Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T13:16:03.410Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Placebo and nocebo effects in gambling disorder pharmacological trials: a meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 November 2024

Konstantinos Ioannidis*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Clinical and Experimental Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, UK Hampshire and Isle of Wight Healthcare NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
Nathan T.M. Huneke
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Clinical and Experimental Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, UK
Jeremy E. Solly
Affiliation:
Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, UK
Guilherme Fusetto Veronesi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Clinical and Experimental Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, UK Hampshire and Isle of Wight Healthcare NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK
Charidimos Tzagarakis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Crete, Iraklion, Greece Organization Against Drugs (OKANA), Athens, Greece Department of Neuroscience, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Valeria Parlatini
Affiliation:
Hampshire and Isle of Wight Healthcare NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK Centre for Innovation in Mental Health, School of Psychology, Faculty of Environmental and Life Sciences, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
Samuel J. Westwood
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
Cinzia Del Giovane
Affiliation:
Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences for Children and Adults, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy Institute of Primary Health Care (BIHAM), University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
David S. Baldwin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Clinical and Experimental Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, UK
Jon E. Grant
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
Samuele Cortese
Affiliation:
Hampshire and Isle of Wight Healthcare NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK Clinical and Experimental Sciences (CNS and Psychiatry), Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK DiMePRe-J-Department of Precision and Rigenerative Medicine-Jonic Area, University of Bari ‘Aldo Moro’, Bari, Italy Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, New York, USA
Samuel R. Chamberlain
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Clinical and Experimental Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, UK Hampshire and Isle of Wight Healthcare NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK
*
Corresponding author: Konstantinos Ioannidis; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Placebo and nocebo effects are widely reported across psychiatric conditions, yet have seldom been examined in the context of gambling disorder. Through meta-analysis, we examined placebo effects, their moderating factors, and nocebo effects, from available randomised, controlled pharmacological clinical trials in gambling disorder.

Methods:

We searched, up to 19 February 2024, a broad range of databases, for double-blind randomised controlled trials (RCTs) of medications for gambling disorder. Outcomes were gambling symptom severity and quality of life (for efficacy), and drop outs due to medication side effects in the placebo arms.

Results:

We included 16 RCTs (n = 833) in the meta-analysis. The overall effect size for gambling severity reduction in the placebo arms was 1.18 (95%CI 0.91–1.46) and for quality of life improvement was 0.63 (0.42-0.83). Medication class, study sponsorship, trial duration, baseline severity of gambling and publication year significantly moderated effect sizes for at least some of these outcome measures. Author conflict of interest, placebo run-in, gender split, severity scale choice, age of participants or unbalanced randomisation did not moderate effect sizes. Nocebo effects leading to drop out from the trial were observed in 6% of participants in trials involving antipsychotics, while this was less for other medication types.

Conclusion:

Placebo effects in trials of pharmacological treatment of gambling disorder are large, and there are several moderators of this effect. Nocebo effects were measureable and may be influenced by medication class being studied. Practical implications of these new findings for the field are discussed, along with recommendations for future clinical trials.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology

Significant outcomes

  • Placebo effects in trials of pharmacological treatment of gambling disorder are large.

  • Medication class, study sponsorship, trial duration, baseline severity of gambling and publication year significantly moderated the placebo effect.

  • Nocebo effects were measurable and may be influenced by medication class being studied.

Significant limitations

  • The number of RCTs available for the pharmacological management of gambling disorder is relatively limited to date, as compared to other areas of mental ill health.

  • The presence or not of comorbidities was not comprehensively assessed in those RCTs, which limited the possibility of those being identified as moderators.

  • Meta-regression was not possible for blinding integrity, as this was not measured in those RCTs.

Introduction

Gambling disorder is a complex mental disorder characterised by persistent and recurrent gambling despite the evidence of negative consequences. It is classified as a behavioural addiction in the International classification of Disease 11th Edition (ICD-11) (ICD-11, 2021) and as a Substance-Related and Addictive Disorder in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual 5th Edition, text revised (DSM-5-TR) (American Psychiatric Association, 2022). Gambling Disorder has a substantial impact on those affected, as well as those around them. For example, it can lead to interpersonal conflict, serious financial problems, homelessness, bankruptcy, and elevated risk of suicide (The Lancet Public Health, 2021; Wardle & McManus, Reference Wardle and McManus2021; Ioannidis & Bowden-Jones, Reference Ioannidis and Bowden‐Jones2023) with substantial public health implications worldwide (The Lancet Public Health, 2021).

Psychological treatments (i.e. mainly in the form of gambling focused cognitive-behavioural therapy, CBT, in its many variants) comprise the current mainstay approach for the treatment of gambling disorder. Pharmacological treatments are also available, with opioid receptor antagonists (nalmefene, naltrexone) currently having the best evidence (Ioannidis et al., Reference Ioannidis, Del, Solly, Westwood, Parlatini, Bowden-Jones, Grant, Cortese and Chamberlain2024), although no pharmacological treatment is licensed for this indication (as is the case for many neglected mental disorders). In the emerging body of randomised clinical trials (RCTs) for gambling disorder, most active medication treatments have been compared against placebo treatment. Placebo and nocebo effects may have occurred in these trials. Placebo and nocebo are effects of patients’ positive and negative expectations relevant to an anticipated intervention; they are pertinent influencers in pharmacological trials in many areas of medicine (Colloca & Barsky, Reference Colloca and Barsky2020). While under-studied (Huneke, et al., Reference Huneke, Van Der Wee, Garner and Baldwin2020), it is likely that placebo and nocebo effects can occur due to a variety of mechanisms (e.g. expectations, interactions with the study team, therapeutic milieu, the choice of self-report versus objective measures (Huneke et al., Reference Huneke, Amin, Baldwin, Bellato, Brandt, Chamberlain, Correll, Eudave, Garner, Gosling, Hill, Hou, Howes, Ioannidis, Köhler-Forsberg, Marzulli, Reed, Sinclair, Singh, Solmi and Cortese2024)), and particular neurobiological pathways (Flaten, et al., Reference Flaten, Simonsen and Olsen1999; Barsky, et al., Reference Barsky2002; Wager & Atlas, Reference Wager and Atlas2015; Ashar, et al., Reference Ashar, Chang and Wager2017). It is argued that, particularly in psychiatry, the predictors and moderators of the placebo response are multiple, diverse and still to be discovered (Weimer, et al., Reference Weimer, Colloca and Enck2015).

It is known from empirical evidence (Huneke et al., Reference Huneke, Amin, Baldwin, Bellato, Brandt, Chamberlain, Correll, Eudave, Garner, Gosling, Hill, Hou, Howes, Ioannidis, Köhler-Forsberg, Marzulli, Reed, Sinclair, Singh, Solmi and Cortese2024) that the placebo effect has variable magnitude, which differs by disorder (e.g. RCTs of medication treatments for generalised anxiety disorder or depression have reported much larger placebo effects as compared to other disorders such as obsessive-compulsive disorder [OCD] or schizophrenia spectrum conditions). At the same time there has not been any comprehensive and in depth investigation of the size and moderating parameters for the placebo effects in gambling pharmacological RCTs.

To our knowledge, only two previous studies have explored potential moderators of placebo response in pharmacological trials for gambling disorder. First, in a previous synthesis of data from n = 152 patients (not systematically collected), placebo ‘responders’ (defined using a cut-off of 35% reduction in symptom severity on the Gambling Symptom Assessment Scale) remained in treatment for significantly longer, were more likely to report ‘enjoyment’ as a trigger for gambling, and were less likely to state that ‘boredom’ or ‘loneliness’ triggered their gambling, compared with ‘non-responders’ (J. Grant & Chamberlain, Reference Grant and Chamberlain2017). Second, in a previous meta-analysis of individual patient data from a selected subset of RCTs (6 studies, n placebo = 67) of the pharmacological management of gambling disorder, decreased baseline symptoms of anxiety, increased baseline symptoms of depression, and non-Caucasian ethnicity were associated with larger placebo response (Huneke, et al., Reference Huneke, Chamberlain, Baldwin and Grant2021). Intriguingly, these moderators differed from moderators associated with larger treatment response. Although these studies have identified possible important moderators of placebo responses in gambling disorder, they are based on an incomplete analysis exploring only a subset of the available data.

In the present study we sought to synthesise data from all the available literature to address the following key questions related to the placebo effect in pharmacological RCTs of gambling disorder: 1) what is the magnitude of placebo effect in available trials and does it differ across outcome measures 2) what factors moderate the placebo effect, 3) what is the magnitude of nocebo effect and does this differ depending on the medication class being examined?

Material and methods

For the identification of available RCTs for the pharmacological management of gambling disorder, the study followed methods described in the pre-registered protocol published on the PROSPERO International prospective register of systematic reviews [Registration number: CRD42022329520 Available from: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?RecordID=329520]. This study reporting followed the PRISMA guidelines (Shamseer et al., Reference Shamseer, Moher, Clarke, Ghersi, Liberati, Petticrew, Shekelle and Stewart2015).

Search strategy

We searched up to 19 February 2024, a broad range of databases, including MEDLINE, EMBASE, PsycINFO, PubMed, CINAHL, AMED, and the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, ERIC and Web of Science (including Science Citation Index Expanded (SCI-EXPANDED), Social Science Citation Index, Conference Proceedings Citation Index-Science (CPCI-S) and Conference Proceedings Citation Index-Social Science and Humanities (CPCI-SSH)) via Web of Knowledge and the WHO International Trials Registry Platform (including ClinicalTrials.gov). The search strings used and full list of electronic databases and clinical trial registries in which the search was conducted are available in the previous publication (Ioannidis, et al., Reference Ioannidis, Del, Solly, Westwood, Parlatini, Bowden-Jones, Grant, Cortese and Chamberlain2024).

Eligibility criteria

We included published or unpublished RCTs comparing an active medication versus placebo, for the treatment of Gambling Disorder/Pathological Gambling. Trials with a cross-over design were included if data from the pre cross-over phase were available, to avoid carry-over effects. We included only studies of adults (>18yrs) with a primary DSM (III onwards) or ICD (9 onwards) diagnosis of Gambling Disorder/Pathological Gambling.

Data extraction and outcomes

Details on data extraction were described in previous work and are available online (Ioannidis, et al., Reference Ioannidis, Del, Solly, Westwood, Parlatini, Bowden-Jones, Grant, Cortese and Chamberlain2024). Additional information about the ascertainment of placebo control processes were collected in this dataset, including 1) the presence of a placebo run-in process (‘Yes/No/Unclear’), use of independent investigators (not involved in clinical care) to assess side effects (‘Yes/No/Unclear’), prevention of side effects (‘Yes/No/Unclear’) and assessment of blinding success (‘Yes/No/Unclear’), 2) full details of nocebo effects (specific side effects produced by placebo, attributed to medication effects) 3) the presence of industry-related influences, including the presence of industry sponsorship (‘Yes/No/Unclear’) and the presence of declared industry-related conflict of interest from the manuscript authors in each respective publication (‘Yes/No/Unclear’).

Clinical measures of efficacy

The primary efficacy outcome was gambling symptom severity measured by well-established and validated instruments, namely the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale adapted for Pathological Gambling (PG-YBOCS), (Pallanti, et al., Reference Pallanti, DeCaria, Grant, Urpe and Hollander2005) the Gambling Symptom Assessment Scale (G-SAS), (Kim, et al., Reference Kim, Grant, Adson and Shin2001) and the Clinical Global Impression-Improvement scale (CGI-I) (Busner & Targum, Reference Busner and Targum2007). If a study reported results from multiple scales, we used the following hierarchy in the choice of the scale: PG-YBOCS as first preference; G-SAS as next preference, CGI-I as third preference. This was done to prioritise structured clinical instruments against unstructured or self-report instruments. The secondary efficacy outcome was the improvement in quality of life and functioning as measured by validated instruments including but not limited to the Sheehan Disability Scale (Sheehan, et al., Reference Sheehan, Harnett-Sheehan and Raj1996) and other quality of life metrics. Further information about the selection of severity metrics are presented in previous work (Ioannidis, et al., Reference Ioannidis, Del, Solly, Westwood, Parlatini, Bowden-Jones, Grant, Cortese and Chamberlain2024).

Nocebo outcomes

Nocebo effects were calculated by the percentage of drop outs from perceived ‘medication side effects’ from the placebo arms, in the RCTs.

Data synthesis

Data were analysed using statistical software R version 4.2.1. Meta-analysis was performed using packages of ‘robumeta’ and ‘metafor’ (Fisher, et al., Reference Fisher, Tipton and Zhipeng2017; Viechtbauer, Reference Viechtbauer2020). The R code used for this analysis is shared in the supplement. We performed analyses for gambling severity, using all types of outcomes, using a hierarchical approach described above, and then for each type of clinical severity outcome separately. We also performed a meta-analysis for quality of life outcomes; all the above were performed separately for the placebo and the treatment arms. We calculated the within-group Standardised Mean Change using Change score standardisation (Gibbons, et al., Reference Gibbons, Hedeker and Davis1993) (SMCC), in active medication and placebo arms separately, to measure the efficacy outcomes. For the calculations of SMCC, we first imputed a correlation of 0.50 between baseline and end-of-treatment within groups. We performed sensitivity analyses with correlations of 0.25 and 0.75 to ascertain any impact on the analyses. The measure of effect for nocebo was the dropout rate due to medication side effects (or attributed to medication side effects in the placebo arm, i.e. nocebo effects), expressed as percentage of drop out, in the placebo arms. Study arms randomising the same compound at different dose were merged into a single arm in line with the recommendations in the Cochrane handbook (The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011). We meta-analysed these outcomes through a random-effects model in all cases to provide a more generalisable model estimate. We ascertained heterogeneity by calculating the Q-statistic, which is the ratio of observed variation to within-study variance, formulated in a null hypothesis test (the null hypothesis here being that all individual studies measure/examine the same effect). The test indicates how much of the overall heterogeneity can be attributed to true between-studies variation. We also assessed the heterogeneity within each comparison visually by considering the forest plot, and quantitatively with the I2 statistic and the τ2. (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins, Thomas, Chandler, Cumpston, Li, Page and Welch2019). Moderator meta-regression analysis was conducted considering the following regressors: year of publication, medication class, presence of company sponsor, duration of the study, gambling severity scale choice, mean age of participants, percentage of gender split, baseline gambling severity scores, presence of declared conflict of interest in the authors and study design with unbalanced randomisation and placebo run-in phase. We calculated the correlation between the placebo effect sizes and the respective effect sizes for the treatment arms in each meta-analysis.

Publication bias assessment

We examined publication bias with the use of funnel plots (visual/graphic inspection for asymmetry), as well as with regression tests for funnel plot asymmetry. Where appropriate we used the trim and fill method to provide an updated effect size estimate.

Risk of bias assessment

For within-study bias, and to assess the methodological quality of each individual RCT included in our meta-analysis we used the Cochrane Risk of Bias tool version 2 (RoB2) (Cochrane, 2023).

Results

The search yielded 4261 references from electronic databases and 71 hits from clinical trial registries. A final set of 16 eligible RCTs were selected for inclusion in the meta-analyses. Full details about the search results are presented in the PRISMA flowchart (Fig. 1). Randomised participants across the included RCTs were ∼ 47% males (394/833), and their ages ranged from 36.2 to 51.5 years (Mean = 43.30; standard deviation SD = 3.38). Each of the 16 RCTs included in the meta-analysis (total participants: 833) contributed to one pairwise comparison (active treatment vs. placebo), totalling 16 pairwise comparisons across studies (16 for gambling severity, five for quality of life).

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart. Comparisons included in the meta-analysis comprised eleven different medications grouped in six classes: three opioid receptor antagonists (naltrexone, nalmefene, naloxone); two selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs – paroxetine and fluvoxamine); two mood stabilisers (topiramate, lithium); one norepinephrine–dopamine reuptake inhibitor (NDRI, bupropion); one antipsychotic (olanzapine); and two supplements (N-acetyl-cysteine, silymarin).

Placebo response

Placebo response (baseline to end-of-treatment) for the severity of gambling (all scales) was SMCC = 1.18 (95%CI 0.91-1.46), whereas for the specific gambling severity scales the effect sizes were 1.24 (95%CI 0.91-1.57) for PG-YBOCS, 1.05 (95%CI 0.79-1.31) for GSAS and 0.79 (95%CI 0.46-1.13) for CGI-I. Quality of life effect sizes were SMCC = 0.63 (95%CI 0.42-0.83) (see Fig. 2). For reference, under active treatment arms, overall gambling severity effect size was SMCC = 1.49 (95%CI 1.18-1.80), whereas quality of life effect was g = 0.69 (95%CI 0.55-0.84). Full details are available in the online supplement (paragraph §S1).

Figure 2. Forest plots meta-analysis of placebo effect in gambling severity (all studies, top left, per specific scale on the right hand side) in RCTs of pharmacological management of gambling disorder; ‘higher response’ indicates improvement in gambling symptom severity compared to baseline; placebo effect for quality of life outcomes (bottom left) in RCTs of pharmacological management of gambling disorder; ‘higher response’ indicates improvement in quality of life compared to baseline.

Heterogeneity within placebo arms

We identified moderate to high variance from heterogeneity measures within the analysed studies of the placebo arms, which was particularly prominent in the studies that used the PG-YBOCS instrument (see Table 1). Heterogeneity in the treatment arms followed a similar pattern (paragraph §S2).

Table 1. Heterogeneity measures

PG-YBOCS = Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale adapted for Pathological Gambling, GSAS = Gambling Symptom Assessment Scale, CGI-I = Clinical Global Impression-Improvement scale; SE = standard error; df = degrees of freedom.

Publication bias assessment

Publication bias was identified graphically and statistically in the case of quality of life, in which the trim and fill method suggested a more conservative placebo effect estimate (0.50 vs. 0.65), which was not present in the treatment arms (Fig. 3). Publication bias assessment for treatment arms is presented in the supplement (paragraph §S3).

Figure 3. Funnel plots. Funnel plots with regression for funnel plot asymmetry p-values. PG-YBOCS = yale-brown obsessive compulsive scale adapted for pathological gambling, GSAS = gambling symptom assessment scale, CGI-I = clinical global impression-improvement scale. Publication bias identified in the quality of life analysis and the trim and fill method was used to provide a new effect size estimate.

Meta-regression analysis

Meta-regression results are presented in detail in Table 2. For gambling severity, recency of publication, supplement medication class and the absence of company sponsorship were associated with higher placebo effect sizes. For gambling severity using PG-YBOCS only, recency of publication, supplement medication class, as well as higher severity of baseline symptoms were associated with higher placebo effect sizes. For gambling severity using GSAS only, supplement and opiate receptor antagonist medication classes and the absence of company sponsorship were associated with higher placebo effect sizes. For CGI, antipsychotics and supplement medication classes were associated with lower placebo response. In the moderation analysis of quality of life, shorter trial duration was associated with larger placebo response, whereas longer duration was associated with lower response. Lower baseline of symptoms was associated with lower placebo response. No other parameters were statistically associated with moderating effect sizes in relation to placebo response on symptom severity or quality of life. Full results for the treatment arms are presented in the supplementary materials (paragraph §S4).

Table 2. Meta-regression – PLACEBO effects

PG-YBOCS = Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale adapted for Pathological Gambling, GSAS = Gambling Symptom Assessment Scale, CGI-I = Clinical Global Impression-Improvement scale; QoL = Quality of life; E.S = effect size; n.s.= non-significant statistically; NA = not available/not appropriate; ORAs = Opiate receptor antagonists; MS = mood stabilisers; AP = antipsychotics; AD = antidepressants; COI = Conflict of Interest (defined as the presence of any declared industry-related conflict of interest by the authors in any section of the published manuscript); statistical significance: n.s. = non-significant; ‘.’ <0.10 (trend); ‘*’ <.05; ‘**’ <0.01; ‘***’ <0.001; NA = not available/not applicable; † = maintained in sensitivity analyses with r = 0.25; †† = maintained in sensitivity analyses with r = 0.75.

Correlation between the placebo effects and the treatment effects

We calculated the correlation between the placebo effect sizes and the respective effect sizes for the treatment arms in each meta-analysis. For gambling severity the placebo and treatment effect sizes were highly correlated in all analyses (see Table 3); however, this was not true for quality of life meta-analyses, in which the placebo and treatment effect sizes were weakly correlated.

Table 3. Correlation between effect sizes (Pearson)

Statistical significance: n.s. = non-significant; ‘.’ <0.10 (trend); ‘*’ <.05; ‘**’ <0.01; ‘***’ <0.001.

Sensitivity analyses

Full results at r = 0.25 and r = 0.75 are presented in the supplementary materials (paragraph §S6 & §S7). Sensitivity analyses did not substantially alter the results (see also Table 2 in which meta-regression analysis results maintained in sensitivity analyses are shown in tabularised format). The effect sizes were notably lower ‘overall’ at lower imputed correlations and higher overall at higher imputed correlation (i.e. SMCC for gambling severity was 1.00, 1.18 and 1.55 at r = 0.25, 0.50 and 0.75 respectively and SMCC for QOL was 0.51, 0.63 and 0.87 at r = 0.25, 0.50 and 0.75 respectively), however remained large in size for gambling severity and moderate-large for quality of life. Heterogeneity results were effectively similar in sensitivity analyses. Publication biases became more prominent at higher imputed correlations and the trim and fill method was required to produce more conservative (lower) effect estimates at r = 0.75 for most of the effect size estimates (see supplementary material §S7). The direction of correction from trim and fill was always towards the effects reported in the main paper (with r = 0.50).

Nocebo effects

Due to paucity of data, those were calculated for each pharmacological class separately. Results indicated 2.4% drop outs from placebo arms in the antidepressant trials (fluvoxamine, paroxetine), 6.1% placebo drop outs for antipsychotics (olanzapine), 1.9% placebo drop outs for opiate receptor antagonists (naltrexone, nalmefene, naloxone), 1.6% placebo drop outs for mood stabilisers (topiramate, lithium) and 0% placebo drop outs for supplements (NAC, silymarin). Detailed results are presented in the supplement (paragraph §S8)

Quality assessment

We completed RoB2 for all papers under scope. All domains had ‘low concern’ as the most common outcome, apart from Domain 5 (bias in selection of the reported result) in which ’some concern’ was the predominant outcome. Consequently, the majority of papers scored as ’some concern’ in the overall risk of bias. Full RoB2 scores are reported in the supplement (paragraph §S9).

Discussion

This is the first systematic review and meta-analysis of placebo effects in RCTs for the pharmacological management of gambling disorder. Our findings indicate that placebo effects are prominent and of large magnitude for clinical efficacy outcomes of gambling severity (across scales), and of moderate-large magnitude for quality of life clinical efficacy measures. We first consider the implications of these high placebo effects, and then discuss moderating factors, and nocebo effects.

Placebo response rates can be influenced by the condition being studied. For example, placebo response rates in clinical trials are generally relatively high for depression, panic disorder, or generalised anxiety disorder but are typically low for certain other mental health conditions such as OCD or schizophrenia (e.g. see (Bernstein, Brown, Professor of Psychiatry, & Behavior, 2017; Cao et al., Reference Cao, Liu, Selvitella, Librenza-Garcia, Passos, Sawalha, Ballester, Chen, Dong, Wang, Kapczinski, Dursun, Li, Greiner and Greenshaw2021; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Razza, Weissman, Karbi, Vine, Mulsant, Brunoni, Husain, Mulsant, Blumberger and Daskalakis2021; Huneke et al., Reference Huneke, Amin, Baldwin, Bellato, Brandt, Chamberlain, Correll, Eudave, Garner, Gosling, Hill, Hou, Howes, Ioannidis, Köhler-Forsberg, Marzulli, Reed, Sinclair, Singh, Solmi and Cortese2024)). Our finding of a relatively high placebo response rate for gambling disorder bears similarity with what has been found for other addictions such as alcohol use disorder, where high placebo response rates have been noted (Anton et al., Reference Anton, O’Malley, Ciraulo, Cisler, Couper, Donovan, Gastfriend, Hosking, Johnson, LoCastro, Longabaugh, Mason, Mattson, Miller, Pettinati, Randall, Swift, Weiss, Williams and Zweben2006; Del Re, et al., Reference Del Re, Maisel, Blodgett, Wilbourne and Finney2013; Scherrer et al., Reference Scherrer, Guiraud, Addolorato, Aubin, de Bejczy, Benyamina, van den Brink, Caputo, Dematteis, Goudriaan, Gual, Kiefer, Leggio, Lesch, Maremmani, Nutt, Paille, Perney, Poulnais, Raffaillac, Rehm, Rolland, Simon, Söderpalm, Sommer, Walter and Spanagel2021), and places gambling disorder among the mental disorders with the largest placebo effect sizes (Huneke et al., Reference Huneke, Amin, Baldwin, Bellato, Brandt, Chamberlain, Correll, Eudave, Garner, Gosling, Hill, Hou, Howes, Ioannidis, Köhler-Forsberg, Marzulli, Reed, Sinclair, Singh, Solmi and Cortese2024). The finding of a high placebo response rate for gambling disorder has a number of implications. Firstly, the results suggest that people who seek treatment for gambling disorder experience notable symptom improvement that is not directly related to the active medication compound being examined – from the perspective of patient outcomes this is a positive, and highlights the potential importance of factors such as therapeutic alliance and non-specific support from research and clinical teams. Secondly, large placebo response potentially represents a design problem for pharmacological clinical trials of gambling disorder, by making it challenging to detect a true active effect of medication. Intriguingly, we found that quality of life measures show a greater disparity between placebo and treatment response. How different outcome measures affect estimates of treatment effect size needs to be further understood. The high placebo response also highlights a disparity in the literature for how different gambling disorder treatment modalities are compared (or contrasted) with each other. In particular, many psychological intervention clinical trials in gambling disorder have used waiting list control (Petry, et al., Reference Petry, Ginley and Rash2017). In practical terms, this suggests that pharmacological options should be considered more often in clinical practice, as they have had to pass a higher clinical standard to show their efficacy in trials. It is likely that randomisation to wait list results in reduced symptom improvement compared with a placebo due to fewer clinical interactions and lower expectations of benefit and/or ‘disappointment’ (Bandelow et al., Reference Bandelow, Reitt, Röver, Michaelis, Görlich and Wedekind2015), thereby substantially inflating effect sizes for active therapy versus control. This issue of inflating treatment effect sizes by using a weak control condition has also been noted for other compulsive conditions such as OCD (Laws, et al., Reference Laws, Pellegrini, Reid, Drummond and Fineberg2022). In contrast, controlled clinical trials of medications have used placebo control, which is much more conservative and scientifically rigorous. This issue needs to be considered when weighing the balance in favour of particular treatment modalities for gambling disorder. Another consideration is around risks – with both medications and therapies being associated with adverse effects for some individuals, yet adverse events are almost always documented in pharmacological trials but rarely in psychological trials in the field (Klatte, et al., Reference Klatte, Strauss, Flückiger, Färber and Rosendahl2023).

Little is yet known about the neurobiological determinants of the placebo effects in pharmacological trials of gambling disorder. We know that, in general, the placebo effect is mediated by diverse neurobiological processes, including learning, expectation and social cognition (Wager & Atlas, Reference Wager and Atlas2015). Commonalities in the neurobiological characteristics of gamblers, for example heightened impulsivity (Ioannidis, et al., Reference Ioannidis, Hook, Wickham, Grant and Chamberlain2019) or a dysregulated anticipatory dopaminergic response (Linnet, Reference Linnet2020) could predispose to heightened placebo or nocebo responses, by altering underlying learning and reward expectation processes. Future studies could further investigate those neurobiological processes involved in the production of placebo and nocebo effects in gambling disorder.

Study design elements, including blinding integrity

Study duration significantly moderated placebo response specifically for one symptom severity measure (CGI) and also for quality of life: with shorter duration linked to lower placebo effect on CGI, and linked to larger placebo effect on quality of life. The reasons for these results are unclear. We did not find that unbalanced randomisation moderated any of the meta-analyses effects.

In addition, placebo run-in (present in 38.9% or 7 out of 18 studies) did not significantly moderate the placebo response, indicating that inclusion of such a run-in did not help minimise placebo effect size – contrary to what might be anticipated. Nonetheless, this accords with findings in antidepressant trials, where placebo run-in periods are associated with reductions in treatment response in both arms and thereby not altering the efficacy comparison between medication and placebo (Scott, et al., Reference Scott, Sharpe, Quinn and Colagiuri2022). Given that using placebo run-in periods does not minimise placebo effect size, could affect the external validity of the trial, and necessarily involves deception which could be considered unethical, we would recommend against their use in pharmacological trials for gambling disorder.

We also found that none of the studies assessed blinding success (i.e. through debriefing of participants and/or research staff). One study made a specific effort to prevent side effects (J. E. Grant, et al., Reference Grant, Suck and Hartman2008) and utilised an independent investigator at expected peak of side effects to avoid un-blinding due to common side effects.

Medication class

Turning now to significant moderators of placebo effects in this study, we found that the supplements medication class, here including n-acetyl cysteine (NAC) and silymarin, moderated a higher placebo effect size for symptom severity measures overall. This is interesting in that supplement medications are often perceived by service users as ‘innocuous’ or not likely to have any side effects (Ernst, Reference Ernst1998). Stronger placebo responses might occur because individuals perceive supplements as a ‘more natural’ way to correct symptoms or more acceptable. Another possibility is that due to relatively good side effect profiles observed for these supplements in clinical trial conditions, blinding may have been ‘truer’ as compared to studies of other classes of medications that are more likely to have side effects. However, we currently do not have the data to make inferences regarding the effect that side effects or beliefs might have on blinding and thus estimates of efficacy. Future studies could address the success or otherwise of blinding by debriefing both study participants and investigators after trial completion with standard instruments (Haq, et al., Reference Haq, Molteni and Huneke2024).

Recency of publication

We found that later year of publication predicted higher placebo effect sizes, for symptom severity measures overall. One could expect that newer studies would follow more rigorous approaches to standardising placebo procedures, leading to diminished or stable placebo effects, however this is not the case here. Interestingly, across psychiatric disorders in general, it has been shown that placebo effects have been larger with more recent publications (Weimer et al., Reference Weimer, Colloca and Enck2015; Huneke et al., Reference Huneke, Amin, Baldwin, Bellato, Brandt, Chamberlain, Correll, Eudave, Garner, Gosling, Hill, Hou, Howes, Ioannidis, Köhler-Forsberg, Marzulli, Reed, Sinclair, Singh, Solmi and Cortese2024). The reasons for this change are simply not known, but findings herein for gambling disorder are consistent with those in many other mental health conditions.

Industry influence

We found that the absence of company sponsorship was associated with higher effect sizes in the placebo arms for symptom severity measures overall. This is a novel result with a lot of interesting implications. It is possible that participants being aware of company sponsorship are negatively influenced towards the beneficial effects of the medication treatment, thus generating a lesser placebo response. However, it has also been shown that industry sponsorship is associated with higher effect sizes in RCTs in general (Lundh, et al., Reference Lundh, Lexchin, Mintzes, Schroll and Bero2017), in which case this applies to both treatment and placebo arms, which are highly correlated in this dataset. Another possibility is that clinical trials conducted in academic settings may offer more additional support (irrespective of randomisation) as contrasted to commercial studies potentially involving generic / non-specialist recruitment sites, such as spending time with expert clinicians who take time to speak with patients or offer formal psychological support. Interestingly, self-reported industry-related conflict of interest from the manuscripts authors did not moderate any effect sizes, meaning that we did not find any evidence that those declared relationships from the authors were associated with the effect sizes reported. It is possible that moderation by absence of industry sponsorship was related to the moderation by medication class, as both studies which included supplements were unsponsored.

Choice of severity instrument, baseline severity

While placebo effects were numerically larger in the studies which assessed gambling severity improvements via clinician structured instruments, than self-reported instruments, or unstructured clinical instruments, choice of severity instrument was not statistically significant in moderation analyses, suggesting that approaches similarly capture placebo effects, in terms of gambling severity. However, those scales had differences as well; for example, we found higher baseline symptom severity leading to higher placebo response, using the PG-YBOCS instrument, which was also true for the treatment arms, using both PG-YBOCS and GSAS. That is an interesting finding, which may suggest that some placebo effects follow a ‘regression to the mean’ pattern (Cummings et al., Reference Cummings, Tractenberg, Gamst, Teri, Masterman and Thal2004), that is can be attributed to statistical artefacts which are unequally distributed across levels of baseline severity, or that they are impacted by floor effects. Furthermore, it is interesting that the reverse has been shown prior to the current study, for placebo effects in alcohol dependence RCTs (Scherrer et al., Reference Scherrer, Guiraud, Addolorato, Aubin, de Bejczy, Benyamina, van den Brink, Caputo, Dematteis, Goudriaan, Gual, Kiefer, Leggio, Lesch, Maremmani, Nutt, Paille, Perney, Poulnais, Raffaillac, Rehm, Rolland, Simon, Söderpalm, Sommer, Walter and Spanagel2021) and quality of life followed the same reverse pattern (lower baseline was associated with higher effect seizes) so this result merits exploration in future work.

Nocebo effects

In terms of nocebo effects, low placebo dropout rates (<2%) were found for clinical trials focusing on supplements (NAC, silymarin), mood stabilisers, and opioid receptor antagonists; slightly higher for SSRIs (2.4%); and higher rates were observed for placebo drop outs for antipsychotic (olanzapine) studies (6.1%). This variability could reflect expectation (e.g. people may anticipate ‘worse’ side effects for olanzapine had they read about it in advance, not knowing if they were then assigned to active or placebo treatment (Faasse et al., Reference Faasse, Huynh, Pearson, Geers, Helfer and Colagiuri2019)). Low nocebo effects is useful because it reduces clinical trial drop out, which can undermine the integrity of clinical trials.

Limitations

Several possible limitations should be considered, reflecting both limitations of the included RCTs and of our meta-analysis. In terms of the included RCTs, the number available for gambling disorder is relatively limited to date, as compared to other areas of mental ill health (e.g. depression). The reasons for this are multi-fold, and include a historical (and persisting) lack of funding for gambling disorder research from independent national funding schemes globally. Available RCTs also have a number of methodological issues such as relatively small sample sizes (in many cases) as compared to other areas of mental ill health. The available studies did not generally examine views towards medications (e.g. pharmaceuticals versus nutraceuticals), expectation, or other variables (e.g. personality) that may relate to placebo response. We also identified a moderate-high degree of heterogeneity, particularly in the meta-analyses of gambling severity using the PG-YBOCS scale; this might reflect differences in other elements of study design (e.g. duration, presence of comorbidities etc). Specifically, when it comes to the placebo effect, the presence of comorbidities may play a role. Particularly axis-II issues may influence placebo responses (Yadav, Reference Yadav2020) and those have been under-investigated in gambling disorder RCTs.

In terms of limitations of the meta-analysis, we could not consider time course of placebo responses – for example, we could not establish who responded to placebo for the whole of a given study as opposed to responding just at the end. The meta-regression analyses could not be conducted for all a priori hypothesised predictors due to lack of data in given categories (e.g. no study assessed blinding integrity); and it should also be considered that the number of studies in the explored moderator categories was small in some cases (e.g. having only one RCT with unbalanced randomisation), potentially limiting power to detect effects of moderators. The other limitation of the moderation analyses is that those were not corrected for multiple comparisons, which suggests that those should be considered as exploratory, requiring further investigation once (if) a substantially larger number of clinical trials become available for analysis in the future. Finally, one of the limitations of examining nocebo effects in this analysis is that we were only able to explore the aspect of nocebo effect which led to drop out from the trials. Other, potentially more minor nocebo effects, which may though influence clinical efficacy or blinding success, were not consistently reported to allow for robust examination. Future studies should maintain a consistent study design and reporting, to allow for the reduction of heterogeneity, but also to include study design elements like the assessment of comorbidities, assessment of blinding success and nocebo effect details, to aid the interpretation of future findings.

Conclusions and implications for future research

The current meta-analysis provides insights into the importance of placebo effects in pharmacological RCTs for gambling disorder. Considering these effects is also of relevance to routine clinical practice, for example, being aware of placebo effects may encourage clinicians to spend more time addressing a patient’s beliefs and attitudes around medication before starting treatment to enhance chances they positively respond. We found that placebo effects are prominent (large) across these trials and that there are several moderators, on at least some outcome measures of these placebo effects (year of publication, medication class, duration of treatment, company sponsorship status). Nocebo effects were measurable and may be influenced by medication class being studied in a particular trial. The study also found a lack of assessment of blinding integrity across the available RCTs and unclear use of independent raters. Future studies should consider those parameters when designing RCTs for the assessment of clinical efficacy for the pharmacological management of gambling disorder. It remains unclear how best to minimise (or at least make predictable) placebo effects in clinical trials for gambling disorder, because placebo ‘run-in’ did not significantly moderate placebo response herein. Finally, we recommend that future RCTs of psychological interventions for gambling disorder adopt rigorous control conditions and measure adverse events – to ensure parity with the standards already being set by pharmacological RCTs in the field.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/neu.2024.52.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available upon reasonable request from the corresponding author (e.g. for peer review purposes) and will be published upon acceptance of the manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank authors of published papers included in this meta-analysis who responded to requests for additional information to enable the meta-analysis.

Author contribution

KI, CT, JES, SC, SRC contributed to the design of the study; CT and JES led the search and screening process; KI, CDG, JES, SRC, GFV contributed to data collection. KI, JES, and SRC had access to the data. VP and SJW conducted the RoB2 and contributed equally to the manuscript. KI conducted the meta-analysis and takes responsibility for the integrity and accuracy of the data analysis itself. All authors have intellectually contributed and reviewed the final submitted manuscript. All authors accept responsibility for the conduct of the study and its integrity.

Financial support

This study was supported by unrestricted grant funds to Professor Chamberlain held at the University of Southampton, originating from the NHS. The funding source had no role in the design, conduct, or reporting of the study.

Competing interests

Dr Ioannidis is clinical lead for the Southern Gambling Service and receives a stipend from Elsevier for journal editorial work. Professor Chamberlain is service director for the NHS Southern Gambling Service. Professor Chamberlain receives a stipend from Elsevier for journal editorial work. Drs Solly and Fusetto Veronesi are NIHR Academic Clinical Fellows, and Dr Huneke is an NIHR Clinical Lecturer. Professor Baldwin receives a stipend from Wiley for journal editorial work. Cinzia del Giovane’s time on the project was funded partly through the grant funding to SRC. Dr Grant has received research grants from Janssen and Biohaven Pharmaceuticals. He receives yearly compensation from Springer Publishing for acting as Editor-in-Chief of the Journal of Gambling Studies and has received royalties from Oxford University Press, American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc., Norton Press, and McGraw Hill. None of the authors have conflicts of interest in relation to the gambling or gaming industry. Samuele Cortese, NIHR Research Professor (NIHR303122) is funded by the NIHR for this research project. Samuele Cortese is also supported by NIHR grants NIHR203684, NIHR203035, NIHR130077, NIHR128472, RP-PG-0618-20003 and by grant 101095568-HORIZONHLTH-2022-DISEASE-07-03 from the European Research Executive Agency. Prof. Cortese has declared reimbursement for travel and accommodation expenses from the Association for Child and Adolescent Central Health (ACAMH) in relation to lectures delivered for ACAMH, the Canadian AADHD Alliance Resource, the British Association of Psychopharmacology, and from Healthcare Convention for educational activity on ADHD, and has received honoraria from Medice. The views expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NIHR, NHS or the UK Department of Health and Social Care. None of the authors accept voluntary donations from the gambling or gaming industry either personally or in terms of institutional funds held in their name.

Analytic code availability

Example R code and package information for meta-analysis can be found in the supplementary material (§S10).

References

American Psychiatric Association (2022) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edn. American Psychiatric Association. Text Revision (DSM-5-TR). DOI: 10.1176/APPI.BOOKS.9780890425787.Google Scholar
Anton, RF, O’Malley, SS, Ciraulo, DA, Cisler, RA, Couper, D, Donovan, DM, Gastfriend, DR, Hosking, JD, Johnson, BA, LoCastro, JS, Longabaugh, R, Mason, BJ, Mattson, ME, Miller, WR, Pettinati, HM, Randall, CL, Swift, R, Weiss, RD, Williams, LD, Zweben, A, COMBINE Study Research Group for the (2006) Combined pharmacotherapies and behavioral interventions for alcohol dependence: the COMBINE study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 295(17), 2003. DOI: 10.1001/JAMA.295.17.2003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashar, YK, Chang, LJ and Wager, TD (2017) Brain mechanisms of the placebo effect: an affective appraisal account. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 13(1), 7389. DOI: 10.1146/ANNUREV-CLINPSY-021815-093015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandelow, B, Reitt, M, Röver, C, Michaelis, S, Görlich, Y and Wedekind, D (2015) Efficacy of treatments for anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. International Clinical Psychopharmacology 30(4), 183192. DOI: 10.1097/YIC.0000000000000078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsky, AJ (2002) Nonspecific medication side effects and the nocebo phenomenon. JAMA 287(5), 622627. DOI: 10.1001/JAMA.287.5.622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, M. H., Brown, W. A., Professor of Psychiatry, C., & Behavior, H (2017) The placebo effect in psychiatric practice. Current Psychiatry 16(11), 2934. Retrieved from /pmc/articles/PMC6003660/.Google Scholar
Busner, J and Targum, SD (2007) The clinical global impressions scale: Applying a research tool in clinical practice. Psychiatry (Edgmont) 4(7), 2837. Retrieved from /pmc/articles/PMC2880930/.Google ScholarPubMed
Cao, B, Liu, YS, Selvitella, A, Librenza-Garcia, D, Passos, IC, Sawalha, J, Ballester, P, Chen, J, Dong, S, Wang, F, Kapczinski, F, Dursun, SM, Li, X-M, Greiner, R, Greenshaw, A (2021) Differential power of placebo across major psychiatric disorders: a preliminary meta-analysis and machine learning study. Scientific Reports 11(1), 19. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-99534-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cochrane (2023) RoB 2: A revised cochrane risk-of-bias tool for randomized trials | cochrane bias. Retrieved June 25, 2023. Available at https://methods.cochrane.org/bias/resources/rob-2-revised-cochrane-risk-bias-tool-randomized-trials.Google Scholar
Collaboration The Cochrane (2011). Cochrane handbook, 7.7 extracting study results and converting to the desired format. Retrieved from 5.1.0. Available at: https://handbook-5-1.cochrane.org/chapter_7/7_7_extracting_study_results_and_converting_to_the_desired.htm.Google Scholar
Colloca, L and Barsky, AJ (2020) Placebo and Nocebo Effects. The New England journal of medicine 382(6), 554561. DOI: 10.1056/NEJMRA1907805/SUPPL_FILE/NEJMRA1907805_DISCLOSURES.PDF.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, JL, Tractenberg, RE, Gamst, A, Teri, L, Masterman, D and Thal, LJ (2004) Regression to the mean: Implications for clinical trials of psychotropic agents in dementia. Current Alzheimer Research 1(4), 323328. DOI: 10.2174/1567205043332027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Re, AC, Maisel, N, Blodgett, JC, Wilbourne, P and Finney, JW (2013) Placebo group improvement in trials of pharmacotherapies for alcohol use disorders: a multivariate meta-analysis examining change over time. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 33(5), 649657. DOI: 10.1097/JCP.0B013E3182983E73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernst, E (1998) Harmless herbs? A review of the recent literature. The American Journal of Medicine 104(2), 170178. DOI: 10.1016/S0002-9343(97)00397-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faasse, K, Huynh, A, Pearson, S, Geers, AL, Helfer, SG and Colagiuri, B (2019) The influence of side effect information framing on nocebo effects. Annals of Behavioral Medicine : A Publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine 53(7), 621629. DOI: 10.1093/ABM/KAY071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, Z, Tipton, E and Zhipeng, H (2017). Robumeta: an R-package for robust variance estimation in meta-analysis, Retrieved June 27, 2020, from CRAN Repository website: https://github.com/zackfisher/robumeta Google Scholar
Flaten, MA, Simonsen, T and Olsen, H (1999) Drug-related information generates placebo and nocebo responses that modify the drug response. Psychosomatic Medicine 61(2), 250255. DOI: 10.1097/00006842-199903000-00018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbons, RD, Hedeker, DR and Davis, JM (1993) Estimation of effect size from a series of experiments involving paired comparisons. Http: //Dx. Doi. Org/10.3102/10769986018003271 18(3), 271279. DOI: 10.3102/10769986018003271.Google Scholar
Grant, J and Chamberlain, S (2017) The placebo effect and its clinical associations in gambling disorder. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry 3, 167172. Retrieved from https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28738096/ Google Scholar
Grant, JE, Suck, WK and Hartman, BK (2008) A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of the opiate antagonist naltrexone in the treatment of pathological gambling urges. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 69(5), 783789. DOI: 10.4088/JCP.v69n0511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haq, R, Molteni, L and Huneke, N (2024) The relationship between blinding integrity and medication efficacy in randomised-controlled trials in patients with anxiety disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 150(4), 187197. DOI: 10.1111/acps.13741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, J, Thomas, J, Chandler, J, Cumpston, M, Li, T, Page, M and Welch, V (2019) Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions, 2nd Edn. Chichester (UK): John Wiley & Sons CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huneke, NTM, Amin, J, Baldwin, DS, Bellato, A, Brandt, V, Chamberlain, SR, Correll, CU, Eudave, L, Garner, M, Gosling, CJ, Hill, CM, Hou, R, Howes, OD, Ioannidis, K, Köhler-Forsberg, O, Marzulli, L, Reed, C, Sinclair, JMA, Singh, S, Solmi, M, Cortese, S (2024) Placebo effects in randomized trials of pharmacological and neurostimulation interventions for mental disorders: An umbrella review. Molecular Psychiatry. Online ahead of print. DOI: 10.1038/S41380-024-02638-X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huneke, NTM, Chamberlain, SR, Baldwin, DS and Grant, JE (2021) Diverse predictors of treatment response to active medication and placebo in gambling disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research 144, 96101. DOI: 10.1016/J.JPSYCHIRES.2021.09.053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huneke, NTM, Van Der Wee, N, Garner, M and Baldwin, DS (2020) Why we need more research into the placebo response in psychiatry. Psychological Medicine 50(14), 23172323. DOI: 10.1017/S0033291720003633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
ICD-11 (2021) ICD-11 for mortality and morbidity statistics. Retrieved September 19, 2021. Available at: https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en#/http%3A%2F%2Fid.who.int%2Ficd%2Fentity%2F263852475.Google Scholar
Ioannidis, K, Bowden‐Jones, H (2023) Online gambling among young men: A rising tide? Trends in Urology & Men’s Health 14(5), 69. DOI: 10.1002/TRE.929.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ioannidis, K, Del, G., Solly, JE, Westwood, S J, Parlatini, V, Bowden-Jones, H, Grant, JE, Cortese, H and Chamberlain, SR (2024) Pharmacological management of gambling disorder: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. MedRxiv. DOI: 10.1101/2023.10.20.Google Scholar
Ioannidis, K, Hook, R, Wickham, K, Grant, JE and Chamberlain, SR (2019) Impulsivity in gambling disorder and problem gambling: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychopharmacology 44(8), 13541361. DOI: 10.1038/s41386-019-0393-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, BDM, Razza, LB, Weissman, CR, Karbi, J, Vine, T, Mulsant, LS, Brunoni, AR, Husain, MI, Mulsant, BH, Blumberger, DM, Daskalakis, ZJ (2021) Magnitude of the placebo response across treatment modalities used for treatment-resistant depression in adults: A Systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Network Open 4(9), e2125531. DOI: 10.1001/JAMANETWORKOPEN.2021.25531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, SW, Grant, JE, Adson, DE and Shin, YC (2001) Double-blind naltrexone and placebo comparison study in the treatment of pathological gambling. Biological Psychiatry 49(11), 914921. DOI: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01079-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klatte, R, Strauss, B, Flückiger, C, Färber, F and Rosendahl, J (2023) Defining and assessing adverse events and harmful effects in psychotherapy study protocols: A systematic review. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill) 60(1), 130148. DOI: 10.1037/PST0000359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laws, KR, Pellegrini, L, Reid, JE, Drummond, LM and Fineberg, NA (2022) The inflating impact of waiting-list controls on effect size estimates. Frontiers in Psychiatry 13, 877089. DOI: 10.3389/FPSYT.2022.877089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linnet, J (2020) The anticipatory dopamine response in addiction: a common neurobiological underpinning of gambling disorder and substance use disorder? Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 98, 109802. DOI: 10.1016/J.PNPBP.2019.109802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundh, A, Lexchin, J, Mintzes, B, Schroll, JB and Bero, L (2017) Industry sponsorship and research outcome. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2017(2), 86–87. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.MR000033.PUB3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pallanti, S, DeCaria, CM, Grant, JE, Urpe, M and Hollander, E (2005) Reliability and validity of the pathological gambling adaptation of the yale-brown obsessive-compulsive scale (PG-YBOCS). Journal of Gambling Studies 21(4), 431443. DOI: 10.1007/S10899-005-5557-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petry, NM, Ginley, MK and Rash, CJ (2017) A systematic review of treatments for problem gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors : Journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors 31(8), 951961. DOI: 10.1037/ADB0000290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherrer, B, Guiraud, J, Addolorato, G, Aubin, H‐Jean, de Bejczy, A, Benyamina, A, van den Brink, W, Caputo, F, Dematteis, M, Goudriaan, AE, Gual, A, Kiefer, F, Leggio, L, Lesch, O‐Michael, Maremmani, I, Nutt, DJ, Paille, Fçois, Perney, P, Poulnais, R, Raffaillac, Q, Rehm, Jürgen, Rolland, B, Simon, N, Söderpalm, B, Sommer, WH, Walter, H, Spanagel, R (2021) Baseline severity and the prediction of placebo response in clinical trials for alcohol dependence: a meta-regression analysis to develop an enrichment strategy. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 45(9), 17221734. DOI: 10.1111/ACER.14670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, AJ, Sharpe, L, Quinn, V and Colagiuri, B (2022) Association of single-blind placebo run-in periods with the placebo response in randomized clinical trials of antidepressants: A Systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 79(1), 4249. DOI: 10.1001/JAMAPSYCHIATRY.2021.3204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamseer, L, Moher, D, Clarke, M, Ghersi, D, Liberati, A, Petticrew, M, Shekelle, P, Stewart, LA, The PRISMA-P Group (2015) Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (prisma-p) 2015: Elaboration and explanation. BMJ (Online) 349(jan02 1), g7647g7647. DOI: 10.1136/bmj.g7647.Google Scholar
Sheehan, DV, Harnett-Sheehan, K, Raj, BA (1996) The measurement of disability. International Clinical Psychopharmacology 11(Supplement 3), 8995. DOI: 10.1097/00004850-199606003-00015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
The Lancet Public Health (2021) Gambling: A neglected public health issue. The Lancet Public Health 6(1), e1. DOI: 10.1016/S2468-2667(20)30290-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viechtbauer, W (2020) Package ‘metafor’; meta-analysis package for R.Google Scholar
Wager, TD and Atlas, LY (2015) The neuroscience of placebo effects: connecting context, learning and health. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 16(7), 403418. DOI: 10.1038/NRN3976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wardle, H and McManus, S (2021) Suicidality and gambling among young adults in great Britain: Results from a cross-sectional online survey. The Lancet Public Health 6(1), e39e49. DOI: 10.1016/S2468-2667(20)30232-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weimer, K, Colloca, L and Enck, P (2015) Placebo effects in psychiatry: mediators and moderators. The Lancet Psychiatry 2(3), 246257. DOI: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00092-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yadav, D (2020) Prescribing in borderline personality disorder – the clinical guidelines. Progress in Neurology and Psychiatry 24(2), 2530. DOI: 10.1002/PNP.667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart. Comparisons included in the meta-analysis comprised eleven different medications grouped in six classes: three opioid receptor antagonists (naltrexone, nalmefene, naloxone); two selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs – paroxetine and fluvoxamine); two mood stabilisers (topiramate, lithium); one norepinephrine–dopamine reuptake inhibitor (NDRI, bupropion); one antipsychotic (olanzapine); and two supplements (N-acetyl-cysteine, silymarin).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Forest plots meta-analysis of placebo effect in gambling severity (all studies, top left, per specific scale on the right hand side) in RCTs of pharmacological management of gambling disorder; ‘higher response’ indicates improvement in gambling symptom severity compared to baseline; placebo effect for quality of life outcomes (bottom left) in RCTs of pharmacological management of gambling disorder; ‘higher response’ indicates improvement in quality of life compared to baseline.

Figure 2

Table 1. Heterogeneity measures

Figure 3

Figure 3. Funnel plots. Funnel plots with regression for funnel plot asymmetry p-values. PG-YBOCS = yale-brown obsessive compulsive scale adapted for pathological gambling, GSAS = gambling symptom assessment scale, CGI-I = clinical global impression-improvement scale. Publication bias identified in the quality of life analysis and the trim and fill method was used to provide a new effect size estimate.

Figure 4

Table 2. Meta-regression – PLACEBO effects

Figure 5

Table 3. Correlation between effect sizes (Pearson)

Supplementary material: File

Ioannidis et al. supplementary material

Ioannidis et al. supplementary material
Download Ioannidis et al. supplementary material(File)
File 2.6 MB