Published online by Cambridge University Press: 14 November 2008
In the present paper we analyse some of the problems in talk to elders by examining evaluative dimensions of talk in general and of address forms in particular. We first introduce status and solidarity as primary dimensions underlying talk. We briefly review research on attitudes and talk to elders, and develop a framework in terms of status and solidarity for understanding the evaluation of talk to elders. The framework, speech accommodation theory, and politeness theory are then employed in a specific analysis of various aspects of problematic address usage: title and last name v. first name; third person; first person plural; and no-naming. The paper concludes with a discussion of the implications of the analysis for future research, for practical applications, and for the extension and integration of theory
1 Lonner, W. J., The search for psychological universals. In Triandis, H. C. and Lambert, W. W. (eds) Handbook of Cross-cultural Psychology; Vol. 1. Perspectives, pp. 143–204. Allyn & Bacon, Rockleigh, NJ., 1980.Google Scholar
2 Brown, R., Social Psychology. Free Press, New York, 1965.Google Scholar
3 Brown, P. and Levinson, S. C., Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4 For example, Brown, P. and Levinson, 1987, op cit.;Google ScholarDowd, J. J., Conversation and social exchange: managing identities in old age. Human Relations, 34 (1981), 541–53;CrossRefGoogle ScholarRyan, E. B. and Giles, H. (eds) Attitudes Towards Language Variation: Social and Applied Contexts. Edward Arnold, London, 1982.Google Scholar
5 Ryan, and Giles, , 1982, op. cit.Google Scholar
6 Brown, R., 1965, op. cit.Google Scholar
7 Kogan, N., Beliefs, attitudes and stereotypes about old people. Research on Aging, 1 (1979), 11–36;CrossRefGoogle ScholarMcTavish, D. G., Perceptions of old people. In Mangen, D.J. and Peterson, W. A. (eds) Research Instruments in Social Gerontology: Vol. 1. Clinical and Social Gerontology (pp. 533–622). University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, 1982.Google Scholar
8 Kite, M. E. and Johnson, B. T., Attitudes toward older and younger adults: a meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging. 3 (1988), 233–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 See also Crockett, W. H. and Hummert, M. L., Perceptions of aging and the elderly. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 7 (1987), 217–41.Google ScholarPubMed
10 See Harris, L. and Associates. The Myth and Reality of Aging in America. The National Council on Aging, Washington, D.C., 1975.Google Scholar
11 Ryan, E. B. and Heaven, R. K. B., The impact of situational context on agebased attitudes. Social Behaviour, 3 (1988), 105–17.Google Scholar
12 Ashburn, G. and Gordon, A., Features of a simplified register in speech to elderly conversationalists. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 8 (1981), 7–31;Google ScholarCaporael, L., The paralanguage of caregiving: baby talk to the institutionalized aged. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40 (1981), 876–84;CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMedCoupland, N., Coupland, J., Giles, H. and Henwood, K., Accommodating the elderly: invoking and extending a theory. Language in Society, 17 (1988), 1–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13 Coupland, et al. , 1988, op. cit.;Google ScholarRyan, E. B., Giles, H., Bartolucci, G. and Henwood, K., Psycholinguistic and social psychological components of communication by and with the elderly. Language and Communication, 6 (1986), 1–24;CrossRefGoogle ScholarShadden, B. B., Interpersonal communication patterns and strategies in the elderly. In Shadden, B. (ed) Communication Behavior and Aging. A Sourcebook for Clinicians, pp. 182–96. Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore, 1988.Google Scholar
14 Rodin, J. and Langer, E. J., Aging labels: the decline of control and the fall of selfesteem. Journal of Social Issues, 36 (1980), 12–29;CrossRefGoogle ScholarRyan, et al. , 1986, op. cit.Google Scholar
15 Caporael, 1981, op. cit.;Google ScholarCaporael, L. R. and Culbertson, G. H., Verbal response modes of baby talk and other speech at institutions for the aged. Language and Communication, 6, 1/2 (1986), 99–112;CrossRefGoogle ScholarCaporael, L., Lukaszewski, M. and Culbertson, C., Secondary baby talk: judgments by institutionalized elderly and their caregivers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44 (1983), 746–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16 Caporael, , 1981, op. cit.Google Scholar
17 1983, op. cit.
18 Ryan, E. B. and Cole, R., Evaluative perceptions of interpersonal communication with elders. In Giles, H., Coupland, N. and Wiemann, J. (eds) Communication, Health, and the Elderly, pp. 172–90. University of Manchester Press, Manchester, 1990.Google Scholar
19 Ryan, et al. , 1986, op. cit.Google Scholar
20 Ryan, E. B., Bourhis, R. Y. and Knops, U., Evaluative perceptions of patronizing speech addressed to elders. Psychology and Aging (in press).Google Scholar
21 1983, op. cit.
22 Coupland, N., Grainger, K. and Coupland, J., Politeness in context: intergenerational issues (Review article). Language in Society, 17 (1988), 253–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23 In order to simplify the Table, we have omitted speech that is inappropriate to the solidarity dimension or to both dimensions of the relationships. Speech that is inappropriate on solidarity alone would be evaluated as too personal or too impersonal. Speech that is inappropriate on both dimensions would call for slightly different evaluative terms than speech that is inappropriate only on the status dimension. For example, while secondary baby talk in an equal solidarity relationship might be viewed as paternalistic, it would be seen as presumptuous in an equal nonsolidary relationship. We note further that the terms we have suggested are preliminary and should be taken only as illustrative. They are based primarily on etymological considerations and might be used rarely or differently in everyday talk.
24 Lanceley, A., Use of controlling language in the rehabilitation of the elderly. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 10 (1985), 125–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25 1983, op. cit.
26 See Ryan, Bourhis and Knops, in press, op. cit.
27 Brown, P. and Levinson, , 1987, op. cit.Google Scholar
28 Kroger, R. O., Explorations in ethogeny: with special reference to the rules of address. American Psychologist, 37 (1982), 810–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29 Brown, R. Theory of politeness: an exemplary case. Invited address to Society of Experimental Social Psychologists, 10, 1987.Google Scholar
30 Variations of first name, e.g. multiple names (MN) and nicknames (NN), indicate variations in intimacy or solidarity. Solidary superiors who are not kin may be addressed by KT+ (e.g. ‘Aunt’ Joan to a family friend) or by forms such as TFN (e.g. Miss Sally).
31 Brown, R. and Ford, M., Address in American English. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62 (1961), 375–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
32 Brown, R., 1965, op. cit.Google Scholar
33 Kroger, R. O., Wood, L. A. and Kim, U., Are the rules of address universal? III. Comparison of Chinese, Greek, and Korean usage. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15 (1984), 273–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
34 For example, Lanceley, , 1985, op. cit.Google Scholar
35 Roman, O., Negotiation between nurses and elderly patients in hospital and community settings. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto, 1987.Google Scholar
36 Brown, P. and Levinson, 1987, op. cit.Google Scholar
37 As Brown, P. and Levinson, (1987, op. cit.) note, most forms of address are also forms of reference; the important distinction is usage.Google Scholar
38 Elliot, E., My name is MrsSimon, . Ladies Home Journal (1984, 08), pp. 18–21, 150.Google Scholar
39 Brown, R., 1965, op. cit.;Google ScholarBrown, P. and Levinson, , 1987, op. cit.Google Scholar
40 Brown, R., 1965, op. cit.Google Scholar
41 Ervin-Tripp, S., Sociolinguistics. In Berkowitz, L. (ed) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 4, pp. 91–165. Academic Press, New York, 1969.Google Scholar
42 Brown, R. and Gilman, A., The pronouns of power and solidarity. In Sebeok, T. A. (ed) Style in Language, pp. 253–76. MIT Press, Cambridge, 1960.Google Scholar
43 Brown, P. and Levinson, , 1987, op. cit.Google Scholar
44 Ibid.
45 See Ryan, and Cole, , 1990, op. cit.Google Scholar
46 Brown, R., 1965, op. cit.;Google ScholarKroger, , 1982, op. cit.Google Scholar
47 Brown, R., 1965, op. cit.;Google ScholarErvin-Tripp, , 1969, op. cit.Google Scholar
48 1987, op. cit.
49 Goe, R. M., Communication and medical care outcomes: analysis of conversations between doctors and elderly patients. In Ward, R. A. and Tobin, S. S. (eds) Health in Aging: Sociological Issues and Policy Directions, pp. 180–93. Springer, New York, 1987.Google Scholar
50 Brown, P. and Levinson, , 1987, op. cit.Google Scholar
51 Ibid.
52 Ibid.
53 Banks, S. P., Power pronouns and the language of intercultural understanding. In Ting-Toomey, S. and Korzenny, F. (eds) Language, Communication, and Culture: Current Directions, pp. 180–98. Sage, Newbury Park, Ca., 1989.Google Scholar
54 1985, op. cit.
55 In some languages, the plural form to one person can convey distance or respect, as in the use of the second person plural pronoun (Brown, P. and Levinson, , 1987, op. cit.). But in English, pluralisation may signal that the person is viewed as a member of a devalued category, i.e. as lower in status than the speaker.Google Scholar
56 Brown, R., 1965, op. cit.;Google ScholarLittle, C. B. and Gelles, R. J., The social psychological implications of form of address. Sociometry, 38 (1975), 573–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
57 1965, op. cit.
58 Ryan, et al. , 1986, op. cit.Google Scholar
58 1987, op. cit.
60 Dowd, , 1981, op. cit.Google Scholar
61 Wood, L. A., Kroger, R. O. and Leong, I., Social competence and the rules of address. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 5 (1986), 161–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
62 For example, Brown, R., 1965, op. cit.;Google ScholarKemper, T. D., A Social Interactional Theory of Emotions. Wiley, New York, 1978;Google ScholarWood, L. A., Loneliness and social identity. In Sarbin, T. R. and Scheibe, K. E. (eds) Studies in Social Identity, 51–70. Praeger, New York, 1983.Google Scholar
63 Cf., Caporael, 1981, op. cit.;Google ScholarRyan, et al. , 1986, op. cit.Google Scholar