Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T13:06:09.132Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Working in old age in Mexico: implications for cognitive functioning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 March 2021

Francisca S. Rodriguez*
Affiliation:
German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Greifswald, Germany Center for Cognitive Science, University of Kaiserslautern, Kaiserslautern, Germany Institute of Social Medicine, Occupational Health and Public Health, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
Joseph Saenz
Affiliation:
Leonard Davis School of Gerontology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Previous studies indicate that occupation might affect cognitive functioning in late life. As people in low- and middle-income countries often have to work until late life, we sought to investigate if there are cognitive benefits to working later into life and whether cognitive function deteriorates after exiting the labour force. We analysed longitudinal data from the Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS), a nationally representative sample of Mexican adults age 50+ (N = 7,375), that assessed cognitive functioning by verbal learning, delayed recall and visual scanning. Analyses were carried out using mixed-effects modelling corrected for the influence of gender, instrumental activities of daily living, diabetes, stroke, hypertension, depression, income and marital status. Results suggest that working actively, compared to exiting the workforce, was associated with cognitive performance only in context with occupation. Domestic workers had a faster decline in verbal learning (b = −0.02, p = 0.020) and delayed recall (b = −0.02, p = 0.036) if they continued working actively and people working in administration (b = 0.03, p = 0.007), sales (b = 0.02, p = 0.044) and educators (b = 0.03, p = 0.049) had a slower decline in visual scanning if they continued working in old age. Our findings indicate that continued participation in the labour force in old age does not necessarily come with cognitive benefits. Whether or not working actively in later life protects or even harms cognitive functioning is likely to depend on the type of job.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

While population ageing is occurring at a global level (World Health Organization, 2016), this process is occurring especially rapidly in Mexico and Latin America (Wong and Palloni, Reference Wong, Palloni and Uhlenberg2009). The Mexican population aged 60 and over is projected to rise from 3.9 to 16.1 per cent from 2000 to 2050 (Consejo Nacional de Población, 2004). However, population ageing in Mexico has outpaced the development of institutional support systems for aged adults (Wong and Palloni, Reference Wong, Palloni and Uhlenberg2009). Population ageing has also occurred alongside decreases in fertility (Cosío and Ferreiro, Reference Cosío and Ferreiro1992), limiting the potential supply of children to care for older adults with physical and cognitive limitations. These population trends have led to increased attention on the predictors of cognitive health and the costs associated with it (Langa et al., Reference Langa, Chernew, Kabeto, Herzog, Ofstedal, Willis, Wallace, Mucha, Straus and Fendrick2001; Hurd et al., Reference Hurd, Martorell, Delavande, Mullen and Langa2013).

Prior studies of cognitive function among older Mexican adults have highlighted the importance of education and community size (Saenz et al., Reference Saenz, Downer, Garcia and Wong2017), health behaviours (Aguilar-Navarro et al., Reference Aguilar-Navarro, Reyes-Guerrero and Borgues2007b), lifecourse socio-economic status (Zeki Al Hazzouri et al., Reference Zeki Al Hazzouri, Haan, Galea and Aiello2011) and diabetes (Mejia-Arango and Gutierrez, Reference Mejia-Arango and Gutierrez2011). However, less research has investigated the associations between occupation throughout the lifecourse and cognitive function in late life in Mexico. This is a considerable omission as research in developed countries has demonstrated important differences in old age cognition by occupation (Dartigues et al., Reference Dartigues, Gagnon, Letenneur, Barberger-Gateau, Commenges, Evaldre and Salamon1992a; Andel et al., Reference Andel, Crowe, Pedersen, Mortimer, Crimmins, Johansson and Gatz2005; Smart et al., Reference Smart, Gow and Deary2014). Previous studies indicate that having poor memory is more than twice as high for farmers, domestic workers and blue-collar workers compared to managers (Dartigues et al., Reference Dartigues, Gagnon, Mazaux, Barberger-Gateau, Commenges, Letenneur and Orgogozo1992b). In an Australian study, employees in trade, technology and some service occupations were also more likely to have dementia (Jorm et al., Reference Jorm, Rodgers, Henderson, Korten, Jacomb, Christensen and Mackinnon1998). Results from a population-based study in France point out that the risk of farmworkers having cognitive impairments is about six times and of domestic workers about three times as high (Dartigues et al., Reference Dartigues, Gagnon, Letenneur, Barberger-Gateau, Commenges, Evaldre and Salamon1992a). Occupations with the lowest risk for dementia seem to be managers (Helmer et al., Reference Helmer, Letenneur, Rouch, Richard-Harston, Barberger-Gateau, Fabrigoule, Orgogozo and Dartigues2001). There seems to be a tendency for manual work to come with a higher dementia risk (Qiu et al., Reference Qiu, Karp, von Strauss, Winblad, Fratiglioni and Bellander2003) and for sedentary occupations to come with a lower dementia risk (Anttila et al., Reference Anttila, Helkala, Kivipelto, Hallikainen, Alhainen, Heinonen, Mannermaa, Tuomilehto, Soininen and Nissinen2002). Findings are explained with the intellectual complexity of the job. People who have occupations with greater complexity tend to have better cognitive functioning in old age (Finkel et al., Reference Finkel, Andel, Gatz and Pedersen2009) and seem to have a lower dementia risk (Then et al., Reference Then, Luck, Luppa, Thinschmidt, Deckert, Nieuwenhuijsen, Seidler and Riedel-Heller2014).

These observations are explained by the ‘use it or lose it hypothesis’ which posits that engaging in intellectual activities may prevent cognitive abilities from declining (Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2006). ‘Using’ intellectual capacities leads to an activation of neurons, a process that may interfere with the process of ageing by upregulating the neuronal biochemical activity including neurotransmitters and hormones (Swaab, Reference Swaab1991). Intellectual stimulation at work might do exactly this, as results from different studies show (Then et al., Reference Then, Luck, Luppa, Thinschmidt, Deckert, Nieuwenhuijsen, Seidler and Riedel-Heller2014, Reference Then, Luck, Heser, Ernst, Posselt, Wiese, Mamone, Brettschneider, Konig, Weyerer, Werle, Mosch, Bickel, Fuchs, Pentzek, Maier, Scherer, Wagner and Riedel-Heller2017). Occupation may, thus, be an important determinant of cognitive function in late life. However, not all studies confirm that engaging in intellectual activities protects against cognitive decline (Staff et al., Reference Staff, Hogan, Williams and Whalley2018). Hence, it might be a question of what type of activities a person engages in that determines whether cognitive capacities will be lost.

It is unclear both whether there are similar cognitive benefits to working later into life and whether cognitive function deteriorates after exiting the labour force. Results from the Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging show that, before retirement, greater occupational complexity was associated with better cognitive performance in verbal abilities, but, after retirement, greater occupational complexity was associated with faster decline in spatial reasoning (Finkel et al., Reference Finkel, Andel, Gatz and Pedersen2009). This conclusion suggests that late retirement may protect cognitive abilities. Indeed, results from the longitudinal French Three-City cohort study indicate that retiring later in life is associated with a lower dementia risk (Grotz et al., Reference Grotz, Meillon, Amieva, Stern, Dartigues, Adam and Letenneur2015). It seems that retiring later in life delays the age of onset of dementia (Lupton et al., Reference Lupton, Stahl, Archer, Foy, Poppe, Lovestone, Hollingworth, Williams, Owen and Dowzell2010). However, the answer might not be as simple as this. Findings from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) highlight that the decision to retire always led to worse cognitive functioning in people with a low level of occupational complexity, but not in those with high complexity (Carr et al., Reference Carr, Willis, Kail and Carstensen2020). Further evidence shows that individuals with higher mental demands at work had a better cognitive functioning before retirement and a slower rate of cognitive decline after retirement (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Stachowski, Infurna, Faul, Grosch and Tetrick2014). Hence, working later in life might only come with benefits for a certain occupational group.

Moreover, the effects of working in older age might not be the same in developed and less-developed countries. The Mexican occupational context differs from that of the United States of America (USA) in several important ways. Older adults in Mexico are more likely to have been employed in different types of occupations throughout the lifecourse than older adults in the USA and other developed countries, with labour in the agricultural sector making up a considerably larger share of total employment in Mexico (World Bank, 2017). Mexico also contains a large and stable informal labour sector (Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía, 2017), with the share of informal labourers differing substantially by work activity, and informal labourers concentrated at lower levels of the income distribution (Gasparini and Tornarolli, Reference Gasparini and Tornarolli2009). Female labour force participation in Mexico, although rising, has also lagged behind the USA and other Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development countries (Besamusca et al., Reference Besamusca, Tijdens, Keune and Steinmetz2015). The unique context of labour in Mexico implies that older Mexican adults may arrive in old age having interacted with a labour market quite different from the USA and other developing countries. Furthermore, the Mexican retirement pension system is unsustainable, unattainable for many Mexicans and far from including those living in poverty (Calvo et al., Reference Calvo, Bertranou and Bertranou2010). Many Mexicans, especially those with low education and income, have difficulties getting the chance to participate in saving up pension funds and/or have only low pension funds (Tuesta, Reference Tuesta2017). Economic resources, however, are a very influential factor driving retirement decisions (Barnes-Farrell, Reference Barnes-Farrell, Adams and Beehr2003), so that many Mexicans have to continue working in old age.

This study aimed to evaluate variation in cognitive function in old age according to occupation in the context of a developing country. We examined whether, among Mexican adults age 50 and older, baseline cognitive function differs across occupational categories, whether participation versus exiting the labour force in old age affects cognitive functioning, and to what extent the type of occupation moderates associations between labour force participation and cognitive function. Given that there seems to be a protective effect of work on cognition, we hypothesise that working actively in older age comes with a slower cognitive decline (Hypothesis 1). Moreover, based on previous studies, we hypothesise that manual occupations as well as farm and domestic workers have a lower cognitive function and faster cognitive decline (Hypothesis 2). Further, our analyses focus on performance in cognitive tests on verbal learning, delayed recall and visual scanning, abilities that naturally decline with ageing (Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2009). While visual scanning is a marker for attention in ageing (Trick and Enns, Reference Trick and Enns1998), verbal learning and delayed recall are a high-risk indicator for progression to dementia (Prado et al., Reference Prado, Watt, Treeby and Crowe2019). If the ‘use it or lose it hypothesis’ is domain-specific (i.e. only those cognitive abilities used at work are maintained better), then we hypothesise that different occupational types might have different effects on those cognitive abilities (Hypothesis 3).

Methods

Data

We used longitudinal data from the Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS) (Wong et al., Reference Wong, Michaels-Obregon and Palloni2015a). The MHAS began in 2001 as a nationally representative, household-based sample of older Mexican adults (age 50 and over) that is comparable to the US HRS. In stratified random population sampling, a nationally representative sample of individuals born prior to 1951 from both rural and urban areas distributed in all 32 states of the country were selected to participate in the study (for more details, see Wong et al., Reference Wong, Michaels-Obregon, Palloni, Gutierrez-Robledo, Gonzalez-Gonzalez, Lopez-Ortega, Tellez-Rojo and Mendoza-Alvarado2017). Follow-up assessments have been conducted in 2003, 2012 and 2015. The MHAS collected data spanning several domains, including chronic health conditions, mental health, income and employment, among others (explained in greater detail by Wong et al., Reference Wong, Michaels-Obregon and Palloni2015a) by visiting randomly selected individuals in their homes and interviewing them face-to-face. Data files are available at www.MHASweb.org. The MHAS study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board or Ethics Committee of the University of Texas Medical Branch, the INEGI in Mexico and the Instituto Nacional de Salud Pública (INSP) in Mexico.

From N = 15,402 respondents in the baseline assessment in 2001, we excluded N = 1,723 participants because they were not yet age 50, and an additional N = 95 respondents with missing data on education, N = 2,652 with missing data on occupation and N = 3,171 with missing data on performance in the cognitive test that we used in this analysis. The model did not predict a propensity score for 386 participants because they had missing or invalid values on either of the variables. Thus, we excluded those from our analyses. Analyses of differences between those included and those excluded from the analysis due to missing data suggest that those excluded were significantly more likely to be women, be younger, have less education, have impaired instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs), have had cancer, diabetes, hypertension, stroke or depressive symptoms, were less likely to be working, and have a slightly lower income (for details, see Table S1 in the online supplementary material). Our sample for analysis comprised N = 7,375 respondents at baseline. For the follow-up assessment in 2003, our sample comprised 6,835 participants, for 2012, 4,957 participants and for 2015, 4,634 participants. People lost to follow-up in 2015 were significantly older (mean 64.7 versus 58.5 years, F = 1,021.39, p < 0.001), more likely to be men (56.9% versus 51.3%, χ2 = 20.46, p < 0.001) and to be in the lowest income quintile (22.3% versus 16.1%, χ2 = 44.81, p < 0.001), had worked longer (mean 35.5 versus 31.3 years, F = 92.97, p < 0.001) and were less likely to be working at the start of study (48.5% versus 60.5%, χ2 = 93.78, p < 0.001), had slightly more symptoms of depression (mean 3.5 versus 3.3, F = 10.56, p = 0.001), were more likely to have impaired IADLs (7.5% versus 3.7%, χ2 = 48.42, p < 0.001), diabetes (22.1% versus 10.9%, χ2 = 162.23, p < 0.001), hypertension (40.5% versus 34.2%, χ2 = 27.54, p < 0.001) or stroke (2.9% versus 1.5%, χ2 = 18.19, p < 0.001). There was no significant difference with respect to the type of job (χ2 = 22.64, p = 0.124), whether they worked in similar jobs all their life (χ2 = 2.92, p = 0.087) and the number of years of education (F = 2.33, p = 0.127).

Cognitive function

Cognitive function in the MHAS is assessed using the Cross Cultural Cognitive Examination (CCCE; Glosser et al., Reference Glosser, Wolfe, Albert, Lavine, Steele, Calne and Schoenberg1993). The CCCE is preferred for populations with low levels of education or limited literacy and mathematical abilities (Zeki Al Hazzouri et al., Reference Zeki Al Hazzouri, Haan, Galea and Aiello2011). While the CCCE evaluates cognition across several domains, we limited our analyses to verbal learning, delayed verbal recall and visual scanning, and omitted other domains (visuospatial abilities, visual memory) with limited score ranges (range 0–2, possible scores 0/1/2, mean change over follow-up period 0.65 and 0.08). Verbal learning was assessed by having respondents recall an eight-word list three times and calculated by taking the average number of words recalled across attempts (range 0–8). Delayed recall was assessed by having respondents recall the eight-word list after a delay (range 0–8). Both tests measure episodic memory, one the learning aspect and one the recall aspect (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Gagneux-Zurbriggen, Berres, Taylor and Monsch2012), and are good predictors for progression to dementia (Belleville et al., Reference Belleville, Fouquet, Hudon, Zomahoun and Croteau2017). Visual scanning involves having respondents identify a stimulus in a visual array of different stimuli in one minute (range 0–60). The test on visual scanning measures selective attention (Eizenman et al., Reference Eizenman, Yu, Grupp, Eizenman, Ellenbogen, Gemar and Levitan2003). In regression analyses, the z-scores of the test scores were used for easier interpretation. As in this study, the CCCE is often analysed using the individual cognitive test scores and thus treats cognition as a continuous variable (e.g. Glosser et al., Reference Glosser, Wolfe, Kliner-Krenzel and Albert1994; Saenz et al., Reference Saenz, Wong and Ailshire2018). In general, the CCCE has demonstrated high sensitivity and specificity for dementia (Wolfe et al., Reference Wolfe, Imai, Otani, Nagatani, Hasegawa, Sugimoto, Tanaka, Kuroda, Glosser and Albert1992; Mejia-Arango and Gutierrez, Reference Mejia-Arango and Gutierrez2011).

Occupation and working status

During the interview, the participants were asked about their occupation. The answer that they provided to the question ‘What is the name of the office, profession or place where you worked in your main job?’ was categorised by the study team according to the Mexican classification of occupations of the Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía (National Institute of Statistics and Geography of Mexico, INEGI) into occupational types. The INEGI occupational classification was developed by and is used by the Mexican National Institute of Statistics. The study team of the MHAS provided the data already coded accordingly. For our analysis, we only combined occupational groups of few people to larger occupational groups (i.e. labourers = labourers, operators, professionals, security, artistic; supervisors = supervisors, directors, department heads). In addition, participants were asked ‘In this main job, most of your time were you self-employed?’ and ‘Regarding the activities that you normally do at work, would you consider them to be similar to or the same as the activities that you have performed most of your life?’ In the analyses, we will first report on the main job that the participants reported at baseline to have done throughout their life. The mixed-model analyses, however, will treat their main job as a time-varying variable and thus incorporate changes to the main job. Working status was assessed with the question ‘Last week did you work?’ Additionally, participants were asked ‘About how many hours do you work in a usual day?’, but large amounts of missing data prevented further analysis of this question.

Covariates

Covariates used in this analysis included age, sex, marital status, educational attainment, income and chronic health conditions. Educational attainment was based on years of formal education. In the analyses, the years of education that an individual completed were used as a continuous variable. Income was divided in quintiles for the purpose of analysis. Chronic health conditions were based on self-reported doctor-diagnosed cancer, respiratory illnesses, diabetes, hypertension and stroke. Depressive symptoms were ascertained using a nine-item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies – Depression (CES-D) scale (Radloff, Reference Radloff2016) which has been validated for older Mexican adults in previous work (Aguilar-Navarro et al., Reference Aguilar-Navarro, Fuentes-Cantu, Avila-Funes and Garcia-Mayo2007a). Limitations in IADLs were assessed by reporting difficulty in preparing meals, shopping, taking medications and managing money. Impaired IADLs were defined as at least one impairment in any IADL.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses employed an alpha level for statistical significance of 0.05 (two-tailed) and were performed using Stata 14.2. Descriptive analyses were conducted applying the population weights provided by the MHAS study research team.

As most of our covariates (gender, IADLs, diabetes, stroke, hypertension, CES-D, income and marital status) were significantly associated with the predictors as well as the dependent variable, we used a propensity score to combine the covariates (a commonly applied method; Cavuto et al., Reference Cavuto, Bravi, Grassi and Apolone2006; Fang et al., Reference Fang, Johnson, Chevalier, Stopp, Wiebe, Wakschlag and Espy2010; Biondi-Zoccai et al., Reference Biondi-Zoccai, Romagnoli, Agostoni, Capodanno, Castagno, D'Ascenzo, Sangiorgi and Modena2011). In this way, we avoid problems with multicollinearity in the final models. The propensity score was calculated via inverse-probability-weighted regression estimating potential-outcome means. The model for the propensity score included an outcome model to predict performance in each of the cognitive tests and a treatment model predicting the active participation in the labour force: a linear outcome model for the propensity score regressed gender, IADLs, diabetes, stroke, hypertension, CES-D, income and marital status on performance in all three cognitive tests; a multinominal logit treatment model regressed gender, IADLs, diabetes, stroke, hypertension, CES-D, income and marital status on working status (yes/no: indicator variable for being an active part of the workforce in each study year). The inverse-probability-weighted estimated means of the combined models reflect the propensity score.

Characteristics of the study sample were estimated via sampling weight-adjusted proportions and means using logit transformation. Significant differences by occupational group and by working status were tested using the Adjusted Wald Test for categorical variables and survey-based variance estimates for correlations for continuous variables. Associations between occupation, working status and cognitive performance at baseline were first estimated via the Adjusted Wald Test and then linear regression analysis adjusted for working status, education, age and propensity score. For regression analysis, production workers were used as the reference group because they were the largest occupational group who also had low levels of mental demands at work and low educational attainment, and because they made up the same amount among those who stopped working (17%) and those who continued working over the study period (17%).

Associations between occupation categories, working status and cognitive performance over the follow-up period (2001–2015) were estimated via mixed models using age as the time variable and with random effects for education and the propensity score. We also included a squared effect of the time variable in the model, as cognition is considered to decline in a non-linear way, and autoregressive residuals because cognitive performance is dependent on the cognitive performance from the previous timepoint. Model 1 focused on working status and did not include occupation; Model 2 included occupation. The advantage of using mixed models is that the models allow occupation and working status to vary across the follow-up points (i.e. the model included estimations if a participant changed his or her job). The variables for occupation and working status therefore specify for each assessment wave whether the participant was still participating in or had exited the workforce. As a form of sensitivity analysis, we repeated Model 2 including an additional three-way interaction effect for occupation with working status and with time.

Results

Of our sample, 53.6 per cent were men, 69.9 per cent were married, 4.4 per cent had impaired IADLs and the mean age at baseline was 60.5 years (standard deviation (SD) = 8.4, range = 50–105 years). The average years of education completed were 5.10 (SD = 4.58). Details of the socio-economic characteristics are shown in Table 1. Participation in the workforce at the start of the study in 2001 was 56.3 per cent (population-representative percentage, study N = 4,159). A total of 29.7 per cent of older Mexicans (population-representative percentage, study N = 1,943) were self-employed and 65.7 per cent had worked in similar occupations all their life. The distribution of employment types, socio-economic characteristics and work history by occupational groups are displayed in Table S2 in the online supplementary material.

Table 1. Socio-economic and demographic characteristics at study baseline (2001)

Notes: N = 7,375. CI: 95% confidence interval with sampling weights adjusted. IADLs: instrumental activities of daily living. 1. Among those working in 2001.

Exiting the workforce

In 2003, a total of N = 906 of those who were working in 2001 (N = 4,159) stopped working (population-representative 23.2%). By 2012, another N = 1,610 stopped working (population-representative 52.9%) and by 2015, an additional N = 484 participants (population-representative 33.9%) stopped working (details in Table S3 in the online supplementary material). However, 453 participants who had stopped working and 683 participants who were not working at baseline started working again at a later point of the study. There was no significant difference between occupational groups (details in Table S3 in the online supplementary material).

Cognitive performance at baseline

Verbal learning

An Adjusted Wald Test indicated that performance in verbal learning was significantly better among those who were female, did not have impaired IADLs, had not had a stroke, were married, had a higher income, were working a paid job and were not self-employed, but not for working actively compared to not working anymore (see Table 2). Significant correlations were observed between better verbal learning performance and lower CES-D score (−0.106, p < 0.001), younger age (−0.289, p < 0.001) and a higher education (0.349, p < 0.001). Results from a linear regression on verbal learning (z-score, adjusted for education and propensity score) indicated that those working actively had a significantly poorer performance (b = −0.099, p = 0.018) and, compared to production workers, those who worked in administration (b = 0.237, p = 0.014), sales (0.288, p < 0.001), as technicians (b = 0.268, p = 0.011) and as domestic workers (b = 0.140, p = 0.049) had significantly better performance (pairwise comparisons in Table S4 in the online supplementary material).

Table 2. Differences in cognition by participants’ characteristics at study baseline (2001)

Notes: N = 7,375. Range of scores in cognitive testing: verbal learning 0–8; delayed recall 0–8; visual scanning 0–60 (higher scores indicating a better performance). CI: 95% confidence interval with sampling weights adjusted. IADLs: instrumental activities of daily living. A/L: agriculture or livestock. 1. Before or during study. 2. A pairwise comparison was not run, see the results of the regression analyses in Table S4 in the online supplementary material. 3. Among those working in 2001.

Significance levels: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001 (Adjusted Wald Test).

Delayed recall

An Adjusted Wald Test indicated that performance in delayed recall was significantly better among females, those who did not have impaired IADLs, had not had a stroke, did not have diabetes, were married, with a higher income, and were not self-employed, but not for working actively compared to not working anymore (see Table 2). A better performance correlated significantly with a lower CES-D score (−0.048, p < 0.001), younger age (−0.241, p < 0.001) and higher education (0.189, p < 0.001). Results from a linear regression on delayed recall (z-score, adjusted for confounders) indicated that working actively was non-significant (b = −0.050, p = 0.247) and, compared to production workers, those who worked in sales (b = 0.188, p = 0.010), as supervisors/directors (b = 0.283, p = 0.006), domestic workers (b = 0.266, p = 0.001) and technicians (b = 0.339, p = 0.017) had significantly better performance (pairwise comparisons in Table S4 in the online supplementary material).

Visual scanning

An Adjusted Wald Test indicated that performance in visual scanning was significantly better among those who were actively working, did not have impaired IADLs, had not had a stroke, had a higher income and were not self-employed (see Table 2). Significant correlations were observed between better visual scanning performance and a lower CES-D score (−0.180, p < 0.001), younger age (−0.327, p < 0.001) and higher education (0.521, p < 0.001). Results from a linear regression of occupation on visual scanning (z-score, adjusted for confounders) indicated that working actively was non-significant (b = 0.045, p = 0.239) and, compared to production workers, those who worked as domestic workers (b = −0.336, p < 0.001), labourers (including operators, professionals, security and arts: b = −0.165, p = 0.032), and workers in agriculture and with livestock (b = −0.324, p < 0.001) had significantly worse visual scanning performance (pairwise comparisons in Table S4 in the online supplementary material).

Cognitive performance during follow-up period

Associations between occupation and actively working on cognitive performance over the study period (2001–2015) were estimated by mixed models with random effects for education and the propensity score (results and effect estimates are shown in Table 3). Treating working status as a time-varying variable, the results indicate that working actively (compared to exiting the workforce) was not associated with cognitive performance in Model 1 (trends for verbal learning χ2 = 0.43, p = 0.514; delayed recall χ2 = 3.07, p = 0.079; visual scanning χ2 = 2.56, p = 0.109). Only if occupation was added (Model 2) was working actively associated with a poorer performance in verbal learning and delayed recall, as well as a slower decline in all cognitive tests with ageing (see Table 3, trends for verbal learning χ2 = 13.55, p < 0.001; delayed recall χ2 = 16.00, p < 0.001; visual scanning χ2 = 1.78, p = 0.182). Figure 1A shows decline in performance in verbal learning and delayed recall of educators compared to production workers. Figure 1B shows decline of performance in visual scanning of technicians and of those who worked in agriculture and with livestock compared to production workers. Accordingly, Hypothesis 1 that working actively in older age comes with a slower cognitive decline cannot be confirmed in general, but seems to be occupation-specific.

Table 3. Estimates of the fixed-effect of the association of occupation and actively working in old age on cognitive performance over the study period (2001–2015); estimated by mixed models with random effects (not shown) for age, education and the propensity score with autoregressive residuals

Notes: N = 7,375. b: regression coefficient. CI: 95% confidence interval with sampling weights adjusted. AIC: Akaike information criterion. Ref.: reference category. A/L: agriculture or livestock. 1. Effects are conditional on ‘interaction effects’. 2. Including operators, professsionals, workers in arts and security. 3. Including directors and department heads. Random effects verbal learning: Model 1 education b = 0.00, CI = 0.00, 0.00, SE = 0.00; propensity score b = 0.42, CI = 0.00, 7,278.27, SE = 2.07. Model 2 education b = 0.00, CI = 0.00, 0.00, SE = 0.00; propensity score b = 2.15, CI = 0.15, 30.26, SE = 2.89. Random effects delayed recall: Model 1 education b = 0.00, CI = 0.00, 0.00, SE = 0.00; propensity score b = 4.03, CI = 1.34, 12.13, SE = 2.26. Model 2 education b = 0.00, CI = 0.00, 0.00, SE = 0.00; propensity score b = 9.15, CI = 4.13, 20.27, SE = 3.71. Random effects visual scanning: Model 1 education b = 0.001, CI = 0.01, 0.01, SE = 0.00; propensity score b = 0.00, CI = 0.00, 0.00, SE = 0.00. Model 2 education b = 0.00, CI = 0.00, 0.00, SE = 0.00; propensity score b = 0.00, CI = 0.00, 0.00, SE = 0.00.

Significance levels: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

Figure 1. Predicted score in the cognitive tests by working status and occupation, as predicted by the mixed models using year as time and with random effects for education and the propensity score.

Note: A/L: agriculture or livestock.

To test for interaction effects of working status with occupation, we repeated the previous models of cognitive performance over the follow-up period including a three-way interaction for occupation with working status and with time (results in Table S5 in the online supplementary material). Results suggest that, compared to production workers, domestic workers had a significantly faster decline in verbal learning (b = −0.02, p = 0.020) and in delayed recall (b = −0.02, p = 0.036) if they continued working actively in contrast to exiting the workforce (see Figure 2A). Results also suggested that, compared to production workers, those working in administration (b = 0.03, p = 0.007), sales (b = 0.02, p = 0.044) and educators (b = 0.03, p = 0.049) had a slower decline in visual scanning if they continued working (see Figure 2B). Adding a random slope for occupation to the model confirmed these findings (results not shown). Accordingly, the results confirm Hypothesis 2 that manual or non-intellectual occupations have a faster cognitive decline only with respect to visual scanning but not with respect to verbal learning and delayed recall. The results also suggest that Hypothesis 3 is correct, indicating a domain-specific effect of occupational types.

Figure 2. Predicted score in the visual scanning task by working status and occupation in interaction with ageing, as predicted by the mixed models using age as the time variable and with random effects for education and the propensity score.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to examine whether participation versus exiting the labour force in old age affects cognitive functioning among older Mexican adults, and to what extent the type of occupation moderates this association. Our results from longitudinal observations of a population-based study suggest that participating in the workforce in general is not associated with cognitive functioning in ageing (Hypothesis 1); but for some occupations working longer might slow down the decline in memory (verbal learning and delayed recall). Further, the results do not confirm the general notion that manual or non-intellectual occupations have a faster cognitive decline (Hypothesis 2) as we observe this trend only with respect to visual scanning but not with respect to verbal learning and delayed recall. Accordingly, our findings emphasise the relevance of domain-specific effects of occupational types (Hypothesis 3), indicating that the ‘use it or lose it hypothesis’ is domain-specific (i.e. only those cognitive abilities used at work are maintained better). An example of this is the finding that workers in sales, administration and education who continued participating in the workforce at an older age had a significantly slower decline in visual scanning compared to those who dropped out of the labour force. Future studies should investigate the level of visual scanning, verbal learning and delayed recall in the occupational setting in order to gain a better understanding of their association with cognitive decline in old age. Previous studies have investigated the level of mental demands (Dartigues et al., Reference Dartigues, Gagnon, Letenneur, Barberger-Gateau, Commenges, Evaldre and Salamon1992a; Then et al., Reference Then, Luck, Heser, Ernst, Posselt, Wiese, Mamone, Brettschneider, Konig, Weyerer, Werle, Mosch, Bickel, Fuchs, Pentzek, Maier, Scherer, Wagner and Riedel-Heller2017) and job complexity (Andel et al., Reference Andel, Crowe, Pedersen, Mortimer, Crimmins, Johansson and Gatz2005; Smart et al., Reference Smart, Gow and Deary2014), and observed differential effects with respect to the type of intellectual demands faced at work. An important indication from our study findings for public health is that decisions for optimal retirement ages should be occupation-specific as working longer should only come with a slower cognitive decline if the person is actively using cognitive resources.

The cognitive tests used in this study assess memory in terms of verbal learning, and delayed recall and attention in terms of visual scanning. Verbal memory performance is the strongest predictor of dementia (Prado et al., Reference Prado, Watt, Treeby and Crowe2019). In our study, working actively was associated with a poorer performance as well as a slower decline in an occupation-specific context. Especially domestic workers who worked actively had a significantly faster memory decline. Domestic workers might experience high strain in their job (instead of intellectual stimulation) so that working longer in later life accelerates decline of memory abilities (Agbenyikey et al., Reference Agbenyikey, Karasek, Cifuentes, Wolf, Seshadri, Taylor, Beiser and Au2015). Visual scanning, on the other hand, involves selective attention (Eizenman et al., Reference Eizenman, Yu, Grupp, Eizenman, Ellenbogen, Gemar and Levitan2003), and performance in visual scanning typically decreases with age (Trick and Enns, Reference Trick and Enns1998). Slowing the decline of visual scanning might signify a slowing of age-related cognitive decline. Our results from a population-based sample suggest that working actively in later life as a worker in sales, administration and education might have that effect.

In Mexico, the prevalence of dementia is around 6 per cent, and of those with dementia, 90 per cent had less than six years of education (Mejia-Arango and Gutierrez, Reference Mejia-Arango and Gutierrez2011). Low levels of education, rural living conditions and low income (Miu et al., Reference Miu, Negin, Salinas-Rodriguez, Manrique-Espinoza, Sosa-Ortiz, Cumming and Kowal2016), a result also of the economic crises in the 1980s and 1990s, led to the poorer health of older Mexican people (Cutler et al., Reference Cutler, Knaul, Lozano, Mendez and Zurita2002) that may subsequently increase the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia. With the lack of a comprehensive retirement system, many Mexicans may be forced to work in old age despite poor health. This, however, does not always help to maintain their cognitive functioning, as our results show. Hence, for many Mexicans, it means completing the same work tasks in later life and with declining cognitive abilities.

The strengths of our study are the representative population-based sample that included rural areas in Mexico as well as the longitudinal standardised observations. Nonetheless, our analysis comes with several limitations that are worth mentioning. First, we were unable to investigate cognitive function among study participants who required proxies as these respondents do not receive the CCCE. We also had to exclude from the analyses many participants with missing data. Excluded participants were more likely to be sick or have less education so that our sample may then exclude those in the population with poorest cognitive abilities. Conclusions drawn from the analysis cannot be generalised for population groups with low education and with more chronic health problems. Second, occupation is a difficult construct to measure as occupations may change throughout the lifecourse. Our analyses were based on the main job respondents worked and their current job, among those working during the study. Recall bias due to self-reports may have affected the results. Third, due to the long gap between the 2003 and 2012 waves of the MHAS, it was difficult to estimate a cubic decline in cognition over follow-up. Fourth, survival bias may have affected the results. Hence, we can only draw conclusions for those people who achieve a higher age. Fifth, we cannot exclude the possibility of reverse causation, particularly that participation in the labour force may be influenced by cognitive abilities. Sixth, occupational types do not provide specific information on job characteristics such as mental demands and stress, which could play a role, and we recommend that future studies explore these detailed occupational characteristics. Moreover, given the large number of statistical tests, there is an increased probability for incorrectly rejecting the null hypothesis. Finally, we cannot be sure that the cognitive tests we used were sensitive enough to capture the changes in cognition due to occupation and working status.

The decision to continue working in old age might not necessarily lead to beneficial effects for cognitive functioning in older people in Mexico as many Mexicans are employed in occupations that do not provide this benefit. Policy decisions should therefore focus on risk factors that drive dementia risk. Specific factors in the occupational context may determine this risk. Further studies are necessary to devote effort to this topic.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X2100012X

Acknowledgement

The findings of this study were presented at the GSA 2018 in Boston.

Author contributions

FSR and JS made equal contributions to conceptualising the study, planning the analysis and interpreting the data. FSR conducted the data analysis, wrote the first and further drafts of the manuscript, and agreed to the final version of the manuscript. JS critically revised the content of the data analysis and any further versions of the manuscript, and agreed to the final version of the manuscript.

Financial support

This work was supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG) (FSR, grant number TH2137/3-1); Hans and Ilse Breuer Stiftung (FSR); and the National Institutes of Health (NIH) (JS, grant number K99AG058799 and R00 AG058799). The Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS) was funded by the NIH (grant number R01AG018016). The sponsors had no role in the design, methods, subject recruitment, data collections, analysis and preparation of the paper.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Ethical standards

The Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS) study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board or Ethics Committee of the University of Texas Medical Branch, the Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía (INEGI) in Mexico and the Instituto Nacional de Salud Pública (INSP) in Mexico. The study was conducted in accordance with the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. Participants gave their written informed consent.

References

Agbenyikey, W, Karasek, R, Cifuentes, M, Wolf, PA, Seshadri, S, Taylor, JA, Beiser, AS and Au, R (2015) Job strain and cognitive decline: a prospective study of the Framingham Offspring cohort. International Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine 6, 7994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aguilar-Navarro, SG, Fuentes-Cantu, A, Avila-Funes, JA and Garcia-Mayo, EJ (2007 a) Validity and reliability of the screening questionnaire for geriatric depression used in the Mexican Health and Age Study. Salud Pública de México 49, 256262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aguilar-Navarro, SG, Reyes-Guerrero, J and Borgues, G (2007 b) Cognitive impairment and alcohol and cigarette consumption in Mexican adults older than 65 years. Salud Pública de México 49, 467474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andel, R, Crowe, M, Pedersen, NL, Mortimer, J, Crimmins, E, Johansson, B and Gatz, M (2005) Complexity of work and risk of Alzheimer's disease: a population-based study of Swedish twins. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 60, P251P258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anttila, T, Helkala, EL, Kivipelto, M, Hallikainen, M, Alhainen, K, Heinonen, H, Mannermaa, A, Tuomilehto, J, Soininen, H and Nissinen, A (2002) Midlife income, occupation, APOE status, and dementia: a population-based study. Neurology 59, 887893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes-Farrell, JL (2003) Beyond health and wealth: attitudinal and other influences on retirement decision-making. In Adams, GA and Beehr, TA (eds), Retirement: Reasons, Processes, and Results. New York, NY: Springer, pp. 159187.Google Scholar
Beck, IR, Gagneux-Zurbriggen, A, Berres, M, Taylor, KI and Monsch, AU (2012) Comparison of verbal episodic memory measures: Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease-Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (CERAD-NAB) versus California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27, 510519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belleville, S, Fouquet, C, Hudon, C, Zomahoun, HTV, Croteau, J and Consortium for the Early Identification of Alzheimer's disease-Quebec (2017) Neuropsychological measures that predict progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's type dementia in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology Review 27, 328353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Besamusca, J, Tijdens, K, Keune, M and Steinmetz, S (2015) Working women worldwide. Age effects in female labor force participation in 117 countries. World Development 74, 123141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biondi-Zoccai, G, Romagnoli, E, Agostoni, P, Capodanno, D, Castagno, D, D'Ascenzo, F, Sangiorgi, G and Modena, MG (2011) Are propensity scores really superior to standard multivariable analysis? Contemporary Clinical Trials 32, 731740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calvo, E, Bertranou, FM and Bertranou, E (2010) Are old-age pension system reforms moving away from individual retirement accounts in Latin America? Journal of Social Policy 39, 223234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carr, DC, Willis, R, Kail, BL and Carstensen, LL (2020) Alternative retirement paths and cognitive performance: exploring the role of preretirement job complexity. The Gerontologist 60, 460471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavuto, S, Bravi, F, Grassi, M and Apolone, G (2006) Propensity score for the analysis of observational data: an introduction and an illustrative example. Drug Development Research 67, 208216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Consejo Nacional de Población (2004) Envejecimiento de la Población de México: Reto del siglo XXI. Mexico, Mexico: Consejo Nacional de Población.Google Scholar
Cosío, MEZ and Ferreiro, J (1992) Cambios de fecundidad en México y políticas de población. Tlalpan, Mexico: El Colegio de Mexico and Fondo de Cultura Economica.Google Scholar
Cutler, DM, Knaul, F, Lozano, R, Mendez, O and Zurita, B (2002) Financial crisis, health outcomes and ageing: Mexico in the 1980s and 1990s. Journal of Public Economics 84, 279303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dartigues, JF, Gagnon, M, Letenneur, L, Barberger-Gateau, P, Commenges, D, Evaldre, M and Salamon, R (1992 a) Principal lifetime occupation and cognitive impairment in a French elderly cohort (Paquid). American Journal of Epidemiology 135, 981988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dartigues, J-F, Gagnon, M, Mazaux, J, Barberger-Gateau, P, Commenges, D, Letenneur, L and Orgogozo, J (1992 b) Occupation during life and memory performance in nondemented French elderly community residents. Neurology 42, 16971701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eizenman, M, Yu, LH, Grupp, L, Eizenman, E, Ellenbogen, M, Gemar, M and Levitan, RD (2003) A naturalistic visual scanning approach to assess selective attention in major depressive disorder. Psychiatric Research 118, 117128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fang, H, Johnson, C, Chevalier, N, Stopp, C, Wiebe, S, Wakschlag, LS and Espy, KA (2010) Using propensity score modeling to minimize the influence of confounding risks related to prenatal tobacco exposure. Nicotine & Tobacco Research 12, 12111219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkel, D, Andel, R, Gatz, M and Pedersen, NL (2009) The role of occupational complexity in trajectories of cognitive aging before and after retirement. Psychology and Aging 24, 563573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, GG, Stachowski, A, Infurna, FJ, Faul, JD, Grosch, J and Tetrick, LE (2014) Mental work demands, retirement, and longitudinal trajectories of cognitive functioning. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology 19, 231242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gasparini, L and Tornarolli, L (2009) Labor informality in Latin America and the Caribbean: patterns and trends from household survey microdata. Revista Desarrollo y Sociedad 63, 1380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glosser, G, Wolfe, N, Albert, ML, Lavine, L, Steele, JC, Calne, DB and Schoenberg, BS (1993) Cross-Cultural Cognitive Examination – validation of a dementia screening instrument for neuroepidemiological research. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 41, 931939.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glosser, G, Wolfe, N, Kliner-Krenzel, L and Albert, ML (1994) Cross-Cultural Cognitive Examination performance in patients with Parkinson's disease and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 182, 432436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grotz, C, Meillon, C, Amieva, H, Stern, Y, Dartigues, JF, Adam, S and Letenneur, L (2015) Why is later age at retirement beneficial for cognition? Results from a French population-based study. Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging 20, 514519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmer, C, Letenneur, L, Rouch, I, Richard-Harston, S, Barberger-Gateau, P, Fabrigoule, C, Orgogozo, J and Dartigues, J (2001) Occupation during life and risk of dementia in French elderly community residents. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 71, 303309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurd, MD, Martorell, P, Delavande, A, Mullen, KJ and Langa, KM (2013) Monetary costs of dementia in the United States. New England Journal of Medicine 368, 13261334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía (2017) Resultados de la encuesta nacional de ocupación y empleo. Cifras durante el cuarto trimestre de 2016. Aguascalientes, Mexico: Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía.Google Scholar
Jorm, AF, Rodgers, B, Henderson, AS, Korten, AE, Jacomb, PA, Christensen, H and Mackinnon, A (1998) Occupation type as a predictor of cognitive decline and dementia in old age. Age and Ageing 27, 477483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langa, KM, Chernew, ME, Kabeto, MU, Herzog, AR, Ofstedal, MB, Willis, RJ, Wallace, RB, Mucha, LM, Straus, WL and Fendrick, AM (2001) National estimates of the quantity and cost of informal caregiving for the elderly with dementia. Journal of General Internal Medicine 16, 770778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupton, MK, Stahl, D, Archer, N, Foy, C, Poppe, M, Lovestone, S, Hollingworth, P, Williams, J, Owen, MJ and Dowzell, K (2010) Education, occupation and retirement age effects on the age of onset of Alzheimer's disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 25, 3036.Google ScholarPubMed
Mejia-Arango, S and Gutierrez, LM (2011) Prevalence and incidence rates of dementia and cognitive impairment on dementia in the Mexican population: data from the Mexican Health and Aging Study. Journal of Aging and Health 23, 10501074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miu, J, Negin, J, Salinas-Rodriguez, A, Manrique-Espinoza, B, Sosa-Ortiz, AL, Cumming, R and Kowal, P (2016) Factors associated with cognitive function in older adults in Mexico. Global Health Action 9, 30747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prado, CE, Watt, S, Treeby, MS and Crowe, SF (2019) Performance on neuropsychological assessment and progression to dementia: a meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging 34, 954977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qiu, C, Karp, A, von Strauss, E, Winblad, B, Fratiglioni, L and Bellander, T (2003) Lifetime principal occupation and risk of Alzheimer's disease in the Kungsholmen project. American Journal of Industrial Medicine 43, 204211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, LS (2016) The CES-D scale. Applied Psychological Measurement 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saenz, JL, Downer, B, Garcia, MA and Wong, R (2017) Cognition and context: rural–urban differences in cognitive aging among older Mexican adults. Journal of Aging and Health 30, 965986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saenz, JL, Wong, R and Ailshire, JA (2018) Indoor air pollution and cognitive function among older Mexican adults. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 72, 2126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, TA (2006) Mental exercise and mental aging evaluating the validity of the ‘use it or lose it’ hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science 1, 6887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, TA (2009) When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging 30, 507514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smart, EL, Gow, AJ and Deary, IJ (2014) Occupational complexity and lifetime cognitive abilities. Neurology 83, 22852291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Staff, RT, Hogan, MJ, Williams, DS and Whalley, LJ (2018) Intellectual engagement and cognitive ability in later life (the ‘use it or lose it’ conjecture): longitudinal, prospective study. BMJ 363, k4925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swaab, DF (1991) Brain aging and Alzheimer's disease, ‘wear and tear’ versus ‘use it or lose it’. Neurobiology of Aging 12, 317324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Then, FS, Luck, T, Luppa, M, Thinschmidt, M, Deckert, S, Nieuwenhuijsen, K, Seidler, A and Riedel-Heller, SG (2014) Systematic review of the effect of the psychosocial working environment on cognition and dementia. Occupational and Environmental Medicine 71, 358365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Then, FS, Luck, T, Heser, K, Ernst, A, Posselt, T, Wiese, B, Mamone, S, Brettschneider, C, Konig, HH, Weyerer, S, Werle, J, Mosch, E, Bickel, H, Fuchs, A, Pentzek, M, Maier, W, Scherer, M, Wagner, M and Riedel-Heller, SG (2017) Which types of mental work demands may be associated with reduced risk of dementia? Alzheimers & Dementia 13, 431440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trick, LM and Enns, JT (1998) Lifespan changes in attention: the visual search task. Cognitive Development 13, 369386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tuesta, D (2017) Retirement and labour markets under the context of pension reform in Latin America. Economic and Political Studies 5, 475500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfe, N, Imai, Y, Otani, C, Nagatani, H, Hasegawa, K, Sugimoto, K, Tanaka, Y, Kuroda, Y, Glosser, G and Albert, ML (1992) Criterion validity of the cross-cultural cognitive examination in Japan. Journal of Gerontology 47, P289P291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, R and Palloni, A (2009) Aging in Mexico and Latin America. In Uhlenberg, P (ed.), International Handbook of Population Aging. New York, NY: Springer, pp. 231252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, R, Michaels-Obregon, A and Palloni, A (2015 a) Cohort profile: the Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS). International Journal of Epidemiology 46, e2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, R, Michaels-Obregon, A, Palloni, A, Gutierrez-Robledo, LM, Gonzalez-Gonzalez, C, Lopez-Ortega, M, Tellez-Rojo, MM and Mendoza-Alvarado, LR (2017) Progression of aging in Mexico: the Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS) 2012. Salud Pública de México 57, S79S89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Bank (2017) Employment in Agriculture. International Labour Organization, Key Indicators of the Labour Market Database. Washington, DC: World Bank.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2016) Mental Health and Older Adults. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Zeki Al Hazzouri, A, Haan, MN, Galea, S and Aiello, AE (2011) Life-course exposure to early socioeconomic environment, education in relation to late-life cognitive function among older Mexicans and Mexican Americans. Journal of Aging and Health 23, 10271049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Socio-economic and demographic characteristics at study baseline (2001)

Figure 1

Table 2. Differences in cognition by participants’ characteristics at study baseline (2001)

Figure 2

Table 3. Estimates of the fixed-effect of the association of occupation and actively working in old age on cognitive performance over the study period (2001–2015); estimated by mixed models with random effects (not shown) for age, education and the propensity score with autoregressive residuals

Figure 3

Figure 1. Predicted score in the cognitive tests by working status and occupation, as predicted by the mixed models using year as time and with random effects for education and the propensity score.Note: A/L: agriculture or livestock.

Figure 4

Figure 2. Predicted score in the visual scanning task by working status and occupation in interaction with ageing, as predicted by the mixed models using age as the time variable and with random effects for education and the propensity score.

Supplementary material: File

Rodriguez and Saenz supplementary material

Tables S1-S5

Download Rodriguez and Saenz supplementary material(File)
File 70.7 KB