Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T09:06:46.178Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Working memory subsystems are impaired in chronic drug dependents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 February 2013

Abdrabo Moghazy Soliman*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Tanta University, Egypt Department of Psychology, Umm Al-Qura University, Makkah, Saudi Arabia
Hesham Fathy Gadelrab
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Psychology, Mansoura University, Egypt Department of Psychology, Umm Al-Qura University, Makkah, Saudi Arabia
Rania Mohamed Elfar
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Kafr Elshiekh University, Egypt Department of Psychology, Umm Al-Qura University, Makkah, Saudi Arabia
*
Dr. Abdrabo Moghazy Soliman, Department of Psychology, Umm Al‐qura University, Makkah, Saudi Arabia. Tel/Fax: +9665281719; E‐mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Background

A large body of research that has investigated substance dependence and working memory (WM) resources, yet no prior study has used a comprehensive test battery to examine the impact of chronic drug dependence on WM as a multi‐component system.

Objectives

This study examined the efficiency of several WM components in participants who were chronic drug dependents. In addition, the functioning of the four WM components was compared among dependents of various types of drugs.

Method

In total, 128 chronic drug dependents participated in this study. Their average age was 38.48 years, and they were classified into four drug‐dependence groups. Chronic drug dependents were compared with a 36‐participant control group that had a mean age of 37.6 years. A WM test battery that comprised eight tests and that assessed each of four WM components was administered to each participant.

Results

Compared with the control group, all four groups of drug dependents had significantly poorer test performance on all of the WM tasks. Among the four groups of drug users, the polydrug group had the poorest performance scores on each of the eight tasks, and the performance scores of the marijuana group were the least affected. Finally, the forward digit span task and the logical memory tasks were less sensitive than other tasks when differentiating between marijuana users and the normal participants.

Conclusion

The four components of WM are impaired among chronic drug dependents. These results have implications for the development of tools, classification methods and therapeutic strategies for drug dependents.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1Faw, B. Pre‐frontal executive committee for perception, working memory, attention, long‐term memory, motor control and thinking: a tutorial review. Conscious Cogn 2003;12:83139.Google Scholar
2Wells, BM, Skowronski, JJ, Crawford, MT, Scherer, CR, Carlston, DE. Inference making and linking both require thinking: spontaneous trait inference and spontaneous trait transference both rely on working memory capacity. J Exp Soc Psychol 2011;47:11161126.Google Scholar
3Emery, L, Hale, S, Myerson, J. Age differences in proactive interference, working memory and abstract reasoning. Psychol Aging 2008;23:634645.Google Scholar
4Krumm, S, Schmidt‐Atzert, L, Buehner, M, Ziegler, M, Michalczyk, K, Arrow, K. Storage and non‐storage components of working memory predicting reasoning: a simultaneous examination of a wide range of ability factors. Intelligence 2009;37:347364.Google Scholar
5Krumm, S, Ziegler, M, Buehner, M. Reasoning and working memory as predictors of school grades. Learn Individ Differ 2008;18:248257.Google Scholar
6Hinson, JM, Jameson, TL, Whitney, P. Impulsive decision making and working memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2003;29:298306.Google Scholar
7Hinson, JM, Whitney, P. Working memory load and decision making: a reply to Franco‐Watkins, Pashler and Rickard (2006). J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2006;32:448450.Google Scholar
8Altgassen, M, Phillips, L, Kopp, U, Kliegel, M. Role of working memory components in planning performance of individuals with Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 2007;45:23932397.Google Scholar
9Joyce, EM, Leeson, VC, Sharma, P, Harrison, M, Ron, MA, Barnes, TR. Working memory not processing speed is the basis of higher‐order planning deficits in first episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2010;117:175175.Google Scholar
10Macoveanu, J, Klingberg, T, Tegnér, J. A biophysical model of multiple‐item working memory: a computational and neuroimaging study. Neuroscience 2006;14:16111618.Google Scholar
11Monchi, O, Taylor, JG, Dagher, A. A neural model of working memory processes in normal subjects, Parkinson's disease and schizophrenia for fMRI design and predictions. Neural Netw 2000;13:953973.Google Scholar
12Morey, RD. A Bayesian hierarchical model for the measurement of working memory capacity. J Math Psychol 2011;55:824.Google Scholar
13Baddeley, AD, Allen, RJ, Hitch, GJ. Binding in visual working memory: the role of the episodic buffer. Neuropsychologia 2011;49:13931400.Google Scholar
14Baddeley, AD. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci 2000;4:1723.Google Scholar
15Alloway, TP. Working memory, reading and mathematical skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. J Exp Child Psychol 2007;96:2036.Google Scholar
16Holmes, J, Gathercole, SE, Place, M, Alloway, TP, Elliott, JG, Hilton, KA. The diagnostic utility of executive function assessments in the identification of ADHD in Children. Child Adolesc Ment Health 2010;15:3743.Google Scholar
17Engel de Abreu, PMJ, Conway, ARA, Gathercole, SE. Working memory and fluid intelligence in young children. Intelligence 2010;38:552561.Google Scholar
18Alloway, TP, Gathercole, SE, Kirkwood, H, Elliott, J. The working memory rating scale: a classroom‐based behavioral assessment of working memory. Learn Individ Differ 2009;19:242245.Google Scholar
19Gathercole, SE. Working memory in the classroom. Psychologist 2008;21:382385.Google Scholar
20Dehn, MJ. Working memory and academic learning: assessment and intervention. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, 2008.Google Scholar
21Baddeley, A. Working memory and language: an overview. J Commun Disord 2003;36:189208.Google Scholar
22Block, RI, Erwin, WJ, Ghoneim, MM. Chronic use and cognitive impairments. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2002;73:491504.Google Scholar
23Kanayama, G, Rogowska, J, Pope, HG, Gruber, SA, Yurgelun‐Todd, DA. Spatial working memory in heavy cannabis users: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;176:239247.Google Scholar
24Kübler, A, Murphy, K, Garavan, H. Cocaine dependence and attention switching within and between verbal and visuospatial working memory. Eur J Neurosci 2005;21:19841992.Google Scholar
25Madoz‐Gúrpide, A, Blasco‐Fontecilla, H, Baca‐García, E, Ochoa‐Mangado, E. Executive dysfunction in chronic cocaine users: an exploratory study. Drug Alcohol Depend 2011;117:5558.Google Scholar
26Vaz, LJ, Pradella‐Hallinan, M, Bueno, OFA, Pompéia, S. Acute glucocorticoid effects on the multicomponent model of working memory. Hum Psychopharmacol 2011;26:477487.Google Scholar
27Verdejo‐García, A, Pérez‐García, M. Profile of executive deficits in cocaine and heroin polysubstance users: common and differential effects on separate executive components. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;190:517530.Google Scholar
28Zhao‐Xin, W, Zhuang‐Wei, X, Da‐Ren, Z, Chun‐Yu, L, Zhang, JX. Verbal working memory deficits in abstinent heroin abusers. Acta Neuropsychiatr 2008;20:265268.Google Scholar
29Gooding, PA, Isaac, CL, Mayes, AR. Prose recall and amnesia: more implications for the episodic buffer. Neuropsychologia 2005;43:583587.Google Scholar
30Gibbs, SE, Cools, R, Miyakawa, A, D'Esposito, M. Working memory capacity predicts contrasting effects of Dopaminergic drugs on prefrontal cortex and Caudate nucleus during memory encoding. Neuroimage 2009;47:S144.Google Scholar
31Grenard, JL, Ames, SL, Wiers, RW, Thush, C, Sussman, S, Stacy, AW. Working memory capacity moderates the predictive effects of drug‐related associations on substance use. Psychol Addict Behav 2008;22:426432.Google Scholar
32Verdejo‐Garcia, AJ, Lopez‐Torrecillas, F, Aguilarde, AF, Perez‐Garcia, M. Differential effects of MDMA, cocaine and cannabis use severity on distinctive components of the executive functions in polysubstance users: a multiple regression analysis. Addict Behav 2005;30:89101.Google Scholar
33Yurgelun‐Todd, DA, Gruber, SA, Hanson, RA, Baird, AA, Renshaw, P, Pope, HG. Residual effects of marijuana use: an fMRI study. NIDA Res Monogr 1999;179:78.Google Scholar
34Kanayama, G, Rogowska, J, Pope, HG, Gruber, SA, Yurgelynn‐Todd, DA. Spatial working memory in heavy cannabis users: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;176:239247.Google Scholar
35Rippeth, J, Heaton, RK, Carey, CL, Marcotte, TD, Moore, DJ, Gonzalez, R. Methamphetam independence increases risk of neuropsychological impairmentin HIV infected persons. J Neuropsychol Sci 2004;10:11.Google Scholar
36Hoff, AL, Riordan, H, Morris, L, Cestaro, V, Wieneke, M, Alpert, R. Effects of crack cocaine on neurocognitive function. Psychiatry Res 1996;60:167176.Google Scholar
37Basso, MR, Bornstein, RA. Effects of past Noninjection drug abuse upon executive function and working memory in HIV Infection. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2003;25:893903.Google Scholar
38Fernández‐Serrano, MJ, Pérez‐García, M, Verdejo‐García, A. What are the specific vs. generalized effects of drugs of abuse on neuropsychological performance? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011;35:377406.Google Scholar
39Hanson, KL, Luciana, M, Sullwold, K. Reward‐related decision‐making deficits and elevated impulsivity among MDMA and other drug users. Drug Alcohol Depend 2008;96:99110.Google Scholar
40Hester, R, Garavan, H. Neural mechanisms underlying drug‐related cue distraction in active cocaine users. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2009;93:270277.Google Scholar
41American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1994.Google Scholar
42Wechsler, D. A standardized memory scale for clinic use. J Psychol 1945;19:8795.Google Scholar
43Hall, ML, Bavelier, D. Short‐term memory stages in sign vs. speech: the source of the serial span discrepancy. Cognition 2011;120:5466.Google Scholar
44Chan, ME, Elliott, JM. Cross‐linguistic differences in digit memory span. Aust Psychol 2011;46:2530.Google Scholar
45Alloway, TP, Gathercole, SE. The role of sentence recall in reading and language skills of children with learning difficulties. Learn Individ Differ 2005;15:271282.Google Scholar
46Alloway, TP, Gathercole, SE, Willis, C, Adams, A‐M. A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. J Exp Child Psychol 2004;87:85106.Google Scholar
47Leffard, SA, Miller, JA, Bernstein, J, DeMann, JJ, Mangis, HA, McCoy, ELB. Substantive validity of working memory measures in major cognitive functioning test batteries for children. Appl Neuropsychol 2007;13:230241.Google Scholar
48Miyake, A, Friedman, NP, Rcttinger, DA, Shah, P, Hegarty, M. How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning and spatial abilities related? A latent‐variable analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen 2001;130:621640.Google Scholar
49Baddeley, AD, Emslie, HV, Nimmo‐Smith, L. The Spot‐the‐Word test: a robust estimate of verbal intelligence based on lexical decision. Br J Health Psychol 1993;1:5560.Google Scholar
50Soliman, AM. Exploring the central executive in situation awareness. Psychol Rep 2010;106:105118.Google Scholar
51Pickering, SJ. The development of visuo‐spatial working memory. Memory 2001;9:423432.Google Scholar
52Baddeley, AD. Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press, 1986.Google Scholar
53Gathercole, SE. Cognitive approaches to the development of short‐term memory. Trends Cogn Sci 1999;3:410419.Google Scholar
54Lecerf, T, Roulin, J‐L. Individual Differences in visuospatial working memory capacity and distractor inhibition. Swiss J Psychol 2009;68:6778.Google Scholar
55Alloway, TP, Gathercole, SE, Pickering, SJ. Verbal and visuospatial short‐term and working memory in children: are they separable? Child Dev 2006;77:16981716.Google Scholar
56Henry, LA. The episodic buffer in children with intellectual disabilities: an exploratory study. Res Dev Behav 2010;31:16091614.Google Scholar
57RepovŠ, G, Baddeley, A. The multi‐component model of working memory: explorations in experimental cognitive psychology. Neuroscience 2006;139:521.Google Scholar
58Rudner, M, Fransson, P, Ingvar, M, Nyberg, L, Rönnberg, J. Neural representation of binding lexical signs and words in the episodic buffer of working memory. Neuropsychologia 2007;45:22582276.Google Scholar
59Blue, CM. Influence of mode of presentation, age and intelligence on paired‐associates learning. Am J Ment Defic 1970;74:527532.Google Scholar
60Vakil, E, Shelef‐Reshef, E, Levy‐Shiff, R. Procedural and declarative memory processes: Individuals with and without mental retardation. Am J Ment Retard 1997;102:147160.Google Scholar
61Baddeley, AD and Hitesh, G. Working memory. In: Spence, KW and Spence, JT, eds. The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol 8. New York: Academic Press, 1974;6789.Google Scholar
62Basso, MR, Bornstein, RA. Effects of past noninjection drug abuse upon executive function and working memory in HIV infection. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2003;25:893903.Google Scholar
63George, O, Mandyam, CD, Wee, S, Koob, GF. Extended access to cocaine self‐administration produces long‐lasting prefrontal cortex‐dependent working memory impairments. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008;33:24742482.Google Scholar
64Ilan, AB, Smith, ME, Gevins, A. Effects of marijuana on neurophysiological signals of working and episodic memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;176:214222.Google Scholar
65Maurizio, P. High suicide risk after the development of cognitive and working memory deficits caused by cannabis, cocaine and ecstasy use. Subst Abus 2007;28:2530.Google Scholar
66Mayes, L, Snyder, PJ, Langlois, E, Hunter, N. Visuospatial working memory in school‐aged children exposed in utero to Cocaine. Child Neuropsychol 2007;13:205218.Google Scholar
67Schweinsburg, AD, Nagel, BJ, Schweinsburg, BC, Park, A, Theilmann, RJ, Tapert, SF. Abstinent adolescent marijuana users show altered fMRI response during spatial working memory. Psychiatry Res 2008;163:4051.Google Scholar
68Sudai, E, Croitoru, O, Shaldubina, A et al. High cocaine dosage decreases neurogenesis in the hippocampus and impairs working memory. Addict Biol 2011;16:251260.Google Scholar
69Verdejo‐García, AJ, López‐Torrecillas, F, Aguilar de Arcos, F, Pérez‐García, M. Differential effects of MDMA, cocaine and cannabis use severity on distinctive components of the executive functions in polysubstance users: A multiple regression analysis. Addict Behav 2005;30:89101.Google Scholar
70Bechara, A, Dolan, S, Hindes, A. Decision‐making and addiction: II. Myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? Neuropsychologia 2002;40:16901705.Google Scholar
71Breiter, HL, Aharon, I, Kahneman, D, Dale, A, Shizgal, P. Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience of monetary gains and losses. Neuron 2001;30:619639.Google Scholar
72Lundqvist, T. Cognitive consequences of cannabis use: comparison with abuse of stimulants and heroin with regard to attention, memory and executive functions. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2005;81:319330.Google Scholar
73Fuster, JM. Executive frontal functions. Exp Brain Res 2000;133:6670.Google Scholar
74Becker, B, Wagner, D, Gouzoulis‐Mayfrank, E, Spuentrup, E, Daumann, J. The impact of early‐onset cannabis use on functional brain correlates of working memory. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2010;34:837845.Google Scholar
75Smith, AM, Longo, CA, Fried, PA, Hogan, MJ, Cameron, I. Effects of marijuana on visuospatial working memory: an fMRI study in young adults. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010;210:429438.Google Scholar
76Schweinsburg, AD, Schweinsburg, BC, Medina, KL, McQueeny, T, Brown, SA, Tapert, SF. The influence of recency of use on fMRI response during spatial working memory in adolescent marijuana users. J Psychoactive Drugs 2010;42:401412.Google Scholar