Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T09:35:57.994Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social engagement and wellbeing in late life: a systematic review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2024

Joana Miguelote Monteiro*
Affiliation:
Center for Psychology at University of Porto, Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences at University of Porto, Porto, Portugal School of Education, Polytechnic Institute of Viana do Castelo, Viana do Castelo, Portugal
Raquel Gonçalves
Affiliation:
School of Education, Polytechnic Institute of Viana do Castelo, Viana do Castelo, Portugal Centre for Public Administration and Public Policies, Institute of Social and Political Science, Lisbon University, Lisbon, Portugal
Alice Bastos
Affiliation:
School of Education, Polytechnic Institute of Viana do Castelo, Viana do Castelo, Portugal AgeingC, CINTESIS-RISE, Porto, Portugal
Maria Raquel Barbosa
Affiliation:
Center for Psychology at University of Porto, Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences at University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
*
Corresponding author: Joana Monteiro; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Social engagement is considered a relevant modifiable factor for older adults' wellbeing. Theory and policies highlight its importance for ageing well. Empirical evidence shows that social activities are associated with positive psychological outcomes and might buffer wellbeing declines in late life. Despite growing research, social engagement lacks conceptual clarity, it is difficult to adopt standardised measures and findings are sometimes inconsistent. Previous systematic reviews either take a different approach to this topic or were published over a decade ago. Therefore, the present study aimed to review the literature systematically regarding the relationship between social engagement and wellbeing in community-dwelling older adults. Papers published from 2000 to 2021 were searched in five databases using a combination of terms. The reviewers screened the records according to predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria. After identifying eligible articles, the authors extracted data and produced a narrative synthesis covering conceptualisation, measurement and main findings. The review includes 42 papers. Despite great conceptual and methodological diversity, research supports that older adults with higher participation in social activities have improved wellbeing. Findings also suggest that these associations are stronger for individuals with disadvantages and have a cumulative nature whereby the greater the social engagement, the higher the wellbeing. Conversely, for more demanding activities, there might be optimal participation levels. Regardless of accumulating knowledge, social engagement remains diffuse and difficult to measure. This paper summarises the current state of research on this topic, showing encouraging evidence of social engagement benefits, but also questions that deserve further inquiry. Future studies should be anchored in a clear conceptual framework, use robust measures, and explore hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing. Social engagement can be an important developmental resource for social interventions and policies aimed at improving people's lives.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

It really seems no man is an island. Theory, empirical findings and policy all point to social engagement as a determining factor in the wellbeing of older adults. Late life can bring particular challenges concerning social relationships and activities, but these are modifiable. In other words, if social engagement is a way to improve people's lives, we can take action to facilitate it.

Research concerning social engagement in late life has considerably multiplied in the last two decades. However, scientific endeavours face a lack of conceptual consensus, obstacles in developing standardised measures and some inconsistency in findings, making it difficult to grasp the available evidence. Previous systematic reviews either take a different approach to this topic or were published over a decade ago. Therefore, this study aimed to review the literature systematically regarding the relationship between social engagement and wellbeing in community-dwelling older adults.

Social engagement and ageing well

Social activity has received much attention as a defining aspect of human existence. There are many terms for it, sometimes used interchangeably, including social participation, social involvement and social engagement, with several authors proposing conceptual definitions. Levasseur et al. (Reference Levasseur, Richard, Gauvin and Raymond2010) define social participation as involvement in activities that provide interaction with others in society or the community. These authors propose a taxonomy of social activities based on activities' goals and level of involvement with others. There are six levels in this taxonomy: (1) performing activities in preparation to connect with others; (2) being alone but with people around; (3) interacting with others without performing an activity with them; (4) doing an activity with others; (5) helping others; and (6) contributing to society. Social participation corresponds to levels 3–6, including social engagement, which matches levels 5 and 6. Based on this and other contributions (Bukov et al., Reference Bukov, Maas and Lampert2002; Levasseur et al., Reference Levasseur, Richard, Gauvin and Raymond2010; Scharlach and Lehning, Reference Scharlach and Lehning2016; Aroogh and Shahboulaghi, Reference Aroogh and Shahboulaghi2020), we understand social engagement as participating in activities embedded in social interactions that occur in a collective setting and require sharing individual resources.

While being socially engaged is vital throughout our lifespan, it has particular relevance in old age. During later life, social losses become more prominent due to changes such as retirement, death of family and friends, or children leaving home (Scharlach and Lehning, Reference Scharlach and Lehning2016; Windsor et al., Reference Windsor, Curtis, Luszcz, Kendig, McDonald and Piggot2016; Bruggencate et al., Reference Bruggencate, Luijkx and Sturm2018). This means that remaining socially engaged might be especially challenging for older people. Yet, several successful ageing models highlight that social relationships and roles are essential factors of a good old age. Firstly, in the MacArthur Model (Rowe and Kahn, Reference Rowe and Kahn1997), engagement with life is one of three elements of successful ageing. According to the authors, engagement with life entails the maintenance of interpersonal relations and meaningful activities that foster closeness, social support and purpose. Secondly, the Preventive and Corrective Proactivity Model (Kahana and Kahana, Reference Kahana, Kahana and Bengtson1996) posits valued activities and relationships as successful ageing outcomes, and socially oriented behaviours (e.g. helping others, role substitution) as proactive mechanisms to age well.

Simultaneously, ageing policies also incorporate social engagement, even though under different designations. Participation is one of the United Nations' principles for older people (United Nations, 1991) and one of the active ageing pillars (World Health Organization, 2002). These policy frameworks recognise that social activity promotes a sense of belonging, purpose in life and positive relations.

Globally, conceptual and theoretical contributions display social engagement as a construct with two main dimensions: social exchanges and meaningful activities. Moreover, social engagement is a hierarchic construct, moving from less-complex and self-focused expressions to more-demanding and other-oriented activities. Social engagement occurs at the intersection between individual and community, reflecting commitment to oneself, others and the world.

The many faces of wellbeing

Wellbeing is a multi-dimensional construct that addresses desirable psychological functioning and experiences (Ryan and Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2001; Huta and Waterman, Reference Huta and Waterman2014). Two main approaches to this concept can be distinguished, with philosophical roots dating back to ancient Greeks and their interrogations about what constitutes a good life.

The hedonic perspective conceives wellbeing as happiness or pleasure attained by pursuing human appetites. The focus is on living pleasant and avoiding unpleasant experiences (Ryan and Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2001; Huta and Waterman, Reference Huta and Waterman2014). Accordingly, this tradition is reflected in the threefold structure of subjective wellbeing: positive affect, negative affect and life satisfaction. These operational definitions were mainly empirically driven rather than theoretically based. Positive and negative affect tap into emotional aspects of human experience. In turn, life satisfaction is a cognitive component, referring to judgements about one's life (Ryff et al., Reference Ryff, Boylan, Kirsch, Lee, Kubzansky and VanderWeele2021). So, in this case, wellbeing is mainly associated with enjoyment and feeling good.

The eudaimonic perspective views wellbeing as more than happiness. Instead, wellbeing corresponds to fulfilling human potential (Ryan and Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2001) and ‘achieving the best that is within us’ (Ryff et al., Reference Ryff, Boylan, Kirsch, Lee, Kubzansky and VanderWeele2021: 94). The focus is on human flourishing and excellence (Huta and Waterman, Reference Huta and Waterman2014). Hence, wellbeing is attained not by following transitory desires but by pursuing what is worth, leading to individual growth. From this point of view, wellbeing stands upon the congruence between what an individual is and what he or she does.

Within the eudaimonic perspective, Carol Ryff (Ryff, Reference Ryff1989a, Reference Ryff1989b; Ryff et al., Reference Ryff, Boylan, Kirsch, Lee, Kubzansky and VanderWeele2021) proposed a model in which psychological or eudaimonic wellbeing has six dimensions: self-acceptance, positive relations with others, autonomy, environmental mastery, purpose in life and personal growth. Ryff's multi-dimensional model acknowledges that change is present in any life period and that gains and losses in wellbeing coexist throughout life, in line with a lifespan perspective of human development (Baltes et al., Reference Baltes, Lindenberger, Staudinger, Lerner and Damon2006).

Both wellbeing approaches have been essential to further knowledge about psychological wellness. Research shows associations between hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing but also supports that these are distinct constructs. Sociodemographic variables tend to correlate differently with each. Also, there is evidence of within-person and between-person differences in wellbeing components (Ryan and Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2001; Ryff et al., Reference Ryff, Boylan, Kirsch, Lee, Kubzansky and VanderWeele2021). Namely while increasing age tends to predict increased hedonic wellbeing, some dimensions of eudaimonia (purpose in life, personal growth) decrease with age even though others (positive relations, autonomy, self-acceptance) remain stable (Ryff et al., Reference Ryff, Boylan, Kirsch, Lee, Kubzansky and VanderWeele2021). So, while competing philosophically, these two views can complement in developmental research.

Social engagement and wellbeing in late life

Several cross-sectional and longitudinal studies observed that participation in social activities is associated with positive psychological functioning in old age, namely with psychological wellbeing (Sharifian and Grühn, Reference Sharifian and Grühn2019), quality of life (Park et al., Reference Park, Chun, Choi, Lee, Kim and Park2015; Hajek et al., Reference Hajek, Brettschneider, Mallon, Ernst, Mamone, Wiese, Weyerer, Werle, Pentzek, Fuchs, Stein, Luck, Bickel, Weeg, Wagner, Heser, Maier, Scherer, Riedel-Heller and König2017; He et al., Reference He, Cui, Liang, Zhong, Li, Li, Lv and Huang2017), self-esteem, satisfaction with life and positive affect (Huxhold et al., Reference Huxhold, Fiori and Windsor2013a, Reference Huxhold, Miche and Schüz2013b; Kang and Ahn, Reference Kang and Ahn2014; Michèle et al., Reference Michèle, Guillaume, Alain, Nathalie, Claude and Kamel2019), as well as absence of negative affect and depressive symptoms (Huxhold et al., Reference Huxhold, Fiori and Windsor2013a, Reference Huxhold, Miche and Schüz2013b; Morrow-Howell et al., Reference Morrow-Howell, Putnam, Lee, Greenfield, Inoue and Chen2014; Hajek et al., Reference Hajek, Brettschneider, Mallon, Ernst, Mamone, Wiese, Weyerer, Werle, Pentzek, Fuchs, Stein, Luck, Bickel, Weeg, Wagner, Heser, Maier, Scherer, Riedel-Heller and König2017; Michèle et al., Reference Michèle, Guillaume, Alain, Nathalie, Claude and Kamel2019). Moreover, social engagement may buffer declines in wellbeing and quality of life associated with advancing age (Nimrod and Shrira, Reference Nimrod and Shrira2016; Sharifian and Grühn, Reference Sharifian and Grühn2019). In a longitudinal approach, Sharifian and Grühn (Reference Sharifian and Grühn2019) found that individuals with high social participation displayed better initial wellbeing, as well as less decline in wellbeing over time.

It has been proposed that social engagement benefits wellbeing by providing access to several psychologically beneficial resources. By participating in a meaningful social context, people live fully, they access companionship and sociability. Consequently, they feel attached to others, feel they belong and are valued, enabling self-acceptance. Also, involvement in social activities helps define and reinforce one's identity and social roles, fostering a sense of coherence, mastery and meaning in life (Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000; Adams et al., Reference Adams, Leibbrandt and Moon2011; Thoits, Reference Thoits2011).

Furthermore, according to Carstensen (Carstensen et al., Reference Carstensen, Isaacowitz and Charles1999; Carstensen and Löckenhoff, Reference Carstensen and Löckenhoff2003), individuals actively prioritise emotionally rewarding experiences when they perceive future time as limited, which tends to occur with advancing age. Older adults seek to maximise emotional gains by investing in meaningful relationships and pro-social activities. Hence, social engagement can be seen as an optimisation mechanism through which people transform their social interactions to enhance the emotional resources derived from them, influencing wellbeing.

Nevertheless, social engagement does not depend only on individual volition. Social behaviour is influenced by personal (e.g. health, education) and contextual (e.g. social support, physical barriers) factors, and also by events (e.g. retirement, widowhood) typically linked to later life (Agahi et al., Reference Agahi, Lennartsson, Kåreholt and Shaw2013; Curl et al., Reference Curl, Stowe, Cooney and Proulx2014; Sabbath et al., Reference Sabbath, Lubben, Goldberg, Zins and Berkman2015). At a macro-level, policies, social norms and cultural values structure social engagement opportunities, with substantial differences between countries (Hank, Reference Hank2011; van Tienoven et al., Reference van Tienoven, Craig, Glorieux and Minnen2020; Lakomý, Reference Lakomý2021). In Europe, participation in social activities is high in social-democratic and liberal welfare regimes (e.g. Denmark, England), but less widespread in conservative regimes (e.g. France), and even lower in the Mediterranean (e.g. Portugal, Spain) and post-communist (e.g. Bulgaria) regimes (Principi et al., Reference Principi, Santini, Socci, Smeaton, Cahill, Vegeris and Barnes2018; Lakomý, Reference Lakomý2021). Older adults in the United States of America (USA) display higher involvement in paid work and volunteering than Belgians, possibly due to specificities of the US pension system and deep-rooted volunteering culture (van Tienoven et al., Reference van Tienoven, Craig, Glorieux and Minnen2020). Macro- and micro-level factors intersect to shape older adults' social engagement.

Overall, theory, empirical findings and policy suggest that social engagement can lead to desirable outcomes in late life. However, this concept gathers limited consensus and clarity. Consequently, it is difficult to communicate about this topic and to develop or select standardised measures (Levasseur et al., Reference Levasseur, Richard, Gauvin and Raymond2010; Adams et al., Reference Adams, Leibbrandt and Moon2011; Douglas et al., Reference Douglas, Georgiou and Westbrook2017). There is also diversity in the definition and measurement of wellbeing. Hence, research comparability becomes problematic, which has implications for policy and intervention.

Furthermore, it is important to uncover moderators and mediators, namely individual and activity characteristics (Adams et al., Reference Adams, Leibbrandt and Moon2011; Douglas et al., Reference Douglas, Georgiou and Westbrook2017; Bruggencate et al., Reference Bruggencate, Luijkx and Sturm2018). Finally, there is a need to integrate findings regarding social engagement benefits and the contraction of social ties and activities in late life (Carstensen et al., Reference Carstensen, Isaacowitz and Charles1999; Pinto and Neri, Reference Pinto and Neri2017). Previous systematic reviews either focus on conceptual matters (Levasseur et al., Reference Levasseur, Richard, Gauvin and Raymond2010; Aroogh and Shahboulaghi, Reference Aroogh and Shahboulaghi2020) or assume a different approach (Douglas et al., Reference Douglas, Georgiou and Westbrook2017; Bruggencate et al., Reference Bruggencate, Luijkx and Sturm2018). In a review, Adams et al. (Reference Adams, Leibbrandt and Moon2011) focus on social activity and wellbeing in late life but also address leisure and survival.

Therefore, the present study proposes adding to these valuable contributions by adopting a narrower scope and including the period after 2011. The main question of this systematic review is:

  • Do older adults who are more socially engaged have better psychological wellbeing than those who are less socially engaged?

The study aims to: (a) explore how social engagement and wellbeing are conceptualised and measured; (b) synthesise evidence regarding the relationship between social engagement and wellbeing in community-dwelling older adults; and (c) examine moderators and mediators of said relationship.

Methods

The present systematic review follows the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA; Page et al., Reference Page, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow, Shamseer, Tetzlaff, Akl, Brennan, Chou, Glanville, Grimshaw, Hróbjartsson, Lalu, Li, Loder, Mayo-Wilson, McDonald, McGuinness, Stewart, Thomas, Tricco, Welch, Whiting and Moher2021). Initially, the researchers developed a study protocol following PRISMA protocols (PRISMA-P; Moher et al., Reference Moher, Shamseer, Clarke, Ghersi, Liberati, Petticrew, Shekelle and Stewart2015), publicly available at PROSPERO (record CRD42021265191). The following sections present the research plan.

Search strategy

The general purpose of the search strategy was to identify original research articles focused on the connections between social engagement and wellbeing in community-dwelling older adults.

As mentioned, social engagement lacks consensus and terms surrounding this concept proliferate (Levasseur et al., Reference Levasseur, Richard, Gauvin and Raymond2010; Adams et al., Reference Adams, Leibbrandt and Moon2011; Douglas et al., Reference Douglas, Georgiou and Westbrook2017). Despite being more consensual, psychological wellbeing is also conceptualised and operationalised in different ways, namely hedonic and eudaimonic approaches (Ryff et al., Reference Ryff, Boylan, Kirsch, Lee, Kubzansky and VanderWeele2021). Therefore, the reviewers used search terms comprehensively to be as inclusive as possible. Several designations were also used for the population of interest. As a result, the search strategy combined the following terms:

  1. (1) Population: aged OR ageing OR aging OR elder OR old OR older OR seniors.

  2. (2) Exposure: social engagement OR social involvement OR social participation OR community engagement OR community involvement OR community participation OR social activities OR social activity OR social connectedness OR group participation.

  3. (3) Outcome: well-being OR wellbeing OR well being OR satisfaction with life OR life satisfaction OR affect OR happiness OR quality of life.

Using these terms, relevant literature published from January 2000 to June 2021 was searched in the following databases: PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, Scopus, Web of Science Core Collection and PubMed. Electronic database searching took place between 9 and 20 July 2021. Efforts were made to conduct a similar search in all databases by adjusting the search expression. In addition, reference lists of eligible articles were scanned to identify other potentially relevant studies. Final search expressions for each database are displayed in File 1 in the online supplementary material.

Study selection

Records identified through electronic databases were exported to EndNote 20 to enable duplicate removal and reference examination. Afterwards, two reviewers independently screened the records for potential inclusion in a two-step sequence: (a) title and abstract and (b) full-text. During this process, the two researchers were blind to one another's decisions. At the end of each step, reviewers discussed disagreements to find consensus. When necessary, a third element intervened. Inter-rater reliability was calculated at every step.

The researchers followed a set of predefined criteria in the study selection process. Preliminary inclusion and exclusion criteria were formulated based on the research questions, as Siddaway et al. (Reference Siddaway, Wood and Hedges2019) recommend. These criteria were refined throughout the review process and reapplied whenever necessary, according to the iterative nature of systematic reviews. Table 1 presents all the inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Table 1. Inclusion and exclusion criteria used in study selection

This review comprises published original articles to guarantee that the included evidence has been subject to peer review. At the same time, preliminary searches indicated a considerable amount of published and current literature. Additionally, grey literature is harder to search systematically. Studies also had to report quantitative research analysing the relationship between social engagement and wellbeing, precluding solely descriptive studies. Qualitative studies were excluded because they did not quantify variables and were not the most suited to address the review question. Only papers with full-text in English, Portuguese or Spanish were retained due to a lack of translation resources.

Regarding population, this review focuses on late adulthood, including studies in which participants are 55 years of age or older, community-dwelling, and do not belong to clinical or specific groups (e.g. conditions or illnesses, such as stroke, cancer or cognitive impairment; disability, such as sensory impairments or movement difficulties; specific life events or demographics, such as widowhood or immigration). In longitudinal studies, we considered participants' age when the exposure was measured.

Concerning the exposure, social engagement was defined as activities done in interaction with others and in person. To be included in the review, studies had to measure one or several of these activities, such as participation in clubs, groups or associations, educational activities, volunteering, visiting family/friends, etc. The exposure was circumscribed to the last three levels of the taxonomy proposed by Levasseur et al. (Reference Levasseur, Richard, Gauvin and Raymond2010). Accordingly, studies that measured activities outside this definition or not clearly within it (e.g. talking on the phone, shopping, driving, reading books, face-to-face communication) were excluded. Furthermore, studies generically measuring social contact frequency or diversity were excluded because, in such cases, it was impossible to know exactly what was assessed. Social contact might have entailed simply talking to a service provider or crossing paths with a stranger. Papers focused on online/digital activities and paid work were also excluded. Whenever any of these activities were measured together with social engagement ones, without the possibility of isolating the latter, papers were excluded as well.

Regarding outcomes, the aim was to include both hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing approaches. Therefore, studies in this review had to measure any of the following indicators: psychological wellbeing, satisfaction with life, happiness or positive affect. Initially, it was established that quality of life would be considered as well since this concept also captures positive individual functioning. However, as the study proceeded, it became clear that the review should be more precise by focusing on wellbeing only. So, after full-text examination, the research team decided to exclude 11 papers in which the only outcome was quality of life. There were no restrictions based on the nature of the measures, but these were considered in the analysis.

Finally, studies explicitly focused on the COVID-19 pandemic were excluded since this was a very particular context, especially regarding social engagement. Similarly, one study was excluded because data collection occurred during the COVID-19 pandemic. Data collection periods were verified in all eligible articles published since 2020.

Data extraction and analysis

Following the previous procedures, one of the authors used a standardised form (see File 2 in the online supplementary material) to extract data from eligible papers, and another author checked the retrieved information. Data extraction focused on: (a) publication details; (b) conceptual and methodological aspects; and (c) main findings. Specifically, for each included paper, the researchers collected information on the title, author(s), authors' scientific affiliations, publication year, journal, journal's country, study country, goals, design, sampling, participants, social engagement concept and measures, wellbeing concept and measures, moderators and mediators, data analysis, results regarding the relationships between social engagement and wellbeing, and results concerning moderators and mediators.

These data were analysed to produce a narrative synthesis comprising a conceptual and methodological description of included studies and the integration of evidence about social engagement and wellbeing associations. A meta-analysis was deemed inadequate since there was considerable heterogeneity among the studies. Results are presented in the text and tables, separating longitudinal and cross-sectional studies. Each table shows sample features, participant's age and gender, measurement strategy for the central variables, main findings and risk of bias assessment.

Risk of bias assessment

Two authors independently analysed the methodological robustness of the included studies. In line with Boland et al. (Reference Boland, Cherry and Dickson2017), this was done after data extraction to minimise bias. After working independently, the two reviewers compared assessments to resolve disagreements through discussion or with the aid of a third reviewer.

The research team examined several risk-of-bias assessment tools to select the most appropriate one(s). First, the idea was to use a combination of different tools, one for each research design. However, none seemed to fit the body of studies under analysis adequately. Therefore, study quality was assessed using a single instrument – the Quality Assessment Tool for Reviewing Studies with Diverse Designs (QATSDD; Sirriyeh et al., Reference Sirriyeh, Lawton, Gardner and Armitage2012).

The QATSDD has 14 items that apply to quantitative studies, of which one was not used in this review (user involvement in design). The 13 items used cover theoretical framework, goals, research setting, sample size and representativeness, recruitment, data collection tools and procedures, data analysis, strengths and limitations. With the aid of scoring guidance notes, each item is rated on a four-point scale (0–3), producing scores that range from 0 (higher risk of bias) to 39 (lower risk of bias). Even though this type of scale is usually less encouraged (Boland et al., Reference Boland, Cherry and Dickson2017; Siddaway et al., Reference Siddaway, Wood and Hedges2019), Sirriyeh et al. (Reference Sirriyeh, Lawton, Gardner and Armitage2012) state that a scaled response can enhance accuracy in risk of bias assessment. Furthermore, even when using a scale, one can consider its different indicators separately, similar to what Siddaway et al. (Reference Siddaway, Wood and Hedges2019) suggest.

Results

Study selection and description

The PRISMA flowchart (Figure 1) shows that the electronic database search rendered 8,390 records. After duplicate removal, two reviewers independently screened the titles and abstracts of 4,325 references. Subsequently, 276 reports went through full-text analysis, resulting in the retention of 35 articles. The backward citation strategy yielded 123 additional references and, following examination of eligibility criteria, another seven were included. Thus, a final set of 42 papers published between January 2000 and June 2021 were included in this systematic review. Regarding inter-rater reliability, the agreement rate was 92.3 per cent in the first step and 90.8 per cent in the full-text analysis.

Tables 2 and 3 display the characteristics and main findings of included studies for longitudinal and cross-sectional designs, respectively. Overall, scientific production tends to concentrate in more recent years, with 54.8 per cent (N = 23) of the articles published between 2016 and 2021. Only seven papers (16.7%) were published between 2000 and 2010. There are 11 different publication countries (see File 3 in the online supplementary material), among which the USA (N = 17; 40.5%) and the United Kingdom (N = 11; 26.2%) stand out. There is, however, a higher dispersion regarding the countries where studies were conducted – 15 nations with similar frequencies. Studies conducted in Australasian countries predominate (N = 24; 57.1%), followed by the American continent (N = 10; 23.8%) and finally by Europe (N = 8; 19.1%). Based on authors' institutional affiliations, papers were categorised into broad scientific domains (see File 3 in the online supplementary material), revealing a predominance of Social and Behavioural Sciences (N = 24; 60.0%), followed by Health Sciences (N = 17; 42.5%) and Social Work (N = 9; 22.5%).Footnote 1

Table 2. Characteristics and main findings of longitudinal studies included in the review

Notes: Only wellbeing outcomes considered for review purposes are reported. USA: United States of America. UK: United Kingdom. NI: no information available. LSI-A: Life Satisfaction Index-A (Neugarten et al., Reference Neugarten, Havighurst and Tobin1961). PANAS: Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson et al., Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988). PWBS: Psychological Well-Being Scales (Ryff, Reference Ryff1989b). SWLS: Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al., Reference Diener, Emmons, Larsen and Griffin1985). Other: other instruments. ↑: Higher. SE: social engagement variable. WB: wellbeing variable. Vol.: volunteering. RoB: risk of bias assessment result (possible range: 0–39 points). 1. Data correspond to the study's last wave since it was the only one considered in analysis due to participants' age at exposure time. The study follows a cohort mostly born in 1938/1939.

Table 3. Characteristics and main findings of cross-sectional studies included in the review

Notes: Only wellbeing outcomes considered for review purposes are reported. USA: United States of America. UK: United Kingdom. NI: no information available. PANAS: Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson et al., Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988). PWBS: Psychological Well-Being Scales (Ryff, Reference Ryff1989b). SWLS: Satisfaction with life scale (Diener et al., Reference Diener, Emmons, Larsen and Griffin1985). Other: Other instruments. ↑: Higher. SE: social engagement variable. WB: wellbeing variable. Vol.: volunteering. NA: no association. RoB: risk of bias assessment result (possible range: 0–39 points). 1. The study focuses on two different samples, both probabilistic.

In what refers to methods, the majority (N = 28; 66.7%) of the studies are cross-sectional and 33.3 per cent (N = 14) are longitudinal. Except for one experimental study (Pettigrew et al., Reference Pettigrew, Jongenelis, Jackson, Warburton and Newton2020), all follow observational designs. Globally, 87.8 per cent (N = 36; one missing value) use probability samples, with a minimum of 101 participants and a maximum of 31,428 (mean = 4,430.1; standard deviation (SD) = 5,647.72). Females are predominant in 75.0 per cent (N = 30; two missing values) of the cases, even though samples tend to be fairly balanced in gender. In most studies (N = 32; 76.2%), the established age for older participants is 60 or 65 years.

Regarding the risk of bias, QATSDD scores vary from 20 to 33 in a range of 0–39. All included studies are above 50 per cent of the possible maximum score. Globally, 16 (38.1%) papers have a score between 20 and 24; 17 (40.5%) have a score between 25 and 29; and 9 (21.4%) have a score of 30 or more. Overall, studies are less robust in assessing the psychometric qualities of measurement tools, explaining the choice of sample size and describing data collection procedures. Conversely, the risk of bias is lower regarding the presentation of study goals, use of representative samples, and fit between the research question and analysis.

Conceptualisation and measurement

As expected, there is considerable diversity in how the exposure variable is named, even within the same paper. Taking into account authors' preferred designations, social participation is the most frequent (N = 11; 26.2%) and social engagement appears next (N = 5; 11.9%), closely followed by social activity/activities (N = 4; 9.5%) and, then, community involvement and activity/activity participation (N = 3; 7.1%). However, there are 12 different terms, besides nine articles (21.4%) specifically focused on volunteering (for additional information on conceptualisation and measurement, see File 3 in the online supplementary material).

Among the 42 articles, 11 (26.2%) do not have any conceptual definition or framework regarding social engagement, and an equal number provide only the latter. The remaining 20 (47.6%) offer explicit definitions. Authors place social engagement/participation at the intersection between activities and interactions. It is described as performing activities embedded in social relationships, and a few authors assert its different forms (e.g. formal or informal, self or other-oriented).

Regarding assessment strategies, there are no standardised instruments. As shown in Tables 2 and 3, most studies operationalise this variable as participation in several different activities or organisations (N = 29; 69.0%), but eight (19.1%) studies assess involvement in a single activity and five (11.9%) use different or mixed strategies. There is also heterogeneity in the number of activities assessed (minimum = 1; maximum = 19; mean = 5.18; SD = 4.0) and in indicators' nature. Some researchers use specific activities, others use activity types and others use groups/organisations.

Within the wellbeing outcomes covered in this review, hedonic indicators prevail. Satisfaction with life is the most frequent indicator (N = 29; 69.1%), with positive affect present in six (14.3%) papers and happiness present in four (9.5%). However, eudaimonic wellbeing is used in seven (16.3%) studies.Footnote 2

Notably, 19 articles (45.2%) do not include any definition or conceptual discussion of the dependent variable. There is some conceptual or theoretical discussion in 12 (28.6%) articles. Finally, 11 (26.2%) articles explicitly define wellbeing. Overall, existing conceptualisations highlight the following aspects: (a) positive orientation towards life and positive psychological functioning; (b) individual nature; and (c) multi-dimensionality. As to multi-dimensionality, on the one hand, authors distinguish experienced and cognitive components of subjective wellbeing; on the other, they distinguish hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing.

To measure wellbeing, researchers mostly use standardised instruments (N = 23; 54.8%). Nevertheless, there are also non-standardised measures composed of several items (N = 11; 26.2%) and an equal number of cases that use only one item. In line with the conceptual tendencies described above, the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al., Reference Diener, Emmons, Larsen and Griffin1985) is predominant (N = 12; 28.6%) among standardised instruments. Ryff's Psychological Wellbeing Scales (Ryff, Reference Ryff1989b) are used in five cases (11.9%).

Relationships between social engagement and wellbeing in late life

Overall, despite differences in measurement, research supports that those with higher participation in social activities display improved wellbeing (Tables 2 and 3). There is ample evidence for satisfaction with life, but also for positive affect, happiness and eudaimonia. These relationships are observed throughout diverse research strategies: in cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, when activities are analysed individually or as a whole, and in comparisons with continuous or categorical social engagement variables.

After adjustment for sociodemographic and health variables, these associations remain significant in some studies while, in others, social activities lose their predictive power. Nevertheless, in all longitudinal studies, the relationships in question persist after introducing covariates. The exception is the paper by Hoshino et al. (Reference Hoshino, Ishikawa, Tanaka, Usui, Komata, Shizawa and Katsura2020), in which it is unclear whether these were controlled for. Furthermore, some longitudinal studies also control for baseline wellbeing (Piliavin and Siegl, Reference Piliavin and Siegl2007; Matthews and Nazroo, Reference Matthews and Nazroo2021) and reversed causality (Van Willigen, Reference Van Willigen2000; Fancourt and Steptoe, Reference Fancourt and Steptoe2018), offering further proof that social engagement is a significant wellbeing predictor.

When specific activities are investigated, volunteering stands out as a relevant wellbeing correlate. Apart from that, studies almost always find significant associations between some social activity or group and wellbeing, but results vary regarding which. Only two studies did not find any significant associations.

In what refers to moderators, several studies suggest that the relationship between social engagement and wellbeing is stronger or more beneficial in old age, compared with younger life periods (Van Willigen, Reference Van Willigen2000; Chan, Reference Chan2018), but also in older ages within late life (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Feng, Liu and Zhen2015; Fancourt and Steptoe, Reference Fancourt and Steptoe2018; Tosheva, Reference Tosheva2020). Contrarily, Litwin and Stoeckel (Reference Litwin and Stoeckel2013) observed significant associations in the 60–79 age group but not in the 80+. Gender interactions also appear in the literature. Globally, studies find more significant associations in women than men (Gautam et al., Reference Gautam, Saito and Kai2007; Windsor et al., Reference Windsor, Anstey and Rodgers2008; McMunn et al., Reference McMunn, Nazroo, Wahrendorf, Breeze and Zaninotto2009; Lee and Lee, Reference Lee and Lee2013; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Feng, Liu and Zhen2015).

Furthermore, there are studies that show no significant associations between the central variables in participants with higher educational attainment (Lee and Lee, Reference Lee and Lee2013; Tosheva, Reference Tosheva2020) and no financial difficulties (Tosheva, Reference Tosheva2020). Besides that, the beneficial effect of volunteering is more significant in those who lost more friends (Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Hosking, Burns and Anstey2019) and with lower social integration (Piliavin and Siegl, Reference Piliavin and Siegl2007). Finally, reciprocity, i.e. feeling appreciated for the activities one performs, also determines the effects of such actions on wellbeing (McMunn et al., Reference McMunn, Nazroo, Wahrendorf, Breeze and Zaninotto2009; Matthews and Nazroo, Reference Matthews and Nazroo2021).

Additionally, there is evidence of a cumulative effect whereby the greater the social engagement, the higher the wellbeing (Piliavin and Siegl, Reference Piliavin and Siegl2007; McMunn et al., Reference McMunn, Nazroo, Wahrendorf, Breeze and Zaninotto2009; Gilmour, Reference Gilmour2012; Toohey et al., Reference Toohey, Hewson, Adams and Rock2018; Lu et al., Reference Lu, Matsuyama, Tanji, Otsuka, Tomata and Tsuji2021; Matthews and Nazroo, Reference Matthews and Nazroo2021). In contrast, some studies (Van Willigen, Reference Van Willigen2000; Windsor et al., Reference Windsor, Anstey and Rodgers2008; Pilkington et al., Reference Pilkington, Windsor and Crisp2012; Vozikaki et al., Reference Vozikaki, Linardakis, Micheli and Philalithis2017) suggest the existence of non-linear trends in which the association between social engagement and wellbeing declines, disappears or even becomes negative after a certain frequency/intensity. It should be noted that three of these studies focus on volunteering, and the fourth examines productive activities (volunteering, caring and providing help).

Lastly, findings are scarcer regarding the mechanisms that might help explain the associations between social engagement and wellbeing. Three studies examine social support, with mixed conclusions (Gilmour, Reference Gilmour2012; Pilkington et al., Reference Pilkington, Windsor and Crisp2012; Lee and Lee, Reference Lee and Lee2013). Others suggest that positive social exchanges (Pilkington et al., Reference Pilkington, Windsor and Crisp2012), a sense of mattering (Piliavin and Siegl, Reference Piliavin and Siegl2007) and cognitive functioning (Bae and Kim, Reference Bae and Kim2021) might be significant mediators.

Discussion

This study aims to summarise current knowledge about the role of social engagement in ageing well. The authors examined 42 studies, addressing conceptualisation, measurement and main findings.

Firstly, results show much diversity in how social activity is named and measured. Concurrently, some researchers assess the same activities even while naming the independent variable differently. Several authors (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Feng, Liu and Zhen2015; Vozikaki et al., Reference Vozikaki, Linardakis, Micheli and Philalithis2017; Toohey et al., Reference Toohey, Hewson, Adams and Rock2018; Tosheva, Reference Tosheva2020; Lai et al., Reference Lai, Zhou and Yuan2021) mention obstacles in defining and measuring social engagement/participation, following previous literature reviews (Levasseur et al., Reference Levasseur, Richard, Gauvin and Raymond2010; Adams et al., Reference Adams, Leibbrandt and Moon2011; Douglas et al., Reference Douglas, Georgiou and Westbrook2017). Therefore, this concept seems broad and diffuse despite accumulating scientific evidence and previous conceptual contributions (e.g. Levasseur et al., Reference Levasseur, Richard, Gauvin and Raymond2010).

Less than half the papers provide definitions of the independent variable. Although existing definitions align with conceptual proposals in the domain (Rowe and Kahn, Reference Rowe and Kahn1997; Bukov et al., Reference Bukov, Maas and Lampert2002; Levasseur et al., Reference Levasseur, Richard, Gauvin and Raymond2010; Aroogh and Shahboulaghi, Reference Aroogh and Shahboulaghi2020), in other studies, social engagement is established as a potential wellbeing predictor based on previous empirical data, rather than a theoretical framework. We claim that research about social engagement must stem from a robust theoretical base and adopt clear operational definitions of this variable. In other words, it should be known precisely what is under study and why.

Despite these difficulties, researchers' efforts to refine assessment strategies are evident and have the merit of stressing several features of social engagement that might impact wellbeing. In fact, social engagement should not be taken as a whole or reduced to single indicators since, as Huxhold et al. (Reference Huxhold, Fiori and Windsor2013a: 13) put it, ‘different characteristics of the social network and different facets of wellbeing and health form a complicated developmental dynamic’. Measures of social engagement should aggregate several indicators, such as activity content, intensity, diversity and perceived importance. Adams et al. (Reference Adams, Leibbrandt and Moon2011: 704) state that ‘Optimal measures of social and leisure activity should clearly specify the “active ingredients” that the researcher wishes to examine and identify categories that are mutually exclusive’.

In what refers to wellbeing, hedonic outcomes prevail over eudaimonic ones. Even though standardised instruments appear in more than half of the studies, non-standardised and single-item measures are still relatively frequent. Furthermore, several papers do not define or discuss the concept. This sustains the notion of Vozikaki et al. (Reference Vozikaki, Linardakis, Micheli and Philalithis2017) that the lack of a comprehensive conceptual and methodological wellbeing framework hinders the study of this construct.

In contrast, some studies comprise both hedonic and eudaimonic measures, which signals the growing recognition of both wellbeing conceptions as relevant research avenues. Researchers (Greenfield and Marks, Reference Greenfield and Marks2004; Piliavin and Siegl, Reference Piliavin and Siegl2007; Clark and Lee, Reference Clark and Lee2021) affirm that comparing various wellbeing indicators in the same individuals is necessary to understand how such indicators associate with each other and with distinct activity types. In line with Ryan and Deci (Reference Ryan and Deci2001), hedonic enjoyment can be more associated with the absence of problems and relaxing activities, while eudaimonia is linked to activities that offer challenge, effort and personal growth. As already said, hedonism and eudaimonism can be complementary, and their determinants, including age, might differ (Ryan and Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2001; Huta and Waterman, Reference Huta and Waterman2014; Ryff et al., Reference Ryff, Boylan, Kirsch, Lee, Kubzansky and VanderWeele2021). Hence, to fully understand the impact of social engagement on older adults' wellbeing, it is necessary to analyse aspects of immediate pleasure and human flourishing simultaneously. Furthermore, it is important to consider what each of us experiences as emotionally meaningful and worthy of investment.

Regarding the primary review question, research comparability is limited because included studies primarily rely on self-reports, have very different sample sizes and measures, and lack explicit theoretical stances. Nevertheless, there is evidence of positive associations between the central variables, in which longitudinal studies offer particular insight. Research globally supports that community-dwelling older adults who are more socially involved display improved wellbeing. Simultaneously, this is an oversimplification since it also became apparent that this relationship depends on individual characteristics and features of social engagement itself.

Findings suggest that social engagement may be especially beneficial for older and more vulnerable people. Although this evidence is preliminary, previous research on quality of life also found stronger associations with social engagement in older ages (Park et al., Reference Park, Chun, Choi, Lee, Kim and Park2015; Nimrod and Shrira, Reference Nimrod and Shrira2016). Moreover, other studies show that social activities buffer declines in older adults' wellbeing and quality of life (Nimrod and Shrira, Reference Nimrod and Shrira2016; Sharifian and Grühn, Reference Sharifian and Grühn2019). Since age is associated with wellbeing changes, particularly declines in personal growth and purpose in life (Ryan and Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2001; Ryff et al., Reference Ryff, Boylan, Kirsch, Lee, Kubzansky and VanderWeele2021), social engagement can act as a compensation resource for wellbeing in later life. Perhaps this protective effect extends to socioeconomic and social disadvantages.

In turn, the moderator effect of reciprocity indicates that the gains of social activities can depend on their perceived rewards and meaning, following socioemotional selectivity (Carstensen et al., Reference Carstensen, Isaacowitz and Charles1999; Carstensen and Löckenhoff, Reference Carstensen and Löckenhoff2003). Possibly, despite being socially involved, some individuals are not proactively selecting the activities most suited to their goals. As to the intensity of social engagement, non-linear associations suggest there may be optimal participation levels, with moderate involvement being more beneficial. However, the studies that observed this trend assessed more-demanding activities, suggesting this is something to consider when exploring social engagement thresholds and cumulative effects.

The included studies are quite diverse regarding countries of origin. There is evidence of positive associations between social activities and wellbeing across 15 nations. On the one hand, this speaks to the global relevance of social engagement for ageing well; on the other, this issue must be framed in terms of macro-contexts since both nations' longstanding cultural features and policy interventions can impact individual action (Hank, Reference Hank2011; Principi et al., Reference Principi, Santini, Socci, Smeaton, Cahill, Vegeris and Barnes2018). Interventions must be context-sensitive (Lakomý, Reference Lakomý2021) and ‘[c]ulturally blind “one-size-fits-all” strategies to foster social participation and productive aging are thus unlikely to be successful’ (Hank, Reference Hank2011: 537). So, although the present study does not focus on social engagement determinants, these aspects deserve consideration.

As with any study, this systematic review has limitations. First, some relevant papers might have been missed due to exclusion criteria. Articles published in languages other than English, Portuguese and Spanish before 2000 were excluded due to resource limitations. Studies with a qualitative design may provide relevant evidence but do not suit the review's aims. Grey literature was omitted, which might have impacted results, particularly regarding non-significant findings. However, the reviewers favoured a systematic search and peer-reviewed sources. Furthermore, the term leisure was not used in the literature search, which could have precluded some pertinent articles. Still, leisure can entail individual activities.

As mentioned, future research should use measurement strategies that combine different social engagement aspects and analyse hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing. It is also important to explore age differences, paying attention to the agendas of different life periods and how these interact with social activities. Similarly, it is essential to examine mediators and moderators anchored in a developmental perspective. This includes sociodemographic characteristics that structure individual opportunities and constraints, but also social network variables and social activities' demands, degree of choice, reciprocity and perceived value. Likewise, it is necessary to explore how macro aspects shape social engagement and its associations with wellbeing. The buffering effect of social engagement on the wellbeing of vulnerable groups, potentially working as a compensatory mechanism, also deserves further scrutiny since it can be key in designing effective policies and practices.

Conclusion

To conclude, this paper reviews a considerable number of studies. It explores conceptualisation, measurement and findings to provide a snapshot of available knowledge. Robust evidence supports a relationship between social engagement and wellbeing in community-dwelling older adults. However, there is still a long way to go until we can unravel all the nuances of this relationship. Current literature raises many questions and indicates the need to adopt sound conceptual frameworks, along with comprehensive and clear measures.

In any case, this issue deserves further inquiry since social engagement is a modifiable factor that can be especially valuable for those more vulnerable. Social interventions and policies might target involvement in social activities as an important developmental resource to improve people's lives.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X24000011.

Financial support

This work was supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology and the European Social Fund (grant number 2020.05882.BD), which had no role in the design, execution, analysis and interpretation of data, or writing of the study.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical standards

This manuscript relies on secondary data but is part of a research project that received ethical approval from the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences at University of Porto (reference 2021/04-07).

Footnotes

1 There are two missing values and the sum of percentages exceeds 100 per cent because some papers include more than one scientific domain.

2 The sum of percentages exceeds 100 per cent because some papers include more than one wellbeing outcome. Psychological wellbeing includes the following indicators: psychological wellbeing, eudaimonia, purpose in life and personal growth.

References

Adams, KB, Leibbrandt, S and Moon, H (2011) A critical review of the literature on social and leisure activity and wellbeing in later life. Ageing & Society 31, 683712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Agahi, N, Lennartsson, C, Kåreholt, I and Shaw, BA (2013) Trajectories of social activities from middle age to old age and late-life disability: a 36-year follow-up. Age and Ageing 42, 790793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aroogh, MD and Shahboulaghi, FM (2020) Social participation of older adults: a concept analysis. International Journal of Community Based Nursing and Midwifery 8, 5572.Google Scholar
* Bae, S and Kim, J (2021) The long-term association between social activity, cognitive function and life satisfaction in Korean older adults: analyzing 10 years of data using multivariate latent growth modeling. Japan Journal of Nursing Science 18, e12390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baltes, PB, Lindenberger, U and Staudinger, UM (2006) Life span theory in developmental psychology. In Lerner, RM and Damon, W (eds), Handbook of Child Psychology: Theoretical Models of Human Development. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 569664.Google Scholar
Berkman, LF, Glass, T, Brissette, I and Seeman, TE (2000) From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. Social Science & Medicine 51, 843857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Blace, NP (2012) Functional ability, participation in activities and life satisfaction of the older people. Asian Social Science 8, 7587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boland, A, Cherry, G and Dickson, R (eds) (2017) Doing a Systematic Review: A Student's Guide. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Bruggencate, TT, Luijkx, KG and Sturm, J (2018) Social needs of older people: a systematic literature review. Ageing & Society 38, 17451770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bukov, A, Maas, I and Lampert, T (2002) Social participation in very old age: cross-sectional and longitudinal findings from BASE. Journals of Gerontology: Series B 57, P510P517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carstensen, LL and Löckenhoff, CE (2003) Aging, emotion, and evolution: the bigger picture. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1000, 152179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carstensen, LL, Isaacowitz, DM and Charles, ST (1999) Taking time seriously: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist 54, 165181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Chan, M (2018) Digital communications and psychological well-being across the life span: examining the intervening roles of social capital and civic engagement. Telematics and Informatics 35, 17441754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Clark, AE and Lee, T (2021) Early-life correlates of later-life well-being: evidence from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 181, 360368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curl, AL, Stowe, JD, Cooney, TM and Proulx, CM (2014) Giving up the keys: how driving cessation affects engagement in later life. The Gerontologist 54, 423433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E, Emmons, RA, Larsen, RJ and Griffin, S (1985) The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment 49, 7175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douglas, H, Georgiou, A and Westbrook, J (2017) Social participation as an indicator of successful aging: an overview of concepts and their associations with health. Australian Health Review 41, 455462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Fancourt, D and Steptoe, A (2018) Community group membership and multidimensional subjective well-being in older age. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 72, 376382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Gautam, R, Saito, T and Kai, I (2007) Leisure and religious activity participation and mental health: gender analysis of older adults in Nepal. BMC Public Health 7, 299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Gilmour, H (2012) Social participation and the health and well-being of Canadian seniors. Health Reports 23, 2332.Google ScholarPubMed
* Greenfield, EA and Marks, NF (2004) Formal volunteering as a protective factor for older adults’ psychological well-being. Journals of Gerontology: Series B 59, S258S264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajek, A, Brettschneider, C, Mallon, T, Ernst, A, Mamone, S, Wiese, B, Weyerer, S, Werle, J, Pentzek, M, Fuchs, A, Stein, J, Luck, T, Bickel, H, Weeg, D, Wagner, M, Heser, K, Maier, W, Scherer, M, Riedel-Heller, SG and König, H-H (2017) The impact of social engagement on health-related quality of life and depressive symptoms in old age – evidence from a multicenter prospective cohort study in Germany. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 15, 140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hank, K (2011) Societal determinants of productive aging: a multilevel analysis across 11 European countries. European Sociological Review 27, 526541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, Q, Cui, Y, Liang, L, Zhong, Q, Li, J, Li, Y, Lv, X and Huang, F (2017) Social participation, willingness and quality of life: a population-based study among older adults in rural areas of China. Geriatrics and Gerontology International 17, 15931602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Hoshino, A, Ishikawa, N, Tanaka, M, Usui, K, Komata, M, Shizawa, M and Katsura, T (2020) What lifestyles are risk factors for low well-being of healthy elderlies dwelled in a local city in super-aging Japan? – Kizugawa cohort study. Journal of Rural Medicine 15, 7384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Hsu, H-C, Chang, W-C, Chong, Y-S and An, JS (2016) Happiness and social determinants across age cohorts in Taiwan. Journal of Health Psychology 21, 18281839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huta, V and Waterman, AS (2014) Eudaimonia and its distinction from hedonia: developing a classification and terminology for understanding conceptual and operational definitions. Journal of Happiness Studies 15, 14251456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Huxhold, O, Fiori, KL and Windsor, TD (2013 a) The dynamic interplay of social network characteristics, subjective well-being, and health: the costs and benefits of socio-emotional selectivity. Psychology and Aging 28, 316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Huxhold, O, Miche, M and Schüz, B (2013 b) Benefits of having friends in older ages: differential effects of informal social activities on well-being in middle-aged and older adults. Journals of Gerontology: Series B 69, 366375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Jiang, D, Hosking, D, Burns, R and Anstey, KJ (2019) Volunteering benefits life satisfaction over 4 years: the moderating role of social network size. Australian Journal of Psychology 71, 183192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Joe, W, Perkins, JM and Subramanian, SV (2019) Community involvement, trust, and health-related outcomes among older adults in India: a population-based, multilevel, cross-sectional study. Age and Ageing 48, 8793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahana, E and Kahana, B (1996) Conceptual and empirical advances in understanding aging well through proactive adaptation. In Bengtson, VL (ed.), Adulthood and Aging: Research on Continuities and Discontinuities. New York, NY: Springer, pp. 1840.Google Scholar
Kang, J and Ahn, M (2014) Enhancing older females’ psychological well-being through social shopping, social coping, and informal social activities. Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal 42, 341357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Lai, S, Zhou, Y and Yuan, Y (2021) Associations between community cohesion and subjective wellbeing of the elderly in Guangzhou, China – a cross-sectional study based on the structural equation model. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, 953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakomý, M (2021) Differences in social participation of older adults across European welfare regimes: fourteen years of SHARE data collection. International Sociology 36, 906925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Lee, S and Choi, H (2020) Impact of older adults’ mobility and social participation on life satisfaction in South Korea. Asian Social Work and Policy Review 14, 410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Lee, E-KO and Lee, J (2011) Gender differences in predictors of mental health among older adults in South Korea. International Journal of Aging and Human Development 72, 207223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Lee, E-KO and Lee, J (2013) Education, functional limitations, and life satisfaction among older adults in South Korea. Educational Gerontology 39, 514526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levasseur, M, Richard, L, Gauvin, L and Raymond, É (2010) Inventory and analysis of definitions of social participation found in the aging literature: proposed taxonomy of social activities. Social Science & Medicine 71, 21412149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Li, YP, Chen, YM and Chen, CH (2013) Volunteer transitions and physical and psychological health among older adults in Taiwan. Journals of Gerontology: Series B 68, 9971008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Li, C, Jiang, S, Li, N and Zhang, Q (2018) Influence of social participation on life satisfaction and depression among Chinese elderly: social support as a mediator. Journal of Community Psychology 46, 345355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Litwin, H and Stoeckel, KJ (2013) Social networks and subjective wellbeing among older Europeans: does age make a difference? Ageing & Society 33, 12631281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Lu, Y, Matsuyama, S, Tanji, F, Otsuka, T, Tomata, Y and Tsuji, I (2021) Social participation and healthy aging among the older Japanese: the Ohsaki Cohort 2006 Study. Journals of Gerontology: Series A 77, 106113.Google Scholar
* Mair, CA and Thivierge-Rikard, RV (2010) The strength of strong ties for older rural adults: regional distinctions in the relationship between social interaction and subjective well-being. International Journal of Aging and Human Development 70, 119143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Matthews, K and Nazroo, J (2021) The impact of volunteering and its characteristics on well-being after state pension age: longitudinal evidence from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Journals of Gerontology: Series B 76, 632641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* McMunn, A, Nazroo, J, Wahrendorf, M, Breeze, E and Zaninotto, P (2009) Participation in socially-productive activities, reciprocity and wellbeing in later life: baseline results in England. Ageing & Society 29, 765782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michèle, J, Guillaume, M, Alain, T, Nathalie, B, Claude, F and Kamel, G (2019) Social and leisure activity profiles and well-being among the older adults: a longitudinal study. Aging & Mental Health 23, 7783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D, Shamseer, L, Clarke, M, Ghersi, D, Liberati, A, Petticrew, M, Shekelle, P and Stewart, LA (2015) Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Systematic Reviews 4, 1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrow-Howell, N, Putnam, M, Lee, YS, Greenfield, JC, Inoue, M and Chen, H (2014) An investigation of activity profiles of older adults. Journals of Gerontology: Series B 69, 809821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neugarten, BL, Havighurst, RJ and Tobin, SS (1961) The measurement of life satisfaction. Journal of Gerontology 16, 134143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nimrod, G and Shrira, A (2016) The paradox of leisure in later life. Journals of Gerontology: Series B 71, 106111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Okabayashi, H and Hougham, GW (2014) Gender differences of social interactions and their effects on subjective well-being among Japanese elders. Aging & Mental Health 18, 5971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page, MJ, McKenzie, JE, Bossuyt, PM, Boutron, I, Hoffmann, TC, Mulrow, CD, Shamseer, L, Tetzlaff, JM, Akl, EA, Brennan, SE, Chou, R, Glanville, J, Grimshaw, JM, Hróbjartsson, A, Lalu, MM, Li, T, Loder, EW, Mayo-Wilson, E, McDonald, S, McGuinness, LA, Stewart, LA, Thomas, J, Tricco, AC, Welch, VA, Whiting, P and Moher, D (2021) The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372, n71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Palma-Candia, O, Hueso-Montoro, C, Ortega-Valdivieso, A, Montoya-Juárez, R and Cruz-Quintana, F (2016) Factores sociodemográficos y de participación social relacionados con el bienestar psicológico en adultos mayores en la región de Magallanes, Chile. Revista Médica de Chile 144, 12871296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, HK, Chun, SY, Choi, Y, Lee, SY, Kim, SJ and Park, E-C (2015) Effects of social activity on health-related quality of life according to age and gender: an observational study. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 13, 140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Pettigrew, S, Jongenelis, MI, Jackson, B, Warburton, J and Newton, RU (2020) A randomized controlled trial and pragmatic analysis of the effects of volunteering on the health and well-being of older people. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research 32, 711721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Piliavin, JA and Siegl, E (2007) Health benefits of volunteering in the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 48, 450464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Pilkington, PD, Windsor, TD and Crisp, DA (2012) Volunteering and subjective well-being in midlife and older adults: the role of supportive social networks. Journals of Gerontology: Series B 67, 249260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinto, JM and Neri, AL (2017) Trajectories of social participation in old age: a systematic literature review. Revista Brasileira de Geriatria e Gerontologia 20, 260273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Ponce, MSH, Rosas, RPE and Lorca, MBF (2014) Social capital, social participation and life satisfaction among Chilean older adults. Revista de Saúde Pública 48, 739749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Principi, A, Santini, S, Socci, M, Smeaton, D, Cahill, KE, Vegeris, S and Barnes, E (2018) Retirement plans and active ageing: perspectives in three countries. Ageing & Society 38, 5682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, JW and Kahn, RL (1997) Successful aging. The Gerontologist 37, 433440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, RM and Deci, EL (2001) On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology 52, 141166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryff, CD (1989 a) Beyond Ponce de Leon and life satisfaction: new directions in quest of successful ageing. International Journal of Behavioral Development 12, 3555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryff, CD (1989 b) Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57, 10691081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryff, CD, Boylan, JM and Kirsch, JA (2021) Eudaimonic and hedonic well-being: an integrative perspective with linkages to sociodemographic factors and health. In Lee, MT, Kubzansky, LD and VanderWeele, TJ (eds), Measuring Well-being: Interdisciplinary Perspectives from the Social Sciences and the Humanities. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 92135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Ryu, J and Heo, J (2018) Relationships between leisure activity types and well-being in older adults. Leisure Studies 37, 331342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sabbath, EL, Lubben, J, Goldberg, M, Zins, M and Berkman, LF (2015) Social engagement across the retirement transition among ‘young-old’ adults in the French GAZEL cohort. European Journal of Ageing 12, 311320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scharlach, AE and Lehning, AJ (2016) Creating Aging-friendly Communities. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sharifian, N and Grühn, D (2019) The differential impact of social participation and social support on psychological well-being: evidence from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study. International Journal of Aging and Human Development 88, 107126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siddaway, AP, Wood, AM and Hedges, LV (2019) How to do a systematic review: a best practice guide for conducting and reporting narrative reviews, meta-analyses, and meta-syntheses. Annual Review of Psychology 70, 747770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sirriyeh, R, Lawton, R, Gardner, P and Armitage, G (2012) Reviewing studies with diverse designs: the development and evaluation of a new tool. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice 18, 746752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Srivastava, S, Chauhan, S, Muhammad, T, Simon, DJ, Kumar, P, Patel, R and Singh, SK (2021 a) Older adults’ psychological and subjective well-being as a function of household decision making role: evidence from cross-sectional survey in India. Clinical Epidemiology and Global Health 10, 100676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Srivastava, S, Singh, SK, Kumar, M and Muhammad, T (2021 b) Distinguishing between household headship with and without power and its association with subjective well-being among older adults: an analytical cross-sectional study in India. BMC Geriatrics 21, 304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Tang, F, Jang, H, Rauktis, MB, Musa, D and Beach, S (2019) The race paradox in subjective wellbeing among older Americans. Ageing & Society 39, 568589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thoits, PA (2011) Mechanisms linking social ties and support to physical and mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 52, 145161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Toohey, AM, Hewson, JA, Adams, CL and Rock, MJ (2018) Pets, social participation, and aging-in-place: findings from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Canadian Journal on Aging/La Revue canadienne du vieillissement 37, 200217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Tosheva, E (2020) Participation in activities – determinants and reflection on quality of life and satisfaction in old age population in Bulgaria. Revista Inclusiones 7, 353367.Google Scholar
United Nations (1991) United Nations Principles for Older Persons (United Nations General Assembly Resolution 46/91). Available at https://www.ohchr.org/en/professionalinterest/pages/olderpersons.aspx.Google Scholar
van Tienoven, TP, Craig, L, Glorieux, I and Minnen, J (2020) Active participation and well-being among the elderly in Belgium and the USA: a cross-national time-use perspective. Social Indicators Research 160, 625644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
* Van Willigen, M (2000) Differential benefits of volunteering across the life course. Journals of Gerontology: Series B 55, S308S318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Vozikaki, M, Linardakis, M, Micheli, K and Philalithis, A (2017) Activity participation and well-being among European adults aged 65 years and older. Social Indicators Research 131, 769795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D, Clark, LA and Tellegen, A (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54, 10631070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Windsor, TD, Anstey, KJ and Rodgers, B (2008) Volunteering and psychological well-being among young-old adults: how much is too much? The Gerontologist 48, 5970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Windsor, TD, Curtis, RG and Luszcz, MA (2016) Social engagement in late life. In Kendig, H, McDonald, P and Piggot, J (eds), Population Ageing and Australia's Future. Canberra: ANU Press, pp. 185204.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2002) Active Ageing: A Policy Framework. Geneva: World Health Organization. Available at https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/67215.Google Scholar
* Yang, F and Pang, JS (2018) Socioeconomic status, frailty, and subjective well-being: a moderated mediation analysis in elderly Chinese. Journal of Health Psychology 23, 961970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
* Zhang, W, Feng, Q, Liu, L and Zhen, Z (2015) Social engagement and health: findings from the 2013 Survey of the Shanghai Elderly Life and Opinion. International Journal of Aging and Human Development 80, 332356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Inclusion and exclusion criteria used in study selection

Figure 1

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram reporting the study selection process (adapted from Page et al., 2021).Notes: SE: social engagement. WB: wellbeing.

Figure 2

Table 2. Characteristics and main findings of longitudinal studies included in the review

Figure 3

Table 3. Characteristics and main findings of cross-sectional studies included in the review

Supplementary material: File

Monteiro et al. supplementary material 1

Monteiro et al. supplementary material
Download Monteiro et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 102.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Monteiro et al. supplementary material 2

Monteiro et al. supplementary material
Download Monteiro et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 127 KB
Supplementary material: File

Monteiro et al. supplementary material 3

Monteiro et al. supplementary material
Download Monteiro et al. supplementary material 3(File)
File 177.1 KB