Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:32:01.014Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Fish and welfare: do fish have the capacity for pain perception and suffering?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

VA Braithwaite*
Affiliation:
Ashworth Laboratories, School of Biological Sciences, King's Buildings, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH9 3JT, UK
FA Huntingford
Affiliation:
Institute of Biomedical and Life Sciences, Graham Kerr Building, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8QQ, UK
*
* Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Humans interact with fish in a number of ways and the question of whether fish have the capacity to perceive pain and to suffer has recently attracted considerable attention in both scientific and public fora. Only very recently have neuroanatomical studies revealed that teleost fish possess similar pain-processing receptors to higher vertebrates. Research has also shown that fish neurophysiology and behaviour are altered in response to noxious stimulation. In the light of this evidence, and in combination with work illustrating the cognitive capacities of fish, it seems appropriate to respond to a recently published critique (Rose 2002) in which it is argued that it is not possible for fish to experience fear or pain and that, therefore, they cannot suffer. Whilst we agree with the author that fish are unlikely to perceive pain in the same way that humans do, we believe that currently available evidence indicates that fish have the capacity for pain perception and suffering. As such, it would seem timely to reflect on the implications of fish pain and suffering, and to consider what steps can be taken to ensure the welfare of the fish that we exploit.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2004 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Aronson, LR 1951 Orientation and jumping in the Gobiid fish ßathygobius soporator. American Museum Novitates 1486: 122Google Scholar
Aronson, LR 1971 Further studies on orientation and jumping behaviour in the Gobiid fish ßathygobius soporator. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 188: 378392CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balda, R P, Kamil, A C and Pepperberg, I M 1998 Animal Cognition in Nature. Academic Press: San Diego, USAGoogle Scholar
Barton, B A and Iwama, G K 1991 Physiological changes in fish from stress in aquaculture with emphasis on the response and effects of corticosteroids. Annual Review of Fish Disease 1: 326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P 1991 Assessment of pain in animals. Animal ßehaviour 42: 827839CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braithwaite, V A 1998 Spatial memory, landmark use and orientation in fish. In: Healy, SD (ed) Spatial Representations in Animals pp 86102. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
Broglio, C, Rodríguez, F and Salas, C 2003 Spatial cognition and its neural basis in teleost fishes. Fish and Fisheries 4: 247255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broom, D M 1991 Animal welfare: concepts and measurements. Journal of Animal Science 69: 41674175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, M S 1998 Evolution and animal welfare. Quarterly Review of Biology 73: 305328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrensing, R H, Micheli, G F and Kastin, A J 1982 Similar antagonism of morphine analgesia by MIF-I and naloxone in Carassius auratus. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behaviour 17: 757761CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emery, N J and Clayton, N S 2001 Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature 414: 443446CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FSBI 2002 Fish Welfare (Briefing Paper 2). Fisheries Society of the British Isles. Granta Information Systems: Cambridge, UK. Available at: http://www.le.ac.uk/biology/fsbi/briefing.htmlGoogle Scholar
Gentle, M J 1992 Pain in birds. Animal Welfare 1: 235247Google Scholar
Girvan, J R and Braithwaite, V A 1998 Population differences in spatial learning and memory in threespined sticklebacks. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences 265: 913918CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, G and Gauthier, C 2004 Guidelines development and scientific uncertainty: use of previous case studies to promote efficient production of guidelines on the care and use of fish in research, teaching and testing. In: KirkWood, J K, Roberts, E A and Vickery, S (eds) Proceedings of the UFAW Internationol Symposium ‘Science in the Service of Animal Welfare’, Edinburgh 2003. Animal Welfare 13: 51815186 (Suppl)Google Scholar
Griffiths, S W 2003 Learned recognition of conspecifics by fishes. Fish and Fisheries 4: 256268Google Scholar
Hampton, R R 2001 Rhesus monkeys know when they remember. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98: 53595362CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harding, E J and Mendl, M 2001 Is the glass half empty or half full? A novel approach for assessing mental states in laboratory rodents. In: Garner, J P, Mench, J A and Heekin, S P (eds) Proceedings of the 35th International Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology p 64 (Abstract). Centre for Animal Welfare at UC Davis: California, USAGoogle Scholar
Harding, E J, Mendl, M, Nicol, C and Paul, E S 2001 Using discrimination training to assess mental states. Animal Welfare 10: 5240 (Abstract)Google Scholar
Heyes, C M 1998 Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 101148Google Scholar
Huntingford, F A and Wright, P J 1989 How sticklebacks learn to avoid dangerous feeding patches. Behavioural Processes 19: 181189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, J L and Magurran, A E 2003 Learned predator recognition and antipredator responses in fishes. Fish and Fisheries 4: 216226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kotraschal, K, van Staaden, M J and Huber, R 1998 Fish brains: evolution and environmental relationships. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries 8: 373408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K N, Brown, C and Krause, J 2003 Learning in fishes: from three-second memory to culture. Fish and Fisheries 4: 199202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laming, P R and McKinney, S J 1990 Habituation in goldfish (Crassius auratus) is impaired by increased interstimulus interval, interval variability, and telencephalic ablation. Behavioral Neuroscience 194: 869875CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macphail, E M 1998 The Evolution of Consciousness. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendl, M 2001 Animal husbandry: assessing the welfare state. Nature 410: 3132CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metcalfe, N B and Thomson, B C 1995 Fish recognize and prefer to shoal with poor competitors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences 259: 207210Google Scholar
Molony, V, Kent, J E and Kendrick, I J 2002 Validation of a method for assessment of acute pain in lambs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 76: 215238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, J S 1994 Fishes of the World. Wiley and Sons: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Odling-Smee, L and Braithwaite, V A 2003a The influence of habitat stability on landmark use during spatial learning in the threespine stickleback. Animal Behaviour 65: 701707CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odling-Smee, L and Braithwaite, V A 2003b The role of learning in fish orientation. Fish and Fisheries 4: 235246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overmeir, J B and Hollis, K 1983 The teleostan telencephalon and learning. In: Davis, R and Northcutt, G (eds) Fish Neurobiology, Volume 2, Higher Brain Functions pp 265284. University of Michigan Press: Ann Arbor, Michigan, USAGoogle Scholar
Overmeir, J B and Papini, M R 1986 Factors modulating the effects of teleost telencephalon ablation on retention, relearning and extinction of instrumental avoidance behaviour. Behavioral Neuroscience 100: 190199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, A D and Pottinger, T G 1989 Stress response and disease resistance in salmonid fish: effects of chronic elevation of plasma cortisol. Fish Physiology and Biochemistry 7: 253258CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reebs, S G 1999 Time-place learning based on food but not on predation risk in a fish, the inanga (Galaxias maculatus) Ethology 105: 361371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez, F, Durán, E, Vargas, J P, Torres, B and Salas, C 1994 Performance of goldfish trained in allocentric and egocentric maze procedures suggest the presence of a cognitive mapping system in fishes. Animal Learning and Behaviour 10: 108114Google Scholar
Rooney, D J and Laming, P R 1988 Effects of telencephalic ablation on habituation of arousal responses within and between daily training sessions in goldfish. Behavioral and Neural Biology 49: 8396CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, J D 2002 The neurobehavioral nature of fishes and the question of awareness and pain. Reviews in Fisheries Science 10: 138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowland, W J 1994 Proximate determinants of stickleback behaviour: an evolutionary perspective. In: Bell, M A and Foster, S A (eds) The Evolutionary Biology of the Threespine Stickleback pp 297344. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
Salas, C, Broglio, C, Rodríguez, F, Lopez, J C, Portavella, M and Torres, B 1996 Telencephalic ablation in goldfish impairs performance in a ‘spatial constancy’ problem but not in a cued one. Behavioural Brain Research 79: 193200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shettleworth, S J 1998 Cognition, Evolution and Behavior. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
Shimizu, T and Karten, H J 1993 The avian visual system and the evolution of the neocortex. In: Zeigler, H P and Bischof, H J (eds) Vision, Brain and Behaviour in Birds pp 103114. MIT Press: Massachussetts, USAGoogle Scholar
Sneddon, L U 2002 Anatomical and electrophysiological analysis of the trigeminal nerve in a teleost fish, Oncorhynchus mykiss. Neuroscience Letters 319: 167171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sneddon, L U, Braithwaite, V A and Gentle, M J 2003 Do fish have nociceptors? Evidence for the evolution of a vertebrate sensory system. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences 270: 11151121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sneddon, L U, Braithwaite, V A and Gentle, M J Novel object test: examining pain and fear in the rainbow trout. Journal of Pain: in press.Google Scholar
Sørensen, J T, Sandøe, P and Halberg, N 2001 Animal welfare as one among several values to be considered at farm level: the idea of an ethical account for livestock farming. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica, Section A, Animal Science 30: 1116Google Scholar
Verheijen, F J and Bulwalda, R J A 1988 Do Pain and Fear Make a Hooked Carp in Play Suffer? CIP-GEGEVENS Koninklijke Bibliotheek: Den Haag, Utrecht, The NetherlandsGoogle Scholar
Wall, P D and Melzack, R 2000 Textbook of Pain, Fourth Edition. Churchill Livingstone: Edinburgh, UKGoogle Scholar
Wedemeyer, G A, Barton, B A and McLeay, D J 1990 Stress and acclimation. In: Schreck C B and Moyle P B (eds) Methods for Fish ßiology pp 451489. American Fisheries Society: Bethesda,. Maryland, USA.Google Scholar
Wendelaar Bonga, S E 1997 The stress response in fish. Physiology Review 77: 591625CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitear, M 1971 The free nerve endings in fish epidermis. Journal of Zoology 663: 231236CrossRefGoogle Scholar