Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T06:52:05.576Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

War Cues and Foreign Policy Acts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2014

Francis A. Beer
Affiliation:
University of Colorado, Boulder
Alice F. Healy
Affiliation:
University of Colorado, Boulder
Grant P. Sinclair
Affiliation:
University of Colorado, Boulder
Lyle E. Bourne Jr.
Affiliation:
University of Colorado, Boulder

Abstract

Cognitive psychological theory and research suggest that specific actions are primed or potentiated by prior related cues. Thus violent cues should influence subsequent conflictual action choices. Laboratory subjects were given one of two written vignettes recalling either the slaughter of World War I or Allied weakness before World War II. A control group received no vignette. Subjects were then asked to specify foreign-policy choices from an inventory of possible cooperative-conflictual acts, relevant to a dispute between two fictional countries, in five sequential rounds. Both war vignettes produced more subsequent conflictual choices for subjects with dominant (i.e., aggressive) personality traits, and less conflictual choices for subjects with submissive personality traits. The findings suggest that priming activates underlying personality predispositions that enhance cooperative or conflictual action choices. The results also point to the potential importance of prior cues in shaping foreign-policy decision makers' and mass-public responses to international events.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © American Political Science Association 1987

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alker, Hayward R. Jr., Bennett, James P., and Mefford, Dwain. 1979. Generalized Precedent Logics for Resolving Security Dilemmas. Cambridge, MA: MIT Center for International Studies.Google Scholar
Anderson, John R. 1980. Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Anderson, John R. 1983. A Spreading Activation Theory of Memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22:261–95.Google Scholar
Anderson, Richard C., and Pichert, James W. 1978. Recall of Previously Unrecallable Information Following a Shift in Perspective. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 17:112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axelrod, Robert. 1984. The Evolution of Cooperation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Azar, Edward E. 1980. The Conflict and Peace Data Bank (COPDAB) Project. Journal of Conflict Resolution 24(1): 143–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azar, Edward E., and Havener, Thomas N. 1976. Discontinuities in the Symbolic Environment: A Problem in Scaling. International Interactions 2: 231–46.Google Scholar
Bargh, John A., Bond, Ronald N., Lombardi, Wendy J., and Tota, Mary E. 1986. The Additive Nature of Chronic and Temporary Sources of Construct Accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50:869–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, John A., and Pietromonaco, Paula. 1982. Automatic Information Processing and Social Perception: The Influence of Trait Information Presented outside of Conscious Awareness on Impression Formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43:437–49.Google Scholar
Bordin, Jeffrey T. 1986. The Effects of Irrelevant Imagery and Historical Lessons Experienced during Young Adulthood on Present-Day Decision-Making. Paper presented at the ninth annual scientific meeting of the International Society of Political Psychology, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Bosman, J. 1986, Political Metaphors. In Political Psychology in the Netherlands: The Nijmegen Papers, ed. Brouwer, Marten, van Ginneken, Jaap, Hagendoorn, Louk, and Meloen, Jos. Nijmegan, Netherlands: University of Nijmegen.Google Scholar
Cartwright, Desmond S. 1974. Introduction to Personality. Chicago: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Dierks, Jack C. 1970. A Leap to Arms: The Cuban Campaign of 1898. Philadelphia: Lippincott.Google Scholar
Etheredge, Lloyd S. 1978. Personality Effects on American Foreign Policy, 1898–1968: A Test of Interpersonal Generalization Theory. American Political Science Review 72:434–51.Google Scholar
Gilovich, Thomas. 1981. Seeing the Past in the Present: The Effect of Associations to Familiar Events on Judgments and Decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 40:797808.Google Scholar
Glenberg, Arthur M., Bradley, Margaret M., Kraus, Thomas A., and Renzaglia, Gary J. 1983. Studies of the Long-Term Recency Effect: Support for a Contextually Guided Retrieval Hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 9: 231–55.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. Tory, and King, Gillian. 1981. Accessibility of Social Constructs: Information Processing Consequences of Individual and Contextual Variability. In Personality, Cognition, and Social Interaction, ed. Cantor, Nancy and Kihlstrom, John F. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. Tory, King, Gillian, and Mavin, Gregory H. 1982. Individual Construct Accessibility and Subjective Impressions and Recall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43:35–47.Google Scholar
Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. 1979. Administrator's Manual for the 16 PF. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, Philip N. 1983. Mental Models. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel, and Tversky, Amos. 1979. Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision and Risk. Econometrica 47:263–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
May, Ernest R. 1973. “Lessons” of the Past: The Use and Misuse of History in American Foreign Policy. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Neustadt, Richard E., and May, Ernest R. 1986. Thinking in Time: The Uses of History for Decision-Makers. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Read, Stephen J. 1984. Analogical Reasoning in Social Judgment: The Importance of Causal Theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46:1425.Google Scholar
Roediger, Henry L. 1980. Memory Metaphors in Cognitive Psychology. Memory & Cognition 8: 231–46.Google Scholar
Ruben, Brent D., ed. 1986. Information and Behavior. Vol. 1. New Brunswick: Transaction Books.Google Scholar
Spanier, John. 1975. Games Nations Play: Analyzing International Politics. 2d ed. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Thomas, Dan, and Sigelman, Lee. 1984. Presidential Identification and Policy Leadership: Experimental Evidence on the Reagan Case. Policy Studies Journal 12(4): 663–75.Google Scholar
Waterman, Donald A., and Hayes-Roth, Frederick, eds. 1978. Pattern-Directed Inference Systems. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Ziegler, David W. 1977. War, Peace, and International Politics. Boston: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.