Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T04:16:13.823Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Less activity means improved welfare? How pair housing influences pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) behaviour

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2024

London M Wolff
Affiliation:
B83 East Stadium, Department of Psychology, Center for Brain, Biology & Behavior, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588, USA
Jeffrey R Stevens*
Affiliation:
B83 East Stadium, Department of Psychology, Center for Brain, Biology & Behavior, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588, USA
*
Corresponding author: Jeffrey R Stevens; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The activity level and specific behaviours exhibited by captive animals are crucial indicators of welfare. Stereotypies, or repetitive behaviours that have no apparent function or goal, are performed by animals experiencing poor conditions in their environment and indicate welfare concerns. Changes in the housing environment in particular may have critical influences on behaviour and welfare. Here, we measured behavioural changes in a captive pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) population (n = 10) associated with a shift from single to pair housing. Using automated video processing, we show that pair housing greatly reduced overall activity levels in these birds. The stark reduction in activity was surprising, as we expected that social housing would increase interactions between birds, thus increasing activity levels. Upon further analysis, however, we found that stereotypic behaviours, such as beak scraping, jumping, pecking, and route tracing decreased after pair housing, whereas the positive welfare behaviours of perching and preening increased. Our results indicate that pair housing may reduce overall activity in pinyon jays; however, this reduction is primarily in stereotypic behaviours.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Bird owners use changes in behaviour to track well-being in birds, and a dramatic decrease in activity levels can indicate welfare problems. But could decreased behaviour actually be a sign of lower stress? Currently, activity/movement are offered as proxies for the welfare of an animal, with more activity typically linked to improved welfare (Tahamtani et al. Reference Tahamtani, Pedersen and Riber2019; Woods et al. Reference Woods, Eyer and Miller2022). However, when interpreting reduced activity levels, activity quality or type is rarely considered, highlighting a potential confound: if the behaviours that result in activity are themselves signals of stress, lower activity levels may paradoxically indicate better welfare. In this paper, we provide evidence of how pair housing of a social bird species is associated with decreased activity, but that the source of this change is decreased stereotypic behaviours, reflecting better, not worse, welfare.

Relative to other populations, captive animals are more likely to exhibit stereotypic behaviours, or repetitive behaviours that have no apparent function or goal (Mason Reference Mason1991b). Stereotypies, sometimes referred to as abnormal repetitive behaviours, are performed by animals that have in the past or are currently experiencing poor conditions in their environment (Broom Reference Broom and Smidt1983; Mason Reference Mason1991a,Reference Mason, Clubb, Latham and Vickeryb; Mellor et al. Reference Mellor, Brilot and Collins2018). Millions of captive birds — whether kept for companionship, education, production, or research — exhibit stereotypic or other abnormal repetitive behaviours (Mason & Latham Reference Mason and Latham2004; Mason et al. Reference Mason, Clubb, Latham and Vickery2007). These statistics are alarming as these behaviours are known to indicate welfare concerns (Mason Reference Mason1991a,Reference Mason, Clubb, Latham and Vickeryb; Rose et al. Reference Rose, Nash and Riley2017). In birds, stereotypic behaviours include beak scraping, feather plucking, pecking, repetitive pacing, and route tracing (Garner et al. Reference Garner, Mason and Smith2003; Mellor et al. Reference Mellor, Brilot and Collins2018; Woods et al. Reference Woods, Eyer and Miller2022). Importantly, the presence of stereotypic behaviour can inform caretakers that welfare issues are a concern, but caution should be exercised when making causal assumptions, as there can be a lag between changes in the environment and the reduction of stereotypies if a reduction occurs at all.

Even though stereotypies are not causally interpretable, they typically indicate stress. It is therefore in the best interest of the animal for caretakers to apply stress-reducing strategies whenever stereotypies appear. Evidence-based solutions can help reduce or eliminate stereotypic behaviours, which is linked to welfare improvements (Mason & Latham Reference Mason and Latham2004; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Chadwick, Yon and Asher2018). The most widely used form of management to combat these abnormal behaviour patterns is environmental enrichment (Mason et al. Reference Mason, Clubb, Latham and Vickery2007), with a meta-analysis showing that out of 41 studies, approximately half found significant reductions in stereotypy following implementation of environmental enrichment (Swaisgood & Shepherdson Reference Swaisgood and Shepherdson2005). Other possible forms of intervention include punishment, genetic modification, and/or medication. However, these options do not address the underlying issues that cause stereotypies and, in some cases, may even increase or simply change the type of stereotypy an animal exhibits (Mason et al. Reference Mason, Clubb, Latham and Vickery2007). Without addressing the underlying issue of housing or husbandry deficits that cause the stress, reducing stereotypic behaviours themselves is not an ideal endpoint.

One of the main underlying issues of housing or husbandry deficits in social animals is a lack of social interaction. Housing social species individually may induce stress resulting in stereotypies due to a lack of access to environments that allow the normal and natural functioning of their behaviours (Broom Reference Broom and Smidt1983; Swaisgood & Shepherdson Reference Swaisgood and Shepherdson2005). This is especially true in social bird species where the connection between pairs of individuals is formed and strengthened through reciprocal preening and the exchange of food (Clayton & Emery Reference Clayton and Emery2007; Duque & Stevens Reference Duque and Stevens2016; Morales Picard et al. Reference Morales Picard, Mundry, Auersperg and Slocombe2020; Miller et al. Reference Miller, Schiestl, Clayton, Golledge and Richardson2024).

For many social species, pair or group housing is recommended (Hawkins Reference Hawkins, Duncan and Hawkins2010; Baumans & Van Loo Reference Baumans and Van Loo2013). In a highly social bird species, parrots, social enrichment helps reduce stereotypies (Meehan et al. Reference Meehan, Garner and Mench2003; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Hoppitt and Grant2017). In fact, 57% of single-housed parrots (Amazona amazonica) developed stereotypies in the first 12 months of being housed in isolation, while pair-housed parrots developed no stereotypies (Meehan et al. Reference Meehan, Garner and Mench2003). Yet the manner of pairing is also critical. Forced pairings of male and female partridges (Alectoris rufa) resulted in more aggression and injuries than when females were allowed to choose their partner (Prieto et al. Reference Prieto, Sánchez-García, Alonso, Rodríguez and Gaudioso2012). Thus, pair housing birds can confer benefits to welfare, but there are also welfare costs that require monitoring.

Present study

Corvids comprise a family of birds found worldwide that includes ravens, crows, magpies, and jays. Due to their sophisticated cognition and varied social structures and feeding ecologies, corvids are a popular study species in the field of animal behaviour and cognition (Balda & Kamil Reference Balda, Kamil, Bekoff, Allen and Burghardt2002; Clayton & Emery Reference Clayton and Emery2007). With a number of research teams around the world maintaining corvids in captivity to study their behaviour, understanding their welfare is critical to this enterprise (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Schiestl, Clayton, Golledge and Richardson2024).

Here, we investigate the effects of housing practices on welfare for pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus), a highly social corvid species that lives in mountainous regions of western North America (Marzluff & Balda Reference Marzluff and Balda1992; Balda & Kamil Reference Balda, Kamil, Balda, Pepperberg and Kamil1998). Pinyon jays live in flocks ranging from 50 to 500 birds and experience frequent changes in the size and composition of their social groups (Marzluff & Balda Reference Marzluff and Balda1992; Wiggins Reference Wiggins2005). They exhibit sophisticated social behaviours, such as inferring transitive social relationships (Paz-y-Miño-C et al. Reference Paz-y-Miño-C, Bond, Kamil and Balda2004), exhibiting social learning (Templeton et al. Reference Templeton, Kamil and Balda1999), food sharing (Duque & Stevens Reference Duque and Stevens2016), prosocial behaviour (Duque et al. Reference Duque, Leichner, Ahmann and Stevens2018), and attending to competitor behaviour (Vernouillet et al. Reference Vernouillet, Casidsid and Kelly2021).

Our colony of pinyon jays has historically been housed individually to maintain careful control of food intake (both in terms of food restriction and ensuring adequate access to food), which influences experimental motivation and performance as well as nutrition. However, given the need for social enrichment in corvids (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Schiestl, Clayton, Golledge and Richardson2024), we moved to pair house our birds. Pair housing balances the welfare needs of social housing with the logistic needs of ensuring food intake, easing the capture of birds for experimental sessions, and controlling exposure to conspecifics for social experiments. Pairs were fixed throughout this change to maintain consistent social relationships and to avoid confounding factors associated with multiple new partners. We therefore leveraged this change in housing to investigate the effects of different housing conditions on pinyon jay welfare as defined by activity and behaviour changes. We hypothesised that moving the birds to larger cages with a conspecific would result in more activity overall due to the new opportunity for social interactions.

Since manually observing and recording behaviours live is so time intensive (Rushen et al. Reference Rushen, Chapinal and de Passilé2012; Whitham & Miller Reference Whitham and Miller2016) and could disturb the birds if an observer is present, we video-recorded our pinyon jays before, during, and after they moved to new housing. We then employed automatic video analysis to quantify activity patterns by tracking pixel changes in the video images. After quantifying their overall activity, we then viewed the video and recorded specific behaviours that the birds exhibited, allowing us to map overall activity onto specific behaviours across the housing transition. We categorise those behaviours as positive or negative for welfare based on previous literature. Stereotypical/negative welfare behaviours in birds include beak scraping, pecking, repetitive jumping, and route tracing (Garner et al. Reference Garner, Mason and Smith2003; Mellor et al. Reference Mellor, Brilot and Collins2018; Woods et al. Reference Woods, Eyer and Miller2022); whereas foraging, perching, playing, and preening indicate positive welfare (Papageorgiou et al. Reference Papageorgiou, Goliomytis, Tzamaloukas, Miltiadou and Simitzis2023).

Materials and methods

Ethical approval

All procedures were conducted in an ethical and responsible manner and were approved by the University of Nebraska-Lincoln Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol number 2059), conforming to the ASAB/ABS Guidelines for the use of animals in research.

Study animals

Our study population included 12 (three female) pinyon jays. On moving day, two male birds were replaced with two other males from a different housing room due to unrelated husbandry concerns. As a result, we only focused on the ten birds that completed all phases of the study when measuring individual behaviour (Table 1).

Table 1. Individual subject information

* AZ = Arizona, CA = California

All birds were wild born, captured in either Arizona or California (United States Fish and Wildlife permit MB694205) between 2006 and 2011. At capture, they were estimated to be between one and three years of age. The birds in this study ranged in age from ten to 17 years with a mean of 13.1 years. During their time in the laboratory, all subjects experienced non-invasive cognitive and behavioural experiments and were handled by humans regularly. These experiments included studies of decision-making, numerical cognition, problem-solving, and social interactions (Paz-y-Miño-C et al. Reference Paz-y-Miño-C, Bond, Kamil and Balda2004; Bond et al. Reference Bond, Kamil and Balda2007; Duque & Stevens Reference Duque and Stevens2016; Stevens et al. Reference Stevens, Kennedy, Morales and Burks2016; Duque et al. Reference Duque, Leichner, Ahmann and Stevens2018; Wolff et al. Reference Wolff, Carey and Stevens2024).

Procedures

Housing

Data were collected over a three-week period between February 15th, 2021 and March 7th, 2021. During the first week, birds were kept in the single (0.10 m3) cages that they had been housed in upon entry to the colony (42 × 42 × 60 cm [length × width × height]; Figure 1[a]). After the first week, we moved each individual animal to their new larger cage (0.46 m3) with another bird (46 × 96 × 105 cm; Figure 1[b]). Birds were housed together based upon size, previous interactions, and sex. Where possible, we created female/male pairs as we have found that they are less likely to have behavioural issues together. The pairs were fixed throughout the study period. We label the first week as the pre-move phase, the second week as the during-move phase, and the third week as the post-move phase.

On moving day (February 22nd, 2021), the pinyon jays were placed on either side of the new cage with a divider in place to allow for the animals to acclimate to each other. After an hour of acclimation, the dividers were removed. Laboratory staff then watched the pairs continuously for the following 20 min and periodically for a further 2 h to ensure that no animals exhibited aggression or stress. As there was no evidence of negative interactions during this observation period, birds were allowed to remain with their original partners. Of the five pairs created, three were male/female and two were male/male. No enrichment was provided to the birds during the three phases of this study to prevent movement from objects being mistakenly recorded as bird movement. Following this study, we implemented enrichment by rotating through various toys and foraging tasks.

Recording

We conducted 15-min video recordings of subjects in their home cage during the three-week study period 2–5 times per day (mean: 3.7× per day) between 0900 and 1700h CST. All recording occurred during the light phase within the rooms with a 14:10 h light:dark cycle. In the first week of recording, the animals resided in their original single housing, whereas in the subsequent two weeks, they were in the new pair housing. Three days prior to the first recording, we habituated the birds to the presence of a tripod and blue-coloured tape markings on the floor to signal the tripod’s location.

For recording sessions, an experimenter placed the camera (GoPro HERO9 Black; GoPro, San Mateo, CA, USA) on the tripod, turned on the camera, and left the room. No one entered the room during recording sessions. After 15 min had elapsed, the experimenter re-entered the room, turned off and removed the camera (leaving the tripod), and stored the video recordings. For pair housing, the tripod was adjusted to account for the new height of the paired cages; there were no other changes made to the recording protocol.

Video processing and analysis

Activity levels

To quantify the amount of activity, we used a MATLAB script that calculated the sum of pixel changes across successive frames using the estimateFlow() function from the Computer Vision Toolbox®. The code started analysing frames 45 s into each video (to eliminate extraneous movement from the birds reacting to the experimenter turning on the camera) and ran until 10 min of video had elapsed. Three videos were removed from the analysis due to staff entering the housing room during recording. In total, 74 videos with 10 min of footage were used in the activity level analysis.

Behaviour data collection

To further investigate how specific behaviours changed over the three weeks, we coded the birds’ behaviours during week one and three. The first author (LW) created an ethogram of 18 behaviours that were present during the recordings: beak scraping, drinking, feeding, flapping, foraging, head through bars, hopping, jumping, laying down, other, out of view, pecking, perching, playing, preening, route tracing, standing, and walking (for behaviour definitions, see Table 2).

Table 2. Ethogram of pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) behaviours used to code video

Table used with permission under a CC-BY 4.0 licence: Wolff and Stevens (Reference Wolff, Carey and Stevens2024); available at https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.02.07.579343.

In the post-move phase two birds were present per cage (Figure 1). As individuals were difficult to identify, it was not possible to tell which of the paired birds were performing a specific behaviour. Therefore, we only coded whether a behaviour was present in either bird in a cage. To stay consistent across the phases, we also combined both of the birds that would eventually be housed together when coding pre-move phase video data. That is, we combine the behavioural data for each pair throughout both phases. Additionally, our analysis was limited to ten out of the 12 birds as only ten birds remained unchanged across phases.

Figure 1. Camera screenshots of pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) in their cages showing (a) single and (b) pair housing. Figure used with permission under a CC-BY 4.0 licence: Wolff and Stevens (Reference Stevens2024), available at https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.02.07.579343.

For the behavioural analyses, we trimmed the videos to 10 min to match the activity level data. We then sampled a 10-s clip per minute per video. The first sample began at the 45-s mark and ended at the 55-s mark. The second sample began at 1 min 45 s and so on, until 9 min 45 s. We coded 20 recordings from the pre-move phase and 14 recordings from the post-move phase (we did not code any recordings from the during-move phase). We coded ten samples per pair per video, resulting in a total of 1,700 samples.

For each of the 18 behaviours on the ethogram, coders recorded the number of times that either bird in a pair exhibited each behaviour within every sample. Three coders coded the 1,700 samples. To ensure inter-rater reliability, prior to coding the full set, the three coders scored a test set of four videos. LW was aware of the response variable, but the other two coders were unaware. After training on the ethogram and common issues in coding, each coder received the same randomised subset of four videos to code. We calculated the intra-class correlation of their coded responses using a two-way random effects model for the average of three coders (ICC2k). Based on interpretations from Koo and Li (Reference Koo and Li2016), the intra-class correlation demonstrated good reliability between raters (0.89). To score the full set of videos for analysis, the two unaware coders each scored half of the remaining videos.

Data analysis

We used R (Version 4.4.0; R Core Team 2024) and the R-packages BayesFactor (Version 0.9.12.4.7; Morey & Rouder Reference Morey and Rouder2024), cocoon (Version 0.0.0.9000; Stevens Reference Stevens2024), easystats (Version 0.7.1; Lüdecke et al. Reference Lüdecke, Ben-Shachar, Patil and Makowski2022), here (Version 1.0.1; Müller Reference Müller2020), lme4 (Version 1.1.35.3; Bates et al. Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015), papaja (Version 0.1.2; Aust & Barth Reference Aust and Barth2023), patchwork (Version 1.2.0; Pedersen Reference Pedersen2024), psych (Version 2.4.3; Revelle Reference Revelle2024), and tidyverse (Version 2.0.0; Wickham et al. Reference Wickham, Averick, Bryan and Yutani2019) for our analyses. The manuscript was created using knitr (Version 1.46; Xie Reference Xie2015), kableExtra (Version 1.3.4.9000; Zhu Reference Zhu2023), rmarkdown (Version 2.27; Xie et al. Reference Xie, Allaire and Grolemund2018), and papaja (Version 0.1.2; Aust & Barth Reference Aust and Barth2023). Data, analysis scripts, supplementary materials, and reproducible research materials are available at the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/v9r6q/).

Though we present both Bayesian and frequentist statistics (i.e. P-values), we draw inferences based on Bayes factors because they offer bidirectional information about evidence supporting both the alternative (H1) and the null (H0) hypotheses. Bayes factors provide the ratio of evidence for H1 over evidence for H0 (Wagenmakers Reference Wagenmakers2007; Wagenmakers et al. Reference Wagenmakers, Lodewyckx, Kuriyal and Grasman2010). Therefore, a Bayes factor of 3 (BF 10 = 3) indicates three times more evidence for H1 than H0, whereas a Bayes factor of 1/3 (the reciprocal of 3) indicates three times more evidence for H0 than H1. We interpret Bayes factors based on Wagenmakers et al. (Reference Wagenmakers, Marsman, Jamil and Morey2018), where a BF 10 > 3 is considered sufficient evidence for the alternative hypothesis, BF 10 < 1/3 is considered sufficient evidence for the null hypothesis, and 1/3 < BF 10 < 3 indicate neither hypothesis has evidence supporting it (suggesting the sample size is too small to draw conclusions).

Activity levels

We estimated our response variable of activity level by calculating a mean number of pixel changes between video frames. To test the change in activity level over the different phases, we used model selection on linear models calculated with the lm() function. We then derived Bayes factors for comparing models from model BIC values using the test_performance() function from the performance package (Lüdecke et al. Reference Lüdecke, Ben-Shachar, Patil, Waggoner and Makowski2021). This approach implicitly assumes a unit information prior. Although we were primarily interested in the effect of phase on activity level, we also included time of day as a potential factor since activity may vary throughout the day. Therefore, we compared five models: (1) an intercept only model lm(activity ~ 1); (2) a phase only model lm(activity ~ phase); (3) a time of day only model lm(activity ~ timeofday); (4) a phase and time of day with no interaction model lm(activity ~ phase + timeofday); and (5) a phase and time of day with interaction model lm(activity ~ phase + timeofday) (Table 3). We calculated Bayes factors comparing each of the models with factors (models 2–4) to the intercept only model (1). We considered the model with the highest Bayes factor as the best fitting model.

Table 3. Model comparison for effect of phase and time of day on pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) activity level

AIC = Akaike Information Criterion, BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion, BF = Bayes factor

Behaviour data

For behaviour data, we calculated the mean frequency of each behaviour per pair for both the pre- and post-move phases. We then conducted frequentist and Bayesian paired t-tests to compare behaviour frequency across phases. For the Bayesian t-tests, we employed the ttestBF() function from the BayesFactor R package (Morey & Rouder Reference Morey and Rouder2024) using default priors (Cauchy distributions for effect sizes and noninformative/uniform distributions for variance).

Results

Activity levels

Figure 2(a) shows the range of activity levels across time of day for the three phases. Our comparisons of models (Table 3) showed that the phase only model performed best. The phase only model had the highest Bayes factor (BF 10 = 2.9 × 1023) compared to the time of day only model (BF 10 = 0.14) and the phase and time of day model (BF 10 = 4.4 × 1022). In fact, there was 6.5 times ore evidence favouring the phase only model over the next best (phase and time of day) model. Therefore, phase was an important predictor of activity levels, but time of day was not.

Figure 2. Activity levels of pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) before, during, and after moving from individual to pair housing (ten birds groups into five pairs). (a) Mean activity levels per sample across time of day for each phase. Points represent mean levels per individual video recording with phase indicated by colour and symbol. (b) Mean activity levels per sample across date. Points present mean levels averaged over dates with phase indicated by colour and symbol. Dots represent estimated marginal means per phase, and error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Figure used with permission under a CC-BY 4.0 licence: Wolff and Stevens (Reference Wolff and Stevens2024); available at https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.02.07.579343.

Since phase was important in predicting activity, we computed pair-wise contrasts for the different phases. These contrasts suggest that activity during the pre-move phase was substantially higher than both the during-move phase (mean difference = 4.15, t(71) = 16.1; P < 0.001, d = 3.8, BF 10 = 3.0 × 1020) and post-move phase (mean difference = 3.34, t(71) = 11.4; P < 0.001, d = 2.7, BF 10 = 1.2 × 1010). Further, activity levels increased slightly between the last two phases (mean difference = –0.81, t(71) = –2.9; P = 0.004, d = –0.7, BF 10 = 7.3). Thus, changing housing greatly reduced overall activity levels (Figure 2[b]).

Behaviour

The stark reduction in activity was surprising, as we expected that social housing would increase interactions between birds, thus increasing activity levels. After uncovering this finding, we investigated the exploratory hypothesis that reduction in activity was driven by reductions in stereotypic behaviours. Figure 3 shows the mean frequencies for all of the behaviours, along with Bayes factors and P-values for the paired t-tests comparing frequencies in the pre- and post-move phases. Of the 18 behaviours, we observed a decrease in beak scraping, feeding, foraging, jumping, pecking, playing, route tracing, and walking. We observed an increase in perching and preening. We did not have enough evidence to detect differences or lack of differences in drinking, flapping, head through bar, hopping, laying down, or standing. We omitted analysis of behaviours other and out of view since they are not specific behaviours of interest.

Figure 3. Mean frequencies of behaviours in the pre- and post-move phases for pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) (ten birds in five pairs). The behaviors other and out of view are omitted. Grey lines connect means for each of the five bird pairs. Dots represent overall means per phase, and error bars represent within-pair 95% confidence intervals. Figure used with permission under a CC-BY 4.0 licence: Wolff and Stevens (Reference Stevens2024); available at https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.02.07.579343.

Weight

Throughout the study, we weighed our birds several times a week, so we could investigate the effects of the housing transition on this important measure of well-being (Labocha & Hayes Reference Labocha and Hayes2012). Group housing in other wild-caught species has been shown to increase weight compared to individual housing (McLeod et al. Reference McLeod, Thompson, Crawford and Shackell1997). Bird weights increased from the pre-move phase (M = 98.6 g, 95% CI [96.9, 100.3]) to the post-move phase (M = 100.2 g, 95% CI [98.8, 101.5]). A linear model with weights as the response variable and phase as a predictor outperformed an intercept-only model (BF 10 = 148.4), indicating that phase influenced weight. Whether this weight increase was due to social facilitation or other aspects of pair housing is unclear. Regardless, moving to pair housing increased the birds’ weights, suggesting that their welfare improved.

Discussion

We examined behavioural changes in pinyon jays during two husbandry interventions of a larger cage and pair housing. After the housing change, birds decreased their activity levels as measured by overall pixel changes during video recording. This dramatic drop in activity was surprising and motivated a more extensive follow-up analysis examining the frequencies of specific behaviours. This exploratory analysis indicated that perching and preening (considered positive welfare behaviours; Papageorgiou et al. Reference Papageorgiou, Goliomytis, Tzamaloukas, Miltiadou and Simitzis2023) increased in frequency after the cage change. In contrast, beak scraping, feeding, foraging, jumping, pecking, playing, route tracing, and walking decreased after the cage change. This reduction includes both stereotypic (beak scraping, jumping, pecking, route tracing) and positive behaviours (feeding, foraging, playing). Bird weights also increased following the housing change. Thus, moving to pair housing substantially altered behaviour in the pinyon jays with benefits to their welfare via reduced stereotypies, increased positive welfare behaviours, and increased weight.

The growing prevalence of automated behaviour assessment systems, such as video recording, accelerometers, and GPS devices can facilitate the large-scale collection of activity data (Rushen et al. Reference Rushen, Chapinal and de Passilé2012; Whitham & Miller Reference Whitham and Miller2016). However, researchers and animal caretakers must be mindful that overall patterns of activity do not necessarily provide a complete assessment of welfare. Measuring specific behaviours associated with stress and calm are critical to assessing welfare and formulating care plans. It is imperative to recognise that when employing activity measures as an indicator of welfare in captive animals, the absence of certain behaviours is not inherently problematic. Automated processes can be useful in assessing animal welfare, and improvements in technology such as computer vision may allow currently infeasible automation such as classifying and tracking individual behaviours. However, we argue that human observers provide an invaluable perspective on the welfare of captive animals.

Study limitations

Despite our data providing intriguing insights into the effects of housing changes on captive bird welfare, we note several limitations of our study. First, this study involves a relatively small population of ten birds. Of course, individual differences are a critical component of animal behaviour and welfare (Stamps et al. Reference Stamps, Briffa and Biro2012; Dingemanse & Wolf Reference Dingemanse and Wolf2013; Richter & Hintze Reference Richter and Hintze2019). Interestingly, although some of the behaviours that we scored displayed a degree of variability, others remained relatively consistent. Beak scraping, foraging, jumping, and play all showed both consistent frequencies before the housing change and consistent drops in frequencies after the change. Other behaviours, such as foraging, pecking, perching, route tracing, and walking showed variability in the initial frequencies but consistent decreases (or increases) after the housing change. Thus, despite a relatively small sample size, most of our behavioural measures show consistent patterns across individuals. Moreover, the logistics of viewing videos of birds in pair housing did not enable us to identify and attribute behaviours to specific birds. Instead, behaviours were coded across bird pairs. Our findings are therefore limited to generalisations across pairs, rather than specific behavioural changes by a given individual. Larger samples with individually identifiable subjects would provide more confidence regarding the generalisability of results.

A second limitation is the advanced age of our birds (ten to 17 years old). There is very little work on behavioural changes that occur as birds age and none on age-related behavioural change in pinyon jays. What is considered an ‘older’ bird varies greatly within the existing literature (Collias et al. Reference Collias, Collias, Jacobs, Cox and McAlary1986; Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Davis, Jenkins and Carroll2004; Angelier et al. Reference Angelier, Weimerskirch, Dano and Chastel2007; Class et al. Reference Class, Brommer and van Oers2019). One study found that older passerine birds rested significantly more than young adults (Collias et al. Reference Collias, Collias, Jacobs, Cox and McAlary1986). Thus, the older age of our birds might have resulted in more resting and less active behaviours, which could have reduced the effects of pair housing on more active behaviours. Also, younger birds show differing abilities in adapting to novel changes in their environments (Greenberg Reference Greenberg, Reader and Laland2003). But this does not translate to older animals necessarily being more or less adaptive than younger ones (Dagg Reference Dagg2009). Therefore, more research is needed to understand the interaction between age, housing, and welfare.

It is possible that the reduction in activity and behaviours in our birds could have been an adverse reaction to the changes in housing. The lack of movement and increased perching could indicate more of a ‘freezing’ response to the stress of the change. While this is possible, the increase in preening indicates greater comfort with their surroundings (Papageorgiou et al. Reference Papageorgiou, Goliomytis, Tzamaloukas, Miltiadou and Simitzis2023). However, replicating this work with a larger sample size, a more diverse age range of birds, and perhaps more physiological measures of stress (e.g. cortisol, heart-rate levels) could clarify the effects of pair housing on bird welfare.

Finally, we only recorded behaviour for two weeks after the housing change. Alhough it was a small difference, activity levels in the third week increased compared to the second week. It is possible that the activity levels would have continued to increase over time. Therefore, we cannot claim that the behavioural differences observed here represent a sustained or permanent change in behaviour. Rather, we can only offer a snapshot in time that needs longer-term studies to determine if these activity patterns remain consistent as the pairs become more acquainted.

Animal welfare implications

This study highlights a crucial distinction in the assessment of captive animal welfare: less activity does not necessarily imply poor welfare or increased stress. Rather, it is one facet of animal behaviour that must be examined when determining animal welfare. Our data in particular show that moving from single to pair housing can result in an overall reduction in activity. Yet that reduction does not occur uniformly across all behaviours. Our birds demonstrated reductions in stereotypic behaviours associated with stress, such as beak scraping, jumping, pecking, and route tracing. Therefore, the pair housing seems to have reduced these repetitive behaviours. However, it also decreased seemingly positive behaviours, such as foraging, playing, and walking. These behaviours might have decreased because the social enrichment associated with pair housing substituted for other forms of physical enrichment that the birds engaged in to maintain their own psychological welfare. Having a social partner present may have replaced the need to engage in these other activities. We also observed an increase in preening and perching. These behaviours may indicate a reduction in stress, where the animals feel comfortable enough in their environment that they can rest calmly and engage in self-care. However, there is less research on behaviours associated with improved welfare, so our subjective interpretations of these behaviours as positive may be biased. Overall, the move from single to pair housing seems to have reduced stress-related behaviours and potentially increased calming behaviours.

Conclusion

This research investigated how pinyon jays showed paradoxically lower activity levels after moving from single to pair housing. Upon subsequent video analysis we found that the stereotypic behaviours of beak scraping, jumping, pecking, and route tracing decreased after pair housing, whereas a calming behaviour — preening — increased. Our findings suggest that pairing pinyon jays may decrease their overall activity, but this decrease is mainly observed in stereotypical behaviours. Further research is needed to determine whether this reduction in activity is sustained over time following initiation of pair housing.

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by a US National Science Foundation grant (NSF-1658837). We would like to thank our amazing lab managers, Kylie Hughes and Katie Carey for collecting data and helping the lab run smoothly and our research assistants, Toria Biancalana, Bailey Wilson, and Hailey Wilson for collecting data and Rachel Bruner and Isaac Martinez for helping code the behavioural video data. We are grateful to Tierney Lorenz for comments on an early version of the manuscript.

Competing interest

The funder of this work (US National Science Foundation) played no role in the study design, collection, analysis, and interpretation of data. Nor in the writing of the paper and/or the decision to submit to Animal Welfare.

Data availability

The data and analysis code are available at: https://osf.io/v9r6q/.

Footnotes

Author contributions: LW: Conceptualisation, Methodology, Validation, Investigation, Data Curation, Writing – Original Draft, Writing-review & editing, Supervision, Project administration; JS: Conceptualisation, Methodology, Software, Formal Analysis, Resources, Data Curation, Writing – Original Draft, Writing-review & editing, Visualisation, Supervision, Funding acquisition

References

Anderson, KE, Davis, GS, Jenkins, PK, and Carroll, AS 2004 Effects of bird age, density, and molt on behavioral profiles of two commercial layer strains in cages. Poultry Science 83: 1523. https://doi.org/10.1093/ps/83.1.15CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angelier, F, Weimerskirch, H, Dano, S, and Chastel, O 2007 Age, experience and reproductive performance in a long-lived bird: A hormonal perspective. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61: 611621. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0290-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aust, F and Barth, M 2023 papaja: Prepare reproducible APA journal articles with R Markdown. https://github.com/crsh/papaja (accessed 5/6/24).Google Scholar
Balda, RP and Kamil, AC 1998 The ecology and evolution of spatial memory in corvids of the southwestern USA: The perplexing pinyon jay. In: Balda, RP, Pepperberg, IM and Kamil, AC (eds) Animal Cognition in Nature: The Convergence of Psychology and Biology in Laboratory and Field pp 2964. Academic Press: London, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balda, RP and Kamil, AC 2002 Spatial and social cognition in corvids: An evolutionary approach. In: Bekoff, M, Allen, C and Burghardt, GM (eds) The Cognitive Animal: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives on Animal Cognition pp 129134. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D, Mächler, M, Bolker, B and Walker, S 2015 Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67: 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumans, V and Van Loo, PLP 2013 How to improve housing conditions of laboratory animals: The possibilities of environmental refinement. The Veterinary Journal 195: 2432. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2012.09.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bond, AB, Kamil, AC, and Balda, RP 2007 Serial reversal learning and the evolution of behavioral flexibility in three species of North American corvids (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Nucifraga columbiana, Aphelocoma californica). Journal of Comparative Psychology 121: 372379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broom, DM 1983 Stereotypies as animal welfare indicators. In: Smidt, D (ed) Indicators Relevant to Farm Animal Welfare pp 8187. Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-6738-0_11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Class, B, Brommer, JE and van Oers, K 2019 Exploratory behavior undergoes genotype–age interactions in a wild bird. Ecology and Evolution 9: 89878994. https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.5430CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, NS and Emery, NJ 2007 The social life of corvids. Current Biology 17: R652R656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.070CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collias, NE, Collias, EC, Jacobs, CH, Cox, CR, and McAlary, FA 1986 Old age and breeding behavior in a tropical passerine bird Ploceus cucullatus under controlled conditions. The Auk 103: 408419. https://doi.org/10.1093/auk/103.2.408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dagg, AI 2009 The Social Behavior of Older Animals. Johns Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, US.Google Scholar
Dingemanse, NJ and Wolf, M 2013 Between-individual differences in behavioural plasticity within populations: Causes and consequences. Animal Behaviour 85: 10311039. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duque, JF, Leichner, W, Ahmann, H and Stevens, JR 2018 Mesotocin influences pinyon jay prosociality. Biology Letters 14: 20180105. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2018.0105CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duque, JF and Stevens, JR 2016 Voluntary food sharing in pinyon jays: The role of reciprocity and dominance. Animal Behaviour 122: 135144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.09.020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garner, JP, Mason, GJ and Smith, R 2003 Stereotypic route-tracing in experimentally caged songbirds correlates with general behavioural disinhibition. Animal Behaviour 66: 711727. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.2254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, R 2003 The role of neophobia and neophilia in the development of innovative behaviour of birds. In: Reader, SM and Laland, KN (eds) Animal Innovation pp 175196. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198526223.003.0008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, P 2010 The welfare implications of housing captive wild and domesticated birds. In: Duncan, IJH and Hawkins, P (eds) The Welfare of Domestic Fowl and Other Captive Birds pp 53102. Springer Netherlands: Dordrecht, The Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-3650-6_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koo, TK and Li, MY 2016 A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. Journal of Chiropractic Medicine 15: 155163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcm.2016.02.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labocha, MK and Hayes, JP 2012 Morphometric indices of body condition in birds: A review. Journal of Ornithology 153: 122. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10336-011-0706-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lüdecke, D, Ben-Shachar, MS, Patil, I and Makowski, D 2022 easystats: Framework for easy statistical modeling, visualization, and reporting CRAN. https://easystats.github.io/easystats/ (accessed 5/6/24).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lüdecke, D, Ben-Shachar, MS, Patil, I, Waggoner, P and Makowski, D 2021 performance: An R package for assessment, comparison and testing of statistical models. Journal of Open Source Software 6: 3139. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.03139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marzluff, JM and Balda, RP 1992 The Pinyon Jay: Behavioral Ecology of a Colonial and Cooperative Corvid. A&C Black: London, UK.Google Scholar
Mason, G, Clubb, R, Latham, N and Vickery, S 2007 Why and how should we use environmental enrichment to tackle stereotypic behaviour? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102: 163188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.041CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, GJ 1991a Stereotypies and suffering. Behavioural Processes 25: 103115. https://doi.org/10.1016/0376-6357(91)90013-PCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, GJ 1991b Stereotypies: A critical review. Animal Behaviour 41: 10151037. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80640-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, GJ and Latham, NR 2004 Can’t stop, won’t stop: Is stereotypy a reliable animal welfare indicator? Animal Welfare 13: S57S69. https://doi.org/10.1017/S096272860001438XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLeod, BJ, Thompson, EG, Crawford, JL and Shackell, GH 1997 Successful group housing of wild-caught brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula). Animal Welfare 6: 6776. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600019400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meehan, CL, Garner, JP and Mench, JA 2003 Isosexual pair housing improves the welfare of young Amazon parrots. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 81: 7388. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00238-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellor, E, Brilot, B and Collins, S 2018 Abnormal repetitive behaviours in captive birds: A Tinbergian review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 198: 109120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2017.09.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, R, Schiestl, M and Clayton, NS 2024 Corvids. In: Golledge, H and Richardson, C (eds) The UFAW Handbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory and Other Research Animals, 9th Edition pp 839852. John Wiley & Sons: London, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales Picard, A, Mundry, R, Auersperg, AM and Slocombe, KE 2020 Why preen others? Predictors of allopreening in parrots and corvids and comparisons to grooming in great apes. Ethology 126: 207228. https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12999CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morey, RD and Rouder, JN 2024 BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs. https://richarddmorey.github.io/BayesFactor/ (accessed 5/6/24).Google Scholar
Müller, K 2020 here: A simpler way to find your files. https://here.r-lib.org/ (accessed 5/6/24).Google Scholar
Papageorgiou, M, Goliomytis, M, Tzamaloukas, O, Miltiadou, D and Simitzis, P 2023 Positive welfare indicators and their association with sustainable management systems in poultry. Sustainability 15: 10890. https://doi.org/10.3390/su151410890CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paz-y-Miño-C, G, Bond, AB, Kamil, AC and Balda, RP 2004 Pinyon jays use transitive inference to predict social dominance. Nature 430: 778781. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02723CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedersen, TL 2024 patchwork: The composer of plots. https://patchwork.data-imaginist.com (accessed 5/6/24).Google Scholar
Prieto, R, Sánchez-García, C, Alonso, ME, Rodríguez, PL and Gaudioso, VR 2012 Do pairing systems improve welfare of captive red-legged partridges (Alectoris rufa) in laying cages? Poultry Science 91: 17511758. https://doi.org/10.3382/ps.2011-01677CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team 2024 R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed 5/6/24).Google Scholar
Revelle, W 2024 psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, USA. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych (accessed 5/6/24).Google Scholar
Richter, SH and Hintze, S 2019 From the individual to the population – and back again? Emphasising the role of the individual in animal welfare science. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 212: 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.12.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, PE, Nash, SM and Riley, LM 2017 To pace or not to pace? A review of what abnormal repetitive behavior tells us about zoo animal management. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 20: 1121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2017.02.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rushen, J, Chapinal, N and de Passilé, A 2012 Automated monitoring of behavioural-based animal welfare indicators. Animal Welfare 21: 339350. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.21.3.339CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stamps, JA, Briffa, M and Biro, PA 2012 Unpredictable animals: Individual differences in intraindividual variability (IIV). Animal Behaviour 83: 13251334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.02.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, JR 2024 cocoon: Extract, format, and print statistical output. https://github.com/JeffreyRStevens/cocoon (accessed 5/6/24).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, JR, Kennedy, BA, Morales, D and Burks, M 2016 The domain specificity of intertemporal choice in pinyon jays. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23: 915921. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0973-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swaisgood, RR and Shepherdson, DJ 2005 Scientific approaches to enrichment and stereotypies in zoo animals: What’s been done and where should we go next? Zoo Biology 24: 499518. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tahamtani, FM, Pedersen, IJ and Riber, AB 2019 Effects of environmental complexity on welfare indicators of fast-growing broiler chickens. Poultry Science 99: 2129. https://doi.org/10.3382/ps/pez510CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Templeton, JJ, Kamil, AC and Balda, RP 1999 Sociality and social learning in two species of corvids: The pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) and the Clark’s nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana). Journal of Comparative Psychology 113: 450455. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.113.4.450CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vernouillet, A, Casidsid, HJM and Kelly, DM 2021 Conspecific presence, but not pilferage, influences pinyon jays’ (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) caching behavior. Learning & Behavior 49: 2335. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-020-00450-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagenmakers, E-J 2007 A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 14: 779804. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagenmakers, E-J, Lodewyckx, T, Kuriyal, H and Grasman, R 2010 Bayesian hypothesis testing for psychologists: A tutorial on the Savage–Dickey method. Cognitive Psychology 60: 158189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.12.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagenmakers, E-J, Marsman, M, Jamil, T and Morey, RD 2018 Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical advantages and practical ramifications. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 25: 3557. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1343-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitham, JC and Miller, LJ 2016 Using technology to monitor and improve zoo animal welfare. Animal Welfare 25: 395409. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.25.4.395CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H, Averick, M, Bryan, J and Yutani, H 2019 Welcome to the tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software 4: 1686. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01686CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiggins, D 2005 Pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus): A technical conservation assessment. USDA Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Region, USA.Google Scholar
Williams, E, Chadwick, CL, Yon, L and Asher, L 2018 A review of current indicators of welfare in captive elephants (Loxodonta africana and Elephas maximus). Animal Welfare 27: 235249. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.27.3.235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, I, Hoppitt, W and Grant, R 2017 The effect of auditory enrichment, rearing method and social environment on the behavior of zoo-housed psittacines (Aves: Psittaciformes); implications for welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 186: 8592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.10.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolff, LM, Carey, K and Stevens, JR 2024 Friends aren’t food: Pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) show context-dependent quantity preference. Animal Behavior and Cognition 11: 112135. https://doi.org/10.26451/abc.11.02.01.2024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolff, LM and Stevens, JR 2024 Less activity means improved welfare? How pair housing influences pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) behavior. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.02.07.579343CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woods, JM, Eyer, A and Miller, LJ 2022 Bird welfare in zoos and aquariums: General insights across industries. Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens 3: 198222. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg3020017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xie, Y 2015 Dynamic documents with R and knitr, 2nd Edition. Chapman Hall/CRC: Boca Raton, Florida, USA. https://yihui.org/knitr/ (accessed 5/6/24).Google Scholar
Xie, Y, Allaire, JJ and Grolemund, G 2018 R markdown: The definitive guide. Chapman Hall/CRC: Boca Raton, Florida, USA. https://bookdown.org/yihui/rmarkdown (accessed 5/6/24).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhu, H 2023 kableExtra: Construct complex table with “kable” and pipe syntax. http://haozhu233.github.io/kableExtra/ (accessed 5/6/24).Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Individual subject information

Figure 1

Table 2. Ethogram of pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) behaviours used to code video

Figure 2

Figure 1. Camera screenshots of pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) in their cages showing (a) single and (b) pair housing. Figure used with permission under a CC-BY 4.0 licence: Wolff and Stevens (2024), available at https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.02.07.579343.

Figure 3

Table 3. Model comparison for effect of phase and time of day on pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) activity level

Figure 4

Figure 2. Activity levels of pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) before, during, and after moving from individual to pair housing (ten birds groups into five pairs). (a) Mean activity levels per sample across time of day for each phase. Points represent mean levels per individual video recording with phase indicated by colour and symbol. (b) Mean activity levels per sample across date. Points present mean levels averaged over dates with phase indicated by colour and symbol. Dots represent estimated marginal means per phase, and error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Figure used with permission under a CC-BY 4.0 licence: Wolff and Stevens (2024); available at https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.02.07.579343.

Figure 5

Figure 3. Mean frequencies of behaviours in the pre- and post-move phases for pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) (ten birds in five pairs). The behaviors other and out of view are omitted. Grey lines connect means for each of the five bird pairs. Dots represent overall means per phase, and error bars represent within-pair 95% confidence intervals. Figure used with permission under a CC-BY 4.0 licence: Wolff and Stevens (2024); available at https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.02.07.579343.