholas Reference Nicholas2010). Charles Darwin was the first scientist to examine the phenomenon in a systematic manner (Darwin Reference Darwin1859), and the existence of a strong hybrid vigour effect is a foundational assumption underlying many livestock breeding schemes. However, there is only limited evidence supporting a substantial hybrid vigour effect in dogs (Nicholas et al. Reference Nicholas, Arnott and McGreevy2016).
It is important to note that hybrid vigour is not a fixed overall effect but relies on factors including the presence, absence and frequency of disease-liability variants, the heritability of the specific traits, and the levels of inbreeding in the breeds that are crossed (Nicholas et al. Reference Nicholas, Arnott and McGreevy2016). Besides the general effects of hybrid vigour from a genetic diversity perspective, some designer breeds were also created in efforts to reduce the negative effects of extreme conformation of one or more of the parent breeds. For example, an expected increase in nose length on average in puggles (pug × beagle) compared to purebred pugs might be predicted to lead to reduced prevalence of BOAS although – to our knowledge – no systematic studies to confirm these effects are published. However, studies have shown that crossing two breeds with reported predisposition to obesity could lead to even higher levels of obesity in the hybrid dog, stressing that hybrid vigour is not a solution to all types of problems (Pegram et al. Reference Pegram, Raffan, White, Ashworth, Brodbelt, Church and O’Neill2021; O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, Sahota, Brodbelt, Church, Packer and Pegram2022b).
As designer breeds become more established in the general dog population, it is likely that future designer puppies will be produced increasingly from backcrosses to one of the parental breeds or of intercrosses between F1 and later generations of designer crossbred parents (Arendt et al. Reference Arendt, Fagerlund, Gelskov, Lorenz, Nielsen and Sandøe2024). This will turn designer breeds into de facto new breeds where, for example, all the parents, grandparents, great-grandparents and so forth of labradoodle pups will also have been labradoodles. This is now the case in Switzerland where the Swiss Kennel Club accepts labradoodles as a registered breed (Swiss Cynological Federation 2024). Therefore, any positive effects of heterosis are likely to wear out over time, and in terms of breeding-related health problems the designer breeds are likely to face similar problems to those faced by traditional breeds unless steps are taken to avoid the issues of inbreeding, the breeding towards extreme conformation and the lack of adequate focus on reducing disease-predisposing gene variants that have plagued so many of the longer-established dog breeds.
As with the alluring simplicity but faulty logic of considering all pedigree dogs as one group, it is also dangerous to consider all designer breeds as being a single entity, given the range of health risks and issues that could affect their various progenitor breeds (Gough et al. Reference Gough, Thomas and O’Neill2018). The limited research that does exist comparing mental and physical health in designer breeds to pure breeds has delivered mixed results. For example, a prospective study of ophthalmic disorders in labradoodles compared with their parental breeds showed that the prevalence of multifocal retinal dysplasia (MRD) was significantly higher in labradoodles than in Labrador retrievers and poodles. For cataracts, no significant differences were identified (Nicholas et al. Reference Nicholas, Arnott and McGreevy2016).
The standard poodle is recognised as genetically predisposed to hypoadrenocorticism (Friedenberg et al. Reference Friedenberg, Lunn and Meurs2017). A UK VetCompassTM study reported the standard poodle with 51.38 times the odds of hypoadrenocorticism compared to general crossbred dogs while the Labrador retriever had 0.82 times the odds. However, the poodle-based designer crossbreed labradoodle showed an intermediate risk between the two progenitor breeds rather than a total elimination of the combined parental predisposition, with 7.40 times the odds (Schofield et al. Reference Schofield, Woolhead, Johnson, Brodbelt, Church and O’Neill2021). New diseases, which have not been passed down from the parental breeds, have also emerged in designer breeds. For instance, a novel mutation in the dystrophin gene causing a disorder similar to human Duchenne muscular dystrophy (DMD) has been identified in a line of Australian labradoodles (Shrader et al. Reference Shrader, Jung, Denney and Smith2018).
A recent study from the UK compared the odds for 57 common disorders across three designer-crossbreeds (cavapoo, cockapoo, and labradoodle) and each of their progenitor breeds (cavalier King Charles spaniel, cocker spaniel, Labrador retriever, and poodle). The odds did not differ significantly between the designer-crossbreeds and their relevant progenitor breeds in 86.6% of the comparisons challenging the widespread beliefs of major positive hybrid vigour effects for health in designer-crossbreeds (Bryson et al. Reference Bryson, O’Neill, Brand, Belshaw and Packer2024).
A special concern for designer-crossbreeds is their mental health and the resultant expressed behaviours. Selective breeding over the past century has created a range of traditional dog breeds which differ in behaviour and personality traits. The degree of genetic attribution to both working behaviours, including herding and retrieving, and other everyday behaviours such as sociability, fearfulness, and aggression is debated, but it is widely accepted that behaviour is to some extent heritable (Saetre et al. Reference Saetre, Strandberg, Sundgren, Pettersson, Jazin and Bergström2006; MacLean et al. Reference EL, Snyder-Mackler, von Holdt and Serpell2019; Dutrow et al. Reference Dutrow, Serpell and Ostrander2022). Still, very little is known about the expression of behavioural traits in crossbreeds including designer-crossbreeds compared to the behaviour of the constituent pure breeds.
Based on quantitative genetics theory, there are good reasons to anticipate that first-cross designer dogs would exhibit behavioural phenotypes closely aligned with an intermediate profile between their purebred parent breeds (Falconer & Mackay Reference Falconer and Mackay2009 p 254). This expectation generally held true with some notable exceptions in a study of goldendoodles and labradoodles (Shouldice et al. Reference Shouldice, Edwards, Serpell, Niel and Robinson2019). The study analysed breed differences using the online Canine Behavioural Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ; Hsu & Serpell Reference Hsu and Serpell2003). Labrador retriever and standard poodle did not differ significantly from those of labradoodles for any behaviour category, but miniature poodles scored significantly higher than labradoodles for dog rivalry (aggressive or threatening responses to other familiar dogs in the same household). The goldendoodle had higher scores than the parent breeds for both dog-directed aggression and dog-directed fear. To our knowledge, no studies have analysed whether specific combinations between breeds are more likely to result in problematic behaviour than others, but this is a question of key relevance for future research given that most designer-crossbreds are destined for the companion animal market.
Though few studies have so far been published on the health of designer breeds, it seems likely that the assumption of a substantial general health benefit compared to their progenitor breeds is overestimated. Paradoxically, unless the new designer dog breeds are recognised or registered by the national kennel clubs or some other canine organisations, their health will not be systematically monitored, and they will remain outside of health programmes currently used by the organised dog world. However, coming under the umbrella of national kennel clubs could increase these health issues over time in designer breeds as a result of inbreeding, absence of selection against disease predisposing phenotypes and genetic variants and exaggeration towards extreme conformation, just as has happened in many of the traditional breeds.
Mixed-breed dogs with unknown genetic background
Health and longevity
Prior to the emergence of designer breeds, a mixed breed would typically have been the result of casual or unintentional mating between dogs of different breeds or even dogs that were themselves already mixed breeds. Such mixed-breed dogs remain common in most general dog populations. A study of over 2 million dogs under UK veterinary care in 2019 revealed that approximately every fourth dog (24%) was classified as non-designer-crossbred (O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, McMillan, Church and Brodbelt2023). The equivalent figure from the Danish Dog Registry is 15% (Danish Dog Registry 2024).
As with designer breeds, dogs that are non-designer, mixed breed continue to have their advocates, and it is often the potential health benefits from crossbreeding that are highlighted. Several studies have analysed differences in general health between purebred and mixed-breed dogs. In England, data from electronic patient records were used to compare the prevalence of common disorders between purebred and mixed-breed dogs attending primary-care veterinary practices (O′Neill et al. Reference O′Neill, Church, McGreevy, Thomson and Brodbelt2014). Among the twenty most-frequently recorded disorders, purebred dogs had a significantly higher prevalence compared with crossbreds for otitis externa, obesity, and skin lumps. Overall, the study concluded that purebreds showed significantly higher prevalence values for 13 of the 84 (15.5%) disorders but substantial variation was shown across the pure breeds so that it was hard to disentangle the effects of pure breeding per se from the effects of being one of the many individual pure breeds. No instances were identified in which disorder prevalence values were significantly higher in mixed breeds than in purebred dogs overall.
A study from the University of California-Davis evaluated electronic records of referral patients from the Veterinary Medical Teaching Hospital for selected disease categories, including cancer, cardiac disorders, endocrine disorders, and orthopaedic disorders (Bellumori et al. Reference Bellumori, Famula, Bannasch, Belanger and Oberbauer2013). From a total of 24 disorders assessed, 13 did not show a significant difference in risk between purebred and mixed-breed dogs after matching the two populations for age, sex, and bodyweight. Ten disorders were more prevalent in purebred dogs compared with mixed-breed dogs, including aortic stenosis, dilated cardiomyopathy, hypothyroidism, elbow dysplasia and intervertebral disc disease. Mixed-breed dogs were, on the other hand, more prone to cranial cruciate ligament rupture (CCLR) and trauma from car accidents than purebred dogs. Since the latter injuries would be likely to reflect differences in levels of physical exercise and human supervision, it is probable that their higher prevalence in mixed-breed dogs indicates more laissez faire owner attitudes rather than any innate predispositions of the dogs, although breed-related predisposition to CCLR and putative genetic components have been demonstrated (Baird et al. Reference Baird, Carter, Innes, Ollier and Short2014).
A study of owner-reported survey data collected through the Dog Aging Project (DAP) Health and Life Experience Survey for 27,541 companion dogs concluded that, although individual breeds may show higher lifetime prevalence for specific conditions, purebred dogs did not show higher lifetime prevalence of medical conditions compared to mixed-breed dogs, and a higher proportion of purebred dogs than mixed-breed dogs had no owner-reported medical conditions (Forsyth et al. Reference Forsyth, McCoy, Schmid, Promislow and Snyder-Mackler2023).
Recent developments in DNA technology have made it possible to look beyond just clinical health and to study specific disease variants implicated in Mendelian disorders. A recent Finnish study screened around one million dogs for 250 genetic variants across 150 countries and reported that 87.9% of the variants were found in both purebred and mixed-breed dogs (Donner et al. Reference Donner, Freyer, Davison, Anderson, Blades, Honkanen, Inman, Brookhart-Knox, Louviere, Forman and Chodroff Foran2023). Mixed-breed dogs were more likely to be heterozygote carriers of common recessive diseases, whereas purebreds were more likely to be genetically affected (homozygous) but 57% of all dogs in the study carried at least one copy of a known disease-associated variant. The study was a follow-up from a similar survey in 2018 which concluded that the allele frequencies for the most frequent disease variants are essentially the same in purebred and mixed-breed dogs, emphasising their common genetic background and shared inherited disease variants (Donner et al. Reference Donner, Anderson, Davison, Hughes, Bouirmane, Lindqvist, Lytle, Ganesan, Ottka, Ruotanen, Kaukonen, Forman, Fretwell, Cole and Lohi2018).
Average longevity is often used as a proxy scale for overall health, and several studies have reported on longevity for different breeds and mixed breeds. Research on dog longevity has primarily reported average ages at death based on data from veterinary practice (Fleming et al. Reference Fleming, Creevy and Promislow2011; O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, Church, McGreevy, Thomson and Brodbelt2013), owner questionnaires (Proschowsky et al. Reference Proschowsky, Rugbjerg and Ersbøll2003; Lewis et al. Reference Lewis, Wiles, Llewellyn-Zaidi, Evans and O’Neill2018), health insurance databases (Bonnett & Egenvall Reference Bonnett and Egenvall2010) or combined data sources (McMillan et al. Reference McMillan, Bielby, Williams, Upjohn, Casey and Christley2024).
In most studies, the average longevity differs significantly between breeds. Mixed breeds usually end up at the higher end of the spectrum but are surpassed by some purebreds such as terriers and poodles (Proschowsky et al. Reference Proschowsky, Rugbjerg and Ersbøll2003; Lewis et al. Reference Lewis, Wiles, Llewellyn-Zaidi, Evans and O’Neill2018). A recent study applied a different approach and calculated life tables for 30,563 dogs in the UK (Teng et al. Reference Teng, Brodbelt, Pegram, Church and O’Neill2022). Life expectancy tables express the remaining average life expectancy from different ages and provide more information than just the average age at death (i.e. average longevity) across all ages. At age 0, the life expectancy for all dogs in the dataset was 11.23, but life expectancy tables varied from 12.72 years in Jack Russell terriers to 4.53 years in French bulldogs. The overall category of mixed breeds had a life expectancy of 11.82 at age 0. A study based on combined data from breed registries, veterinary corporations, pet insurance companies, animal welfare charities, and academic institutions found that in comparison with the crossbred group, 47.1% of the pure breeds presented longer median survival estimates, 25.8% presented shorter, and 27.1% did not vary significantly from the crossbred group (McMillan et al. Reference McMillan, Bielby, Williams, Upjohn, Casey and Christley2024).
Dogs belonging to large-bodied breeds are reported to generally have shorter average lifespans than dogs from physically smaller breeds (Galis et al. Reference Galis, Van Der Sluijs, Van Dooren, Metz and Nussbaumer2007; Greer et al. Reference Greer, Canterberry and Murphy2007). A negative correlation between longevity and body size was demonstrated in a study of 44,363 dogs representing 134 breeds. However, very small dogs seemed to have a reduced lifespan indicating that the correlation is non-linear and may reflect the effects of extreme conformation at either end of the dog size scale (Galis et al. Reference Galis, Van Der Sluijs, Van Dooren, Metz and Nussbaumer2007). This shortened life effect on extremely small breeds was also demonstrated in the life expectancy table study in which breeds such as the Chihuahua had a life expectancy at age 0 of only 7.91 (Teng et al. Reference Teng, Brodbelt, Pegram, Church and O’Neill2022).
Studies of lifespan and longevity, like the ones presented above, usually evaluate mixed breeds as a single group in the breed-based analyses. The main reason for this can be lack of data regarding size, bodyweight or the progenitor breeds constituting the individual mixed-breed dogs. It is, however, excessively simplistic to consider all mixed-breed dogs as one common group. For example, in many parts of Northern Europe, purebred dogs commonly comprise up to 70–80% of the overall population (O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, McMillan, Church and Brodbelt2023; TASSO 2023; DKC 2024b; KC 2024). Here, practically no truly outbred dogs exist, and most mixed-breed dogs are likely to have identifiable breeds among their closest ancestors.
The finding that some pure breeds outlived mixed breeds overall highlights the great variation between the longevities of the longest- and the shortest-lived pure breeds and suggests that the longevity of any one mixed-breed dog may be highly contingent on whether the parent breeds belonged to short- or long-lived breeds and on the disease-related cause of shortened longevity. Or, to put it in other words: there may be no such thing as the mixed breed but instead there are many different mixed breeds whose health and longevity will depend heavily on that of the (often unknown) progenitor breeds.
Behaviour in purebred and mixed-breed dogs – is the latter less predictable?
It is widely acknowledged that cohabitation between dogs and humans does not always run smoothly, and some dogs develop behaviours that owners may find problematic. Several studies using prevalence data have identified higher levels of reported problematic behaviours in mixed breeds compared to purebreds. Mixed-breed dogs are, for instance, reported to be more nervous, more excitable, and to exhibit excessive barking more frequently (Bennett et al. Reference Bennett, Cooper, Rohlf and Mornement2007), to be ranked higher for different kinds of aggression (Hsu & Sun Reference Hsu and Sun2010) and to be at increased risk of developing noise phobia (Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Bradshaw and Casey2013).
Modern DNA technology can reveal insights into which breeds may have contributed to the genetic profile of an individual mixed breed dog and, despite debate about the accuracy of these tests, the Wisdom Panel homepage claims that their DNA test is able to explain: “why your dog has to herd your whole family after dinner, loves chasing bunnies or hates baths” (Wisdom Panel Reference Panel2024). The implicit message here is that much of the behaviour expressed by dogs is determined by their breed affiliation. This argument is routinely used by the organised dog world to promote acquisition of a pedigree dog instead of a designer-crossbred or mixed breed or even a purebred without a pedigree. However, although the genetic basis for physical traits like body size (Sutter et al. Reference Sutter, Bustamante, Chase, Gray, Zhao, Zhu, Padhukasahasram, Karlins, Davis, Jones, Quignon, Johnson, Parker, Fretwell, Mosher, Lawler, Satyaraj, Nordborg, Lark, Wayne and Ostrander2007) and fur type (Cadieu et al. Reference Cadieu, Neff, Quignon, Walsh, Chase, Parker, VonHoldt, Rhue, Boyko, Byers, Wong, Mosher, Elkahloun, Spady, André, Lark, Cargill, Bustamante, Wayne and Ostrander2009) is largely unchallenged, the extent to which behaviour is determined by genetics continues to be debated.
A much-publicised 2022 study questioned the hereditary basis of dog behaviour by exploring correlations between breed characteristics and DNA-sequencing results from more than 2,000 purebred and mixed-breed dogs (Morrill et al. Reference Morrill, Hekman, Li, McClure, Logan, Goodman, Gao, Dong, Alonso, Carmichael, Snyder-Mackler, Alonso, Noh, Johnson, Koltookian, Lieu, Megquier, Swofford, Turner-Maier, White, Weng, Colubri, Genereux, Lord and Karlsson2022). The authors reported that even though most behavioural traits are heritable to some extent, behaviour itself could not reliably differentiate between breeds, with breed itself explaining only 9% of variation in behaviour. So, contrary to the widespread beliefs about the importance of choosing a dog breed based on breed-related behaviours that will fit your needs and everyday circumstances, this study concluded that “dog breed is generally a poor predictor of individual behaviour and should not be used to inform decisions relating to selection of a pet dog”. The authors claim that the modern domestic dog is a recent invention defined by arbitrary rules regarding physical appearance and purity of ‘bloodlines’ rather than behaviour and that this modern breeding approach is widely detached from the pre-Victorian lineages of dogs that were selected for functional roles such as hunting, guarding, and herding that were largely predicated on predictable behaviours in these dogs.
A contradictory conclusion was drawn in a paper by Dutrow et al. (Reference Dutrow, Serpell and Ostrander2022), which identified genetic drivers of canine behaviour using data from more than 4,000 domestic, semi-feral and wild dogs, and phenotypic data from over 46,000 purebred dogs based on the Canine Behavioural Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ; Hsu & Serpell Reference Hsu and Serpell2003). The authors concluded that diversification of canine behavioural phenotypes predates the formation of modern breeds and identified ten major canine lineages each of which is associated with a unique repertoire of behavioural characteristics related to their historical functional roles (e.g. hunting, herding, scent trailing, etc) reflected in many modern breeds. A Finnish study examined environmental and demographic factors associated with seven personality traits in a survey of over 11,000 dogs and concluded that “a dog’s breed is not a predictor of its personality, but the probability of showing certain personality traits differs between breeds.” Thus, the breed of the dog was the most important determinant underlying personality differences (Salonen et al. Reference Salonen, Mikkola, Niskanen, Hakanen, Sulkama, Puurunen and Lohi2023).
A questionnaire study of 7,700 purebred dogs representing more than 200 breeds, and 7,691 mixed-breed dogs living in Germany reported that, according to their owners, mixed breeds were less calm, less sociable toward other dogs, and showed more problematic behaviour than purebreds (Turcsán et al. Reference Turcsán, Miklósi and Kubinyi2017). Arguing that factors like early socialisation and rearing environment may substantially impact canine behaviour, the authors re-analysed the dataset controlling for the distribution of the demographic and dog-housing factors and concluded that the lower sociability of mixed-breed dogs towards other dogs seemed to be an indirect result of the environmental differences between purebreds and mixed breeds.
The differing inferences from these various studies may reflect differences in the methods used to measure behaviour across breeds and breed types. In addition, most studies also acknowledge substantial within-breed variation when it comes to behaviour. Further research using robust measures of behaviour will be needed to determine the extent to which ‘breed’ is a reliable indicator of expected personality in purebred dogs compared with mixed breeds.
A new future for dog breeding?
The current strong evidence on the scale of reduced health and welfare linked to breed as a concept in dogs and specifically driven by reduced genetic diversity, absence of health-conscious selection against disease predisposing phenotypes and genetic variants, and breeding towards extreme conformation means that actions to redress these issues are urgently needed. This Horizon paper should be taken as both a clarion call for action as well as offering some options for meaningful change. At a time where ideals such as ‘diversity’, ‘naturalness’, and ‘respect for ethnic diversity’ are trending in the human world, it can be tempting to call for a knee-jerk shutting down of all kennel and breed clubs and for humanity to turn our backs on a system of organised dog breeding that has been failing dogs in many ways for over a century.
However, such a draconian approach is unlikely to solve the health and welfare problems of dogs. Newly invented designer breeds and mixed breeds are shown to have important health and behavioural problems too, and the predictability of purebred dogs to some degree with respect to body size, basic behaviours, known need for grooming, disorder profiles and other attributes may well offer some benefits for a mutually fulfilling human-dog relationship.
Some critics of purebred dogs demand an end to breeding and recommend that prospective dog owners rescue dogs from shelters either at home or abroad – the ‘Adopt, Don’t Shop’ mantra (Hanson Reference Hanson2016; Adopt Don’t Shop 2024). However, in many countries, rescue dogs can only satisfy a small fraction of the wider public demand for dogs. A study in Denmark found that around 2,000 dogs enter shelters each year while the annual number of dogs acquired in Denmark is over 60,000 (Sandøe et al. Reference Sandøe, Jensen, Jensen and Nielsen2019). This figure might look different in other countries, but the widespread keeping of dogs as pets means that in most places dogs will have to be bred in large numbers. So, stopping breeding is not the answer (Mäki Reference Mäki2023).
The way forward in terms of improving the breeding-related welfare of companion dogs seems to lie in the middle ground, where whatever future organised dog world exists then (i.e. updated versions of current kennel and breed clubs that have the health of dogs at their core or whatever organisations take their place) would move from the continuing obsession with prioritising the physical appearance of dogs to instead prioritising the overall health and welfare of the dogs under its remit. This would be in line with some current movement by some national kennel clubs and international organisations to publicly acknowledge that past processes prioritising closed breeding pools and promoting breed standards that encouraged extreme conformation no longer fit with the canine welfare understanding and demands of modern human society (FCI 2020). Such a new world would offer more transparent and evidence-based breeding systems that would reverse inbreeding effects and enforce a practice of healthy breeding with direct selection against disease-predisposing phenotypes and gene variants.
Retaining a system of organised dog breeding with formal registers into the future, in our opinion, offers two major advantages compared to uncontrolled dog breeding: traceability and transparency. For pedigree dogs, each puppy can be traced back to the parents and to the breeder who was responsible for producing the litter. Registration of breeding animals and their progeny in a central database of an organisation such as a kennel club – or even in international databases – enables more effective monitoring of results from health and breeding schemes, number of litters, levels of inbreeding etc, through several generations in a more transparent way.
Some countries have decided to focus on legislation as their primary tool to enforce human actions aimed at improving canine health and welfare. The design of these legislative actions currently differs widely between the countries. As much of the current legislation that can be applied to dog health and dog breeding is relatively new and these efforts to actively enforce legislative approaches are relatively recent, there is as yet insufficient evidence to determine the overall effectiveness of this legal approach to trigger positive and widespread change. Hopefully, reliable evaluation will emerge in the years to come to show some benefits to dog welfare. A major advantage from most forms of legislative action is that they, at least theoretically, apply to the entire population of dogs and not simply to pedigree dogs which is the case with breed-specific health schemes administered by the kennel clubs. Hitherto, a specific challenge has been the increasing importation of puppies from countries without regulation of canine breeding and welfare, but this can be expected to change when new EU legislation comes into force. According to a proposal from the EU Commission, imports will be subject to the same or equivalent standards (European Council 2024).
Most production animal breeding programmes today are based on genetic markers linked to desired traits – so-called genomic selection (Meuwissen et al. Reference Meuwissen, Hayes and Goddard2016) and selective breeding has clearly shown the potential for improvements in production and fertility traits with low heritability such as litter size in pigs (Rodenburg et al. Reference Rodenburg, Buitenhuis, Ask, Uitdehaag, Koene, Van Der Poel and Bovenhuis2003; Knap & Su Reference Knap and Su2008). In addition, digital tools have been shown to perform better than human observers when it comes to, for example, reliability of the evaluation of health and welfare in cattle and pigs (Benjamin & Yik Reference Benjamin, Yik, Benjamin and Yik2020; Chapa et al. Reference Chapa, Maschat, Iwersen, Baumgartner and Drillich2020; Bortoluzzi et al. Reference Bortoluzzi, Goering, Ochoa, Holliday, Mumm, Nelson, Wu, Mote, Psota, Schmidt, Jaberi-Douraki and Hulbert2023). Even though these tools may seem promising and theoretically have the power to be applied in dog breeding as well, several obstacles must be overcome before realistic implementation becomes possible for the breeding of companion dogs. These include, among others, definition, and agreement on evaluation parameters – some of which may differ from breed-to-breed – and registration of uniform, high quality phenotypes together with obtained genotype data.
Animal welfare implications
The challenge remains to breed dogs with better physical and mental health. In our opinion, it is now time for those currently in charge of organised dog breeding to take responsibility for this challenge and to put the health and welfare of the dogs ahead of human goals such as heeding tradition, profit, winning prizes at shows and other outcomes that often currently are at odds with the well-being of the dogs themselves. Amongst others, such changes to longstanding practices that are now sadly recognised to be harmful to canine health and welfare could include re-opening the breed registries, removing, or modifying wording from breed standards that currently promotes extreme conformation, maintaining a high effective population size and promoting diversity rather than uniformity within breeds or breed-like dog types. The changes need to be drastic and if those currently charged with responsibility for organised dog breeding feel unable to rise to this challenge, then perhaps it is time that they should step aside and let others who do prioritise the health and welfare of dogs to take the reins. Health and welfare rather than looks should become the new goal in dog breeding. In addition, the responsibility to adhere to breed-, breed type- or conformation-specific phenotypic and genotypic selection criteria for good health outcomes must be clear for every person who produces puppies whether they are purebreds, mixed breeds or designer breeds.
Acknowledgements
This paper originated as a keynote lecture at the 2023 Canine Science Forum in Budapest, given by Peter Sandøe. We are grateful to the organisers for the invitation and to the participants for good comments following the talk. Also, we are grateful for useful comments from colleagues, notably Roi Mandel Briefer, Stine B Christiansen, Björn Forkman and Iben Meyer. The study was supported via the Danish Centre for Companion Animal Welfare, of which Peter Sandøe is a leader. The Centre gets its core funding from the charitable foundation Skibsreder Per Henriksen, R og Hustrus Fond https://skibsrederperhenriksensfond.dk/. The funders had no role in the decision to publish, or in the preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interest
Helle Friis Proschowsky has been employed by, and still carries out tasks on a consultancy basis, for the Danish Kennel Club. Merete Fredholm is a member of the health board of the Danish Kennel Club. Dan O’Neill has received research funding from the UK Kennel Club Charitable Trust. Irena Czycholl has previously written an expert opinion for the German Kennel Club (VDH).