Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T10:47:23.508Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Structure of Emotional Response: 1984 Presidential Candidates

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 September 2013

George E. Marcus*
Affiliation:
Williams College

Abstract

Over the past two decades psychological models of affect have changed from valence (one-dimensional) models to multiple-dimensional models. The most recent models, circumplex models, are two-dimensional. Feeling thermometer measures, which derive their theoretical logic from earlier (valence) models of emotional appraisal, are shown to be confounded. Underlying the variation obtained using feeling thermometer measures are two dimensions of emotional response, mastery (positive emotionality) and threat (negative emotionality). Analysis of the 1984 NES survey suggests that positive emotional response is twice as influential as negative emotional response in predicting presidential candidate vote disposition to the presidential candidates. Reliance on emotional response is shown to be uniformly influential across various strata of the electorate.

Policy considerations have little direct influence on vote disposition, though policy considerations are indirectly related to vote disposition through the influence of issues on the degree of feelings of threat evoked by the candidates.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © American Political Science Association 1988 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abelson, Robert, Kinder, Donald, Peters, Mark, and Fiske, Susan. 1982. Affective and Semantic Components in Political Person Perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 42:619–30.Google Scholar
Bay, Christian. 1965. The Structure of Freedom. New York: Athenaeum.Google Scholar
Bollen, Kenneth, and Barb, Kenney. 1981. Pearson's R and Coarsely Categorized Measures. American Sociological Review 46:232–39.Google Scholar
Bower, Gordon. 1981. Mood and Memory. American Psychologist 36:129–48.Google Scholar
Breckler, Steven J. 1984. Empirical Validation of Affect, Behavior, and Cognition As Distinct Components of Attitude. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47:11911205.Google Scholar
Brody, Richard, and Page, Benjamin. 1972. Comment: The Assessment of Policy Voting. American Politicai Science Review 66:450–58.Google Scholar
Brody, Richard, and Page, Benjamin. 1973. Indifference, Alienation, and Rational Decisions: The Effects of Candidate Evaluations on Turnout and the Vote. Public Choice 15:117.Google Scholar
Chlopan, Bruce, McCain, M., Carbonell, J., and Hagen, R.. 1985. Empathy: A Review of Available Measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48:635–53.Google Scholar
Conover, Pamela J., and Feldman, Stanley. 1986. Emotional Reactions to the Economy: I'm Mad as Hell and I'm Not Going To Take It Any More. American Journal of Political Science 30:50–78.Google Scholar
Craig, Stephen C., and Maggiotto, Michael A.. 1982. Measuring Political Efficacy. Political Methodology 8:85110.Google Scholar
Davies, Alan F. 1980. Skills, Outlooks, and Passions: A Psychoanalytic Contribution to the Study of Politics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Davis, Mark H. 1983. Measuring Individual Differences in Empathy: Evidence from a Multidimensional Approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44:133–26.Google Scholar
Derryberry, Douglas, and Rothbart, Mary. 1984. Emotion, Attention, and Temperament. In Emotion, Cognition, and Behavior, ed. Izard, Carroll, Kagan, Jerome, and Zajonc, Robert. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Diener, Ed, and Emmons, Robert A.. 1984. The Independence of Positive and Negative Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47: 1105–17.Google Scholar
Fishbein, Martin, and Ajzen, Icek. 1975. Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Fiske, Susan, and Pavelchak, M.. 1985. Category-based versus Piecemeal-based Affective Responses: Developments in Schema-triggered Affect. In The Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior, ed. Sorrentino, Richard and Higgins, E.. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fonberg, Elzbieta. 1986. Amygdala, Emotions, Motivation, and Depressive States. In Emotion: Theory, Research, and Experience, ed. Plutchik, Robert and Kellerman, Henry. London: Academic.Google Scholar
Fromm, Erich. 1941. Escape from Freedom. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Gray, Jeffrey A. 1970. The Psychophysiological Basis of Introversion-Extroversion. Behaviour Research and Therapy 8:249–66.Google Scholar
Gray, Jeffrey A. 1973. Causal Theories of Personality and How To Test Them. In Multivariate Analysis and Psychological Theory, ed. Royce, Joseph R.. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Gray, Jeffrey A. 1981. The Psychophysiology of Anxiety. In Dimensions of Personality: Papers in Honour of H. J. Eysenck, ed. Lynn, Richard. New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Greenwald, Anthony. 1980. The Totalitarian Ego: Fabrication and Revision of Personal History. American Psychologist 35:603–18.Google Scholar
Hastie, Reid. 1986. A Primer of Information-processing Theory for the Political Scientist. In Political Cognition, ed. Lau, Richard and Sears, David. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Izard, Carroll E. 1971. Patterns of Emotions: A New Analysis of Anxiety and Depression. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Izard, Carroll E. 1977. Human Emotions. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Jöreskog, Karl G. 1969. A General Approach to Confirmatory Maximum Likelihood Factor Analysis. Psychometrika 34:183202.Google Scholar
Jöreskog, Karl G. 1970. A General Method for Analysis of Covariance Structures. Biometrika 57:239–51.Google Scholar
Jöreskog, Karl G. 1973. A General Method for Estimating a Linear Structural Equation System. In Structural Equation Models in the Social Sciences, ed. Goldberger, Arthur S. and Duncan, O. D.. New York: Seminar.Google Scholar
Kelley, Stanley, and Mirer, Thad. 1974. The Simple Act of Voting. American Political Science Review 68:572–91.Google Scholar
Kinder, Donald. 1986. Presidential Character Revisited. In Political Cognition, ed. Lau, Richard and Sears, David. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kinder, Donald, and Sears, David. 1981. Prejudice and Politics: Symbolic Racism versus Racial Threat to the Good Life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 40:414–31.Google Scholar
Krouse, Richard, and Marcus, George E.. 1984. Electoral Studies and Democratic Theory Reconsidered. Political Behavior 6:2339.Google Scholar
Kuklinski, James, Ottati, E., Schwarz, N., and Wyler, Robert. 1987. Why the Slippage? Explaining Why People Support Abstract Principles More Than the Application of Them. Presented at the annual meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association, Chicago.Google Scholar
Lane, Robert E. 1978. Autonomy, Felicity, Futility: The Effects of the Market Economy on Political Personality. Journal of Politics 40:224.Google Scholar
Lau, Richard. 1986. Political Schemata, Candidate Evaluations, and Voting Behavior. In Political Cognition, ed. author and Sears, David. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lazarus, Richard. 1982. Thoughts on the Relations of Emotion and Cognition. American Psychologist 37:1019–24.Google Scholar
Lazarus, Richard. 1984. On the Primacy of Cognition. American Psychologist 39:124–29.Google Scholar
Lewicki, Pawel. 1986. Nonconscious Social Information Processing. Orlando, FL: Academic.Google Scholar
Lippmann, Walter. 1922. Public Opinion. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Lloyd, Genevieve. 1984. The Man of Reason: “Male” and “Female” in Western Philosophy. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Lushkin, Robert. 1987. Measuring Political Sophistication. American Journal of Political Science 31:856–99.Google Scholar
Marcus, George E. 1985. A Theory and Methodology for Measuring Emotions in Politics. Presented at the annual meeting of the Political Methodology Society, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Marcus, George E. 1987. The Structure of Emotional Appraisal: 1984 Presidential Candidates. Presented at the annual meeting of the International Society for Political Psychology, San Francisco.Google Scholar
Marcus, George E., and Rahn, Wendy. N.d. Emotions and Democratic Politics. In Research in Micropolitics, ed. Long, Samuel. Greenwich, CT; JAI.Google Scholar
Markus, Gregory. 1986. Stability and Change in Political Attitudes: Observed, Recalled, and “Explained.” Political Behavior 8: 2145.Google Scholar
Masters, Roger, and Sullivan, Dennis. 1986. Nonverbal Displays and Political Leadership in France and the United States. Presented at the 1986 annual meeting of the American Political Science Association, Washington.Google Scholar
Mill, John Stuart. 1956. On Liberty. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill.Google Scholar
Millar, Murray, and Tesser, Abraham. 1986. Effects of Affective and Cognitive Focus on the Attitude-Behavior Relation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51:270–76.Google Scholar
Miller, Arthur, Wattenburg, Martin, and Oskana, Malanchuk. 1986. Schematic Assessments of Candidates. American Political Science Review 80: 521–40.Google Scholar
Mueller, John. N.d. Trends in Political Tolerance. Public Opinion Quarterly. Forthcoming.Google Scholar
Neuman, Russell. 1986. The Paradox of Mass Politics: Knowledge and Opinion in the American Electorate. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, Richard, and Ross, Lee. 1980. Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Page, Benjamin. 1976. The Theory of Political Ambiguity. American Political Science Review 70:742–52.Google Scholar
Page, Benjamin. 1978. Choices and Echoes m Presidential Elections. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Page, Benjamin, and Brody, Richard. 1972. Policy Voting and the Electoral Process: The Vietnam War Issue. American Political Science Review 66:979–95.Google Scholar
Phares, E. Jerry. 1978. Locus of Control. In Dimensions of Personality, ed. London, Harvey and Exner, John E. Jr. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Pitkin, Hannah. 1967. The Concept of Representation. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Plutchik, Robert. 1980. Emotion: A Psychoevolutionary Synthesis. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Pomper, Gerald, Baker, Ross, Jacob, Charles, Keeter, Scott, McWilliams, Wilson Carey, and Plotkin, Henry. 1985. The Election of 1984. Chatham, NJ: Chatham.Google Scholar
Rahn, Wendy, Aldrich, John, Borgida, Eugene, and Sullivan, John. N.d. A Social-Cognitive Model of Candidate Appraisal. In Information and Democratic Processes, ed. Ferejohn, John and Kuklinski, James. Urbana-Champaign: University of Illinois Press. Forthcoming.Google Scholar
Roseman, Ira. 1979. Cognitive Aspects of Emotion and Emotional Behavior. Presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington.Google Scholar
Roseman, Ira. 1984. Cognitive Determinants of Emotions: A Structural Theory. In Emotions, Relationships, and Health, Review of Personality and Social Psychology, no. 5, ed. Shaver, P.. Beverly Hills: Sage.Google Scholar
Roseman, Ira, Abelson, Robert, and Ewing, Michael. 1986. Emotions and Political Cognition: Emotional Appeals in Political Communication. In Political Cognition, ed. Lau, Richard and Sears, David. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rotter, J. B. 1966. Generalized Expectancies for Internal versus External Control of Reinforcement. Psychological Monographs 80:1.Google Scholar
Russell, James A. 1980. A Circumplex Model of Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39:1161–78.Google Scholar
Sears, David O., Lau, Richard R., Tyler, Tom R., and Allen, Harris M. Jr. 1980. Self-interest vs. Symbolic Politics in Policy Attitudes and Presidential Voting. American Political Science Review 74:670–84.Google Scholar
Scherer, Klaus. 1982. The Nature and Function of Emotion. Social Science Information 21:507–9.Google Scholar
Seligman, Martin. 1975. Helplessness: On Depression, Development, and Death. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Shingles, Richard. 1986. Dimensions of Political Efficacy: Tests of New and Old Measures. Presented at the American Political Science Association annual meeting, Washington.Google Scholar
Simon, Herbert. 1967. Motivational and Emotional Controls of Cognition. Psychological Review 74: 2939.Google Scholar
Snyder, Mark, and Ickes, William. 1985. Personality and Social Behavior. In The Handbook of Social Psychology, 3d ed., vol. 2, Special Fields and Applications, ed. Lindzey, Gardner and Aronson, Elliot. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Storm, Christine, and Storm, Tom. 1987. A Taxonomic Study of the Vocabulary of Emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53: 805–16.Google Scholar
Sullivan, Dennis, and Masters, Roger. 1988. Happy Warriors: Leaders' Facial Displays, Viewers Emotions, and Political Support. American Journal of Political Science 32:345–68.Google Scholar
Sullivan, John L., Piereson, James, and Marcus, George E.. 1982. Political Tolerance and American Democracy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Tassinary, Louis, Orr, Scott, Wolford, George, Napps, Shirley, and Lanzetta, John. 1984. The Role of Awareness in Affective Information Processing: An Exploration of the Zajonc Hypothesis. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 22:489–92.Google Scholar
Tellegen, Auke. 1985. Structures of Mood and Personality and Their Relevance to Assessing Anxiety, with an Emphasis on Self-Report. In Anxiety and the Anxiety Disorders, ed. Turna, A. Hussein and Maser, J. D.. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Thompson, Dennis. 1971. The Democratic Citizen: Social Science and Democratic Theory in the Twentieth Century. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tomkins, Silvan S. 1981. The Quest for Primary Motives: Biography and Autobiography of an Idea. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 41:306–29.Google Scholar
Tsal, Yehoshua. 1985. On the Relationship between Cognitive and Affective Processes: A Critique of Zajonc and Markus. Journal of Consumer Research 12:358–62.Google Scholar
Warr, Peter, Barter, Joanna, and Brownbridge, Garry. 1983. On the Independence of Positive and Negative Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44:644–51.Google Scholar
Watson, David, and Tellegen, Auke. 1985. Toward a Consensual Structure of Mood. Psychological Bulletin 98:219–35.Google Scholar
Wiener, B., Russell, D., and Lerman, D.. 1978. Affective Consequences of Causal Ascriptions. In New Directions in Attribution Research, 3 vols., ed. Harvey, John H. and Kidd, R. F.. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. 1980. Feeling and Thinking: Preferences Need No Inferences. American Psychologist 35:151–75.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. 1982. On the Primacy of Affect. American Psychologist 39:117–23.Google Scholar
Zevon, M. A., and Tellegen, Auke. 1982. The Structure of Mood Change: An Ideographic/Nomo-thetic Analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43:111–22.Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.