Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:13:42.263Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Anxiolytic-like effects of acute serotonin-releasing agents in zebrafish models of anxiety: experimental study and systematic review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 October 2024

Jakob Näslund*
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology at the Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology at the Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
Jenny Landin
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology at the Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
Fredrik Hieronymus
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology at the Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
Rakesh Kumar Banote
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology at the Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden Department of Neurology, Sahlgrenska University Hosp1ital, Gothenburg, Sweden Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Sweden
Petronella Kettunen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology at the Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, John Radcliffe Hospital, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
*
Corresponding author: Jakob Näslund; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Though commonly used to model affective disorders, zebrafish display notable differences in terms of the structure and function of the brain serotonin system, including responses to pharmacological interventions, as compared to mammals. For example, elevation of brain serotonin following acute administration of serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SRIs) generally has anxiogenic effects, both in the clinical situation and in rodent models of anxiety, but previous research has indicated the opposite in zebrafish. However, several issues remain unresolved. We conducted a systematic review of SRI effects in zebrafish models of anxiety and, on the basis of these results, performed a series of experiments further investigating the influence of serotonin-releasing agents on anxiety-like behaviour in zebrafish, with sex-segregated wild-type animals being administered either escitalopram, or the serotonin releaser fenfluramine, in the light-dark test. In the systematic review, we find that the available literature indicates an anxiolytic-like effect of SRIs in the novel-tank diving test. Regarding the light-dark test, most studies reported no behavioural effects of SRIs, although the few that did generally saw anxiolytic-like responses. In the experimental studies, consistent anxiolytic-like effects were observed with neither sex nor habituation influencing treatment response. We find that the general effect of acute SRI administration in zebrafish indeed appears to be anxiolytic-like, indicating, at least partly, differences in the functioning of the serotonin system as compared to mammals and that caution is advised when using zebrafish to model affective disorders.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology

Significant outcomes

  • Published zebrafish studies employing the novel-tank diving test almost unequivocally indicate an anxiolytic-like effect of acute serotonin elevation, in contrast with rodent studies and the clinical situation. For the other major anxiety test in zebrafish, the light-dark test, the literature is less comprehensive, and the effects of acute SRI treatment are less clear, possibly due to sub-optimal doses often being used.

  • Results from the experimental study here reported that investigated a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), as well as the serotonin releaser fenfluramine, in the light-dark test while controlling for possible effects of sex and habituation indicate that the effects of acute serotonin elevation also in this test are anxiolytic-like. This (as well as several lines of evidence from earlier work) indicates significant differences in terms of the behavioural pharmacology of the serotonin system in zebrafish as compared to mammals.

Limitations

  • For 3R reasons, animals were re-used; however, a separate experiment conducted beforehand indicated that this was unlikely to impact SSRI responses in the light-dark test, especially considering the amount of time allowed to elapse between each session.

  • Though individual differences in temperament may influence responses to pharmacological agents, we neither assayed baseline temperament differences nor tagged animals in order to track the performance of individual fish across the experiments.

Highlights

  • Zebrafish appear to behave differently to mammals when given drugs that rapidly increase serotonin levels: while rodents and humans react with higher levels of fear or anxiety, zebrafish instead appear less ‘anxious’.

  • This seems to hold also when controlling for sex, previous experience of the testing situation and across different classes of drugs.

  • This may indicate that zebrafish are a less-than-optimal choice of model organism when studying, for example, anxiety and depression, especially considering other differences in terms of the functioning of the serotonin system between zebrafish and mammals.

Introduction

Despite decades of research, assessing the validity of animal models of psychiatric disorders remains a difficult task. This reflects both a limited understanding of the pathophysiology of these conditions and a paucity of reliable biological correlates for observed symptoms. For affective disorders in particular, there exist only a few biological correlates with (limited) research utility, such as aberrant REM sleep patterns and decreased autonomic variability in patients with depression (Stein et al., Reference Stein, Carney, Freedland, Skala, Jaffe, Kleiger and Rottman2000; Palagini et al., Reference Palagini, Baglioni, Ciapparelli, Gemignani and Riemann2013) and reduced suppression of cortisol secretion by dexamethasone in those with melancholic depression (Carroll, Reference Carroll1981). Moreover, the presence of such markers is, in general, neither pathognomonic nor common to more than a subset of patients (Nierenberg & Feinstein, Reference Nierenberg and Feinstein1988; Arfken et al., Reference Arfken, Joseph, Sandhu, Roehrs, Douglass and Boutros2014; García-Gutiérrez et al., Reference García-Gutiérrez, Navarrete, Sala, Gasparyan, Austrich-Olivares and Manzanares2020). Behavioural responses to pharmacological interventions thus remain the primary means to assess the validity of putative animal models of affective disorders. Ideally then, substances that are anxiolytic in man should register as anxiolytic-like in an animal model, antidepressants should register as antidepressant-like and so on. For example, while long-term administration of a serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SRI) is an effective treatment for anxiety disorders (Bandelow & Michaelis, Reference Bandelow and Michaelis2015), they do not provide immediate relief in the manner of benzodiazepines – if anything, they increase anxiety after the first dose(s) (Sinclair et al., Reference Sinclair, Christmas, Hood, Potokar, Robertson, Isaac, Srivastava, Nutt and Davies2009; Näslund et al., Reference Näslund, Hieronymus, Emilsson, Lisinski, Nilsson and Eriksson2017), with a therapeutic effect beginning to emerge after roughly a week of treatment (Hieronymus et al., Reference Hieronymus, Nilsson and Eriksson2016). This difference between the acute and chronic effects of SRIs is largely mirrored by rodent models of anxiety-like behaviour (see, e.g. Mombereu and co-workers for a more thorough discussion [Mombereau et al., Reference Mombereau, Gur, Onksen and Blendy2010]).

The zebrafish has seen increased use as a model organism in pre-clinical psychiatric research, but some outstanding questions remain regarding how well findings in zebrafish translate to mammalian models and to the clinical situation and vice versa. The overarching goal of the studies reported in this paper has been to investigate one such issue where discrepancies seem to be present, namely that of behavioural responses to acute administration of serotonin-elevating agents. Studies employing two of the major models of anxiety-like behaviour in zebrafish – the novel-tank diving (geotaxis) test and the light-dark (scototaxis) test – have reported somewhat conflicting results, especially for the latter, with both increased (Magno et al., Reference Magno, Fontes, Gonçalves and Gouveia2015) and decreased (Benneh et al., Reference Benneh, Biney, Mante, Tandoh, Adongo and Woode2017; Giacomini et al., Reference Giacomini, Abreu, v., Siebel, Zimerman, Rambo, Mocelin, Bonan, Piato and Barcellos2016, Reference Giacomini, Piassetta, Genario, Bonan, Piato, Barcellos and de Abreu2020) anxiety-like behaviour being observed after acute SRI administration. Since serotonin is an important mediator of sex differences in rodents as well as in humans (for further discussion and references on this subject, we refer to earlier work by us [Näslund et al., Reference Näslund, Studer, Nilsson, Westberg and Eriksson2013]), it is conceivable that some of these discrepancies could be related to differential SRI responses in males and females as most zebrafish studies on the acute effects of various SRIs have used mixed-sex groups. Another factor that possibly could influence results is habituation to the apparatus; at least in rodent models, it is well-known that prior exposure to tests aiming to measure anxiety-like behaviour generally reduces such behaviour upon re-test (File, Reference File1993).

This two-part study thus aims to systematise what is known about, as well as further investigate, the impact of acute serotonergic interventions in zebrafish tests of anxiety-like behaviour. The first part is a systematic review providing an up-to-date summary of results from previous studies using the novel-tank diving and light-dark tests. These findings are then analysed in relation to data obtained from the second part of the study; a series of experiments in which we investigate the effects of administration of serotonin-elevating agents on behaviour in the scototaxis test, while attempting to control for the influence of sex, mode of action and habituation to the apparatus.

A central focus of the research group is to actively work with 3R practices, and as we desired to be able to re-use the animals in the main experiments (Experiments III–V), we performed a pilot study (Experiment I) investigating whether earlier acute administration of an SRI influences anxiety-like behaviour of zebrafish in the scototaxis test, both in the absence and presence of an SRI. In Experiments I, III and IV, we employed a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), escitalopram. This was chosen as it is the most selective of all the SRIs in clinical use (i.e. possessing the least affinity for targets other than the serotonin transporter) (Sánchez & Hyttel, Reference Sánchez and Hyttel1999), while also having a half-life of little more than a day (Søgaard et al., Reference Søgaard, Mengel, Rao and Larsen2005), which can be contrasted to the SSRI most commonly used in zebrafish studies, fluoxetine, where the active metabolite has a half-life of up to 2 weeks (Preskorn, Reference Preskorn1997) (after repeated administration – but whether zebrafish metabolise as quickly as humans is unknown) – such a long half-life could conceivably have long-term effects that impact fish health and behaviour in an unforeseen manner. In Experiment IV, we attempted to replicate the findings from Experiment III while allowing for a possible effect of habituation to be observed. In Experiment V, the serotonin-releasing agent fenfluramine, anxiogenic in man and anxiogenic-like in rodents (File & Guardiola-Lemaitre, Reference File and Guardiola-Lemaitre1988; Targum & Marshall, Reference Targum and Marshall1989), was employed (a dose-finding study for fenfluramine, Experiment II, had also been performed). This was partly done to control for the possibility that the observed effects in Experiments III and IV had been related to drug-specific effects (i.e. escitalopram having effects on targets other than the serotonin transporter, such as the σ receptor (Albayrak & Hashimoto, Reference Albayrak and Hashimoto2017)) and partly to control for the possibility that a class-specific effect unique to zebrafish could explain the reported discrepancies in terms of behavioural effects of acute SSRI administration and anxiety-like behaviour as compared to humans and other mammals.

In short, our main aims were to (i) systematically summarise previous work regarding acute effects of serotonin-releasing agents in the most common zebrafish models of anxiety, (ii) investigate to what extent administration of serotonin-releasing agents exerts an anxiolytic-like or anxiogenic-like effect in the light-dark test, (iii) investigate if any observed effects are consistent across different classes or serotonin-elevating agents and (iv) investigate if sex, or the novelty of the situation, can influence a possible treatment response.

Experimental procedures

Literature search and systematic review process

A literature search was performed using PubMed/Medline, Web of Science and Google Scholar between December 01, 2021, and March 10, 2022, and repeated between June 1 and August 9, 2023 (Figure 1). Records published between 1945 and 2023 were included. For PubMed, the ‘Advanced search’ of titles and abstracts was used, for Web of Science, the ‘Abstract’ advanced search function was used and for Google Scholar, the ‘allintitle’ function. The search string was ([’zebrafish’or ‘danio rerio’ or ‘danio’] and [’citalopram’ or ‘escitalopram’ or ’sertraline’ or ‘zimelidine’ or ‘paroxetine’ or ‘fluoxetine’ or ‘fluvoxamine’ or ‘fenfluramine’ or ‘duloxetine’ or ‘venlafaxine’ or ‘fenfluramine’ or ’SRI’ or ’SSRI’ or ‘tricyclic’ or ‘amitriptyline’ or ‘imipramine’ or ‘clomipramine’ or ‘desipramine’]). Only studies employing adult zebrafish were considered. Regarding the inclusion of desipramine: note that it, in zebrafish, unlike in mammals, has a significant effect also on serotonin reuptake (Severinsen et al., Reference Severinsen, Sinning, Müller and Wiborg2008). Papers were assessed for eligibility by two independent reviewers, and subsequent quality assessment was performed using the ‘SYRCLE Risk of Bias Tool for Animal Studies’ (Hooijmans et al., Reference Hooijmans, Rovers, de Vries, Leenaars, Ritskes-Hoitinga and Langendam2014); here each paper is evaluated regarding risk of bias across 10 items, assigning a rating of high, low or unclear risk for each item. Any disagreement about a specific score was resolved through discussion and sometimes by consultation with a third reviewer.

Figure 1. A ‘Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-analyses’ (PRISMA) flowchart illustrating the literature search process (A). Cumulative bar charts displaying risk of bias assessment across 10 items specified in SYRCLE’s assessment tool (B); due to the limited methodological descriptions of most articles, it was not possible to confidently classify any work as clearly not conforming to SYRCLE guidelines. Pie charts illustrating the distribution of relevant categories of sex (C) and substance used (D) in comparisons included in the systematic review. Histogram over number of identified studies published by year since 2010 (E). For items B–E, data are shown separately for the light-dark (LD, left) and novel-tank diving test (NTD, right).

Animals

Male and female zebrafish of the wild-type AB line were used, having previously been procured from the Genome Editing Zebrafish facility, SciLifeLab, Uppsala University (Uppsala, Sweden) to the animal facility of the Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences at the University of Gothenburg. Animals in Experiment I were aged 12–18 months at the start of the experiment, while animals in Experiment II were aged 13–21 months. Animals used in Experiments III–V were aged 8–9 months at the start of Experiment III, before which males and females had been put into separate aquaria and left undisturbed for 4 weeks. Animals were housed in 10 l aquaria with continuous filtering, at a density of roughly 1 animal/l, under controlled conditions (conductivity 900 ± 10 µS, pH 7.4 ± 1, temperature of 27–28 °C) and a 14:10 h photoperiod with a gradual onset of lights at 07:45–08:00 a.m. and dimming of lights at 09:45–10:00 p.m.). Care was taken to ensure that all aquaria had similar surroundings and light levels and that they were equally likely to be disturbed during work in the housing room. Aquaria water was produced from Tropic Marine Pro-Reef Salt (Bioted Marine, Kungsbacka, Sweden) adjusted with bicarbonate. The aquaria water was changed twice per week and water quality was checked three times per week using test strips. The environment was enriched with one large and bushy artificial plant per aquarium, as well as images of stones below the aquaria. Fish were fed twice per day with a mixture of Tetra Pro Multi-Crisps flake food (Tetra Fish, Melle, Germany), granular pellets (ZM Fish foods, Winchester, UK) and hatched brine shrimp nauplii.

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant European, national and institutional guidelines on the care and use of laboratory animals. All experimental procedures and animal husbandry protocols also followed the Animal Research: Reporting of In Vivo Experiments (ARRIVE) guidelines and had been approved by the Gothenburg Animal Research Ethics Committee (case numbers 157-2015 and 5.8.18-08496/2018).

Apparatus

Prior to testing in the light-dark apparatus and an hour before the start of any drug treatment, fish were moved from the housing room to the experiment room. Both rooms were lit by fluorescent lights, providing an illumination of roughly 1.5 mW/mm2 at the home aquaria, as well as at the bench where experiments were conducted. The light-dark test apparatus was similar to that of Maximino and co-workers (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Marques de Brito, de Dias C.A.G., Gouveia and Morato2010) and consisted of a half-black, half-white box (15 × 10 × 45 cm), with a 5 cm wide central compartment bound by sliding doors in which the animal was placed at test start. The apparatus was filled with fresh aquaria water, 10 cm deep. Fish were then incubated individually in 500 ml semi-translucent beakers spaced roughly 30 cm apart, filled with 250 ml fresh aquaria water or test solution (drug dissolved in fresh aquaria water) for 10 min. After drug incubation, the fish were netted into the central, closed holding chamber where they remained for 10 min before the sliding doors were removed, allowing the animal to explore the whole apparatus. They were then filmed for 15 min before being returned to the home aquaria. The apparatus was filled with fresh aquaria water of the same characteristics and temperature as described above, with water being changed after every animal. In order to assess if re-testing of animals in Experiments III–V would be likely to influence behaviour in subsequent tests, Experiment I was carried out to investigate whether acute treatment with an SSRI had any influence on anxiety-like behaviour when animals were re-exposed to the same drug 6 weeks later. No such effect was seen, but as an additional precaution, comparatively long intervals between Experiments III–V were chosen; 2 months separated Experiments III and IV, while Experiment V was conducted 4 months after Experiment IV. The shorter interval between Experiments III and IV was chosen to allow a possible habituation to the apparatus to be detected, but also this shorter interval was longer than that in Experiment I and is comparable to drug wash-out times in imaging studies in humans, where subjects often are their own controls In order to assess if re-testing of animals in Experiments III–V would be likely to influence behaviour in subsequent tests, Experiment I was carried out to investigate whether acute treatment with an SSRI had any influence on anxiety-like behaviour when animals were re-exposed to the same drug 6 weeks later. No such effect was seen, but as an additional precaution, comparatively long intervals between Experiments III–V were chosen; 2 months separated Experiments III and IV while Experiment V was conducted 4 months after Experiment IV. The shorter interval between Experiments III and IV was chosen to allow a possible habituation to the apparatus to be detected, but also this shorter interval was longer than that in Experiment I and is comparable to drug wash-out times in imaging studies in humans, where subjects often are their own controls (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Sacher, Geiss-Granadia, Attarbaschi, Mossaheb, Lanzenberger, Pötzi, Holik, Spindelegger, Asenbaum, Dudczak, Tauscher and Kasper2006; Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Mueller, Zsido, Reinelt, Regenthal, Okon-Singer, Forbes, Villringer and Sacher2021).

Drugs

Escitalopram oxalate (Shodana Labs, Hyderabad, India) and d-fenfluramine hydrochloride (Tocris, Bristol, United Kingdom) were dissolved as a stock solution of fresh aquaria water before diluting them further in the working solutions. The escitalopram dose (50 mg/l, i.e. 154.09 µM) was selected from the published literature for the racemate, that is, citalopram (note that the R enantiomer of citalopram is practically inert; S-citalopram (i.e. escitalopram) is active) (Tables 1 and 2). We chose a slightly longer immersion time (10 min vs. 3–5 min in most published studies) as Sackerman and co-workers (Sackerman et al., Reference Sackerman, Donegan, Cunningham, Nguyen, Lawless, Long, Benno and Gould2010) reported comparatively low brain citalopram levels (about a third of the levels seen during treatment with standard doses in rodents) after 3 min of immersion in 100 mg/l of citalopram (i.e. equivalent to 50 mg/l of escitalopram). We chose to include a lower dose of escitalopram in Experiment III than that used in Experiment I as we in an unrelated experiment had found no differences between 25 and 50 mg/l in terms of anxiolytic-like effect and wanted to confirm this, as the possible use of a lower dose was considered desirable from an animal health perspective.

Table 1. Reported effects of acute SRI administration in the novel-tank diving, or geotaxis, test using adult zebrafish

Given are data pertaining to the drugs investigated (substances, doses, modes of administration), various important intervals (time allowed for drugs to work, habituation times and total test times), sex and strain. The following indices of anxiety-like behaviour are given: effects on (i) total time spent in the upper region (variously upper 1/3 or upper 1/2) – an increase here would usually be interpreted as an anxiolytic-like effect – and (ii) latency to first visit to the upper region; here a decrease is generally seen as an anxiolytic-like effect. ↑ indicates a significant increase, ↓ a significant decrease, → no significant differences and / that the parameter in question was not reported. The mode of administration was generally immersion and exceptions are indicated by either i.p. (intraperitoneal injection) or p.o. (peroral gavage). M + F indicates mixed-sex groups, ? that sex was not specified, M/F that males and females were tested separately and M or F that only males or females, respectively, were employed. A question mark under the ‘Strain’ heading indicates that no specification was given while WT (?) indicates that animals were described as wild-type with no further determination. In the paper by Sinyakova et al., strain was specified as ‘NTL’ with no further clarification.

Table 2. Reported effects of acute SRI administration in the scototaxis test using adult zebrafish

Given are data pertaining to the drugs investigated (substances, doses, modes of administration), various important intervals (time allowed for drugs to work, habituation times and total test times), sex and strain. The following indices of anxiety-like behaviour are given: i) total time spent in the white compartment - an increase here would usually be interpreted as an anxiolytic-like effect - and ii) latency to first entry to the white compartment; here a decrease is generally interpreted as an anxiolytic-like effect. ↑ indicates a significant increase, ↓ a significant decrease, → no significant differences and / that the parameter in question was not reported. In general, drugs were administered through immersion, though some studies employed intraperitoneal injection, as indicated by the abbreviation i.p. M+F indicates mixed-sex groups, ? that sex was not specified, M/F that males and females were tested separately and M or F that only males or females, respectively, were employed. A question mark under the ‘Strain’ heading indicates that no specification was given while WT (?) indicates that animals were described as wild-type with no further determination.

We found no previously published behavioural studies regarding fenfluramine and employing adult zebrafish. Therefore, a dose-finding study was conducted, with investigated concentrations (0.5 mg/l, 5 mg/l and 10 mg/l, i.e. 2.16 µM, 21.62 µM and 43.24 µM) being chosen on the basis of experiments in larval zebrafish (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Kecskés, Copmans, Langlois, Crawford, Ceulemans, Lagae, de Witte and Esguerra2015), rodents (Laferrere & Wurtman, Reference Laferrere and Wurtman1989) and trout (Ruibal et al., Reference Ruibal, Soengas and Aldegunde2002).

We did not observe any abnormal behaviour or any indications of negative effects on fish health during, or after, any of the experiments.

Experimental outline

Experiment I: 27 animals (mixed sex, equal proportions of males and females) were randomised to immersion in either escitalopram in solution (50 mg/l) or fresh aquaria water for 10 min and then kept in separate aquaria according to the treatment group. Six weeks later, zebrafish were again treated according to the protocol above, with half of those in the control group now receiving escitalopram and vice versa. Animals were then subjected to a light-dark test.

Experiment II: 32 males and 12 females were randomised to immersion in either fenfluramine in solution (0.5 mg/l, 5 mg/l or 10 mg/l) or fresh aquaria water for 10 min. Animals were then subjected to a light-dark test.

Experiment III: 62 males and 37 females were randomised to immersion in either escitalopram in solution (25 mg/l or 50 mg/l) or fresh aquaria water, for 10 min. Animals were then subjected to a light-dark test.

Experiment IV: 60 males and 34 females were randomised to immersion in either escitalopram in solution (25 mg/l) or fresh aquaria water, for 10 min. Animals were then subjected to a light-dark test.

Experiment V: 52 males and 32 females were randomised to immersion in either fenfluramine in solution (5 mg/l) or fresh aquaria water, for 10 min. Animals were then subjected to a light-dark test.

Randomisation was done in a stratified manner by the first successfully netted animal from the first aquarium being assigned to treatment group A (actual treatment having been assigned by a third party and thus being unknown to the investigator), the second to treatment group B, the third to treatment group C (if three groups were included, otherwise to group A, etc. In the second aquarium, the order was reversed; in the third, the order was as in the first and so on. This was done to reduce the risk of baseline temperament factors being related to how easily an animal is netted (e.g. boldness) becoming unequally distributed among treatment groups, as well as ensuring that any factors relating to housing/aquaria position were equalised among treatment groups. Experimenters and caregivers were blinded to treatment status.

Data analysis

Movies of each 15-min experimental session were manually analysed by an experimenter blind to the treatment condition of the animal. The following behavioural variables were assessed in each video: latency (s) to first entry to the white chamber, number of crossings between white and black chambers and total time (s) spent in the white chamber. These variables had been pre-specified. Apart from latency and total time, which had been commonly reported in earlier studies, we also included total entries to the light area, as reporting of this variable has been standard since the rodent light-dark box (i.e. the ancestor of the scototaxis test in zebrafish) was first developed (Costall et al., Reference Costall, Jones, Kelly, Naylor and Tomkins1989). Due to a camera malfunction, the recordings of eight males (six escitalopram and two control) from Experiment III were not available for analysis.

Statistical procedures

For analysis of behavioural parameters, two-way ANOVAs were employed, the exception being Experiment II where a one-way ANOVA with subsequent t-tests for relevant comparisons was used; as the variables were highly correlated, no correction for multiple comparisons was done. In Experiment I, treatment status at the first and the second sessions was set to be the predictive variable, while for Experiment III–IV, treatment and sex were employed. All analyses were done in SPSS for Mac, version 21 (IBM, Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

Systematic review

The search yielded 308 unique articles in total (304 in PubMed and a further 4 through Web of Science and Google Scholar). All articles found were written in English. All abstracts were then screened and reports clearly irrelevant to the research question were excluded, leaving 87 articles. The full texts of the remaining papers, having been identified as potentially relevant, were screened with a total of 28 papers being included. 24 papers had employed the novel-tank diving test (Benneh et al., Reference Benneh, Biney, Mante, Tandoh, Adongo and Woode2017; Clément et al., Reference Clément, Macrì and Porfiri2020; Alves et al., Reference Alves, Tamagno, Pompermaier, Vanin and Barcellos2023; Costa Iturriaga Vásquez et al., Reference Iturriaga Vásquez, Osorio and Herzog2012; Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Gemaque, Benzecry, Lima, Batista, Picanço-Diniz, Oliveira and Herculano2015; Demin et al., Reference Demin, Kolesnikova, Khatsko, Meshalkina, Efimova, YYu and Kalueff2017; dos Santos Sampaio et al., Reference dos Santos Sampaio, de Melo, de Freitas Paiva, da Silva Aleluia, da Silva Neto, da Silva, Keita, Cruz, Sánchez-Ortiz, Pineda-Peña, Balderas, Navarrete and Carvalho2018; de Melo et al., Reference de Melo, Sánchez-Ortiz, dos Santos Sampaio, Matias Pereira, da Silva Neto FL, da Silva, Alves Soares Cruz, Keita, Soares Pereira and Tavares Carvalho2019; Macrì et al., Reference Macrì, Clément, Spinello and Porfiri2019; Giacomini et al., Reference Giacomini, Piassetta, Genario, Bonan, Piato, Barcellos and de Abreu2020; Lima-Maximino et al., Reference Lima-Maximino, Pyterson, do Carmo Silva, Gomes, Rocha, Herculano, Rosemberg and Maximino2020; Karakaya et al., Reference Karakaya, Scaramuzzi, Macrì and Porfiri2021; Kulikova et al., Reference Kulikova, Bazovkina, Evsyukova and Kulikov2021; Malyshev et al., Reference Malyshev, Sukhanova, Zlobin, Gedzun, Pavshintsev, Vasileva, Zalevsky, Doronin, Mitkin, Golovin, Lovat, Kovalev, Zolotarev, Kuchumov, Babkin and Luscher2021; Fontana et al., Reference Fontana, Alnassar and Parker2022; Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Gemaque, Benzecry, Lima, Batista, Picanço-Diniz, Oliveira and Herculano2015, Reference Maximino, Lima, Costa, Guedes and Herculano2013b, Reference Maximino, Marques de Brito, de Dias C.A.G., Gouveia and Morato2013a; Sackerman et al., Reference Sackerman, Donegan, Cunningham, Nguyen, Lawless, Long, Benno and Gould2010; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Wu, Cachat, Hart, Gaikwad, Wong, Utterback, Gilder, Kyzar, Newman, Carlos, Chang, Hook, Rhymes, Caffery, Greenberg, Zadina and Kalueff2011; Sinyakova et al., Reference Sinyakova, Kulikova, Englevskii and Kulikov2018; Sabadin et al., Reference Stewart, Cachat, Gaikwad, Robinson, Gebhardt and Kalueff2013, Reference Sabadin, Biasuz, Canzian, Adedara and Rosemberg2022) and 12 the light-dark test (Magno et al., Reference Magno, Fontes, Gonçalves and Gouveia2015; Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Gemaque, Benzecry, Lima, Batista, Picanço-Diniz, Oliveira and Herculano2015; Benneh et al., Reference Benneh, Biney, Mante, Tandoh, Adongo and Woode2017; Malyshev et al., Reference Malyshev, Sukhanova, Zlobin, Gedzun, Pavshintsev, Vasileva, Zalevsky, Doronin, Mitkin, Golovin, Lovat, Kovalev, Zolotarev, Kuchumov, Babkin and Luscher2021; Fontana et al., Reference Maximino, Lima, Costa, Guedes and Herculano2014, Reference Fontana, Alnassar and Parker2022, Reference Fontana, Alnassar and Parker2013b, Reference Fontana, Alnassar and Parker2013a, Reference Fontana, Alnassar and Parker2011; Sackerman et al., Reference Sackerman, Donegan, Cunningham, Nguyen, Lawless, Long, Benno and Gould2010; Singer et al., Reference Singer, Oreschak, Rhinehart and Robison2016; Sabadin et al., Reference Sabadin, Biasuz, Canzian, Adedara and Rosemberg2022); some used both. The papers are summarised in Tables 1 and 2, Supplementary Table 1 and Figure 1. Regarding the SYRCLE analysis, we found most papers to have adequately controlled for baseline factors, as well as having addressed missing data in a satisfactory manner while not appearing to selectively report outcomes. Most studies employing the light-dark test also reported the outcome assessor to having been blind to treatment status. However, we found most papers to disclose insufficient details to permit the assessment of most items. We deemed the literature available for the novel-tank diving test to be comprehensive, and there is little need for any further experiments regarding the general influence of SSRIs in this paradigm. However, considerably less work had been done on anxiety-like behaviour in the light-dark test, despite some clear advantages compared to the novel-tank diving test, such as an apparent lack of risk of autonomic responses confounding any ethopharmacological effects (Finney et al., Reference Finney, Robertson, McGee, Smith and Croll2006; Sackerman et al., Reference Sackerman, Donegan, Cunningham, Nguyen, Lawless, Long, Benno and Gould2010). That no clear picture of the effects of SRI administration in the test emerged was then somewhat puzzling, although we did note that studies employing the test generally had used both low doses as well as very few SSRIs other than fluoxetine (with one exception (Sackerman et al., Reference Sackerman, Donegan, Cunningham, Nguyen, Lawless, Long, Benno and Gould2010)). All in all, there seemed to be enough outstanding questions to warrant further experiments.

Experiment I: No effects of previous single-dose exposure to escitalopram were seen on any of the indices investigated, and there were no treatment × treatment interactions. A significant, anxiolytic-like main effect for escitalopram treatment at session 2 on time spent in the light compartment was present (Figure 2).

Figure 2. Experiment I: effects of escitalopram on latency to first entry into the white compartment (A), number of entries into the white compartment (B) and time spent in the white compartment (C) in the light-dark test. The test duration was 900 s. Given in the figure are the results of a two-way ANOVA with treatment status 6 weeks before and treatment status on the day of the light-dark session, respectively, as predictive variables. Error bars indicate SEM. Group sizes: n = 6 for (CTRL-CTRL), otherwise 7. ESC = escitalopram, CTRL = control.

Experiment II: A pattern suggesting a dose-dependent anxiolytic-like effect was observed albeit that a significant difference was only observed between the 5 mg group and the control animals in terms of the total number of entries into the light compartment (Figure 3). As we cannot discount the possibility that the reduction in the total number of entries at 10 mg/l as compared to 5 mg/l is related to effects on, for example, locomotion, the lower dose appears to be the better choice.

Figure 3. Experiment II: effects of fenfluramine (FEN) on latency to first entry into the white compartment (A), number of entries into the white compartment (B) and time spent in the white compartment (C) in the light-dark test. The test duration was 900 s. Indicated in the figure are p-values representing comparisons made to the control group. Error bars indicate SEM. Group sizes: n = 10 for FEN 0.5 mg/ml, otherwise 11. CTRL = control, n.s. = non-significant, * = p < 0.05.

Experiment III: No differences on any behavioural indices were observed between the two dose groups, and they were therefore collapsed into a single escitalopram group in all further analyses (Figure 4, panel 1). Significant main effects were seen for SSRI treatment on time spent in the white compartment (Figure 4, panel 1C), with animals receiving escitalopram spending more time in the light compartment. No effects on the other parameters recorded were observed, no sex effects were noted, and there were no significant interactions.

Figure 4. Experiments III–V: effects of escitalopram (ESC) (Experiments III and IV) and fenfluramine (FEN) (Experiment V) on latency to first entry into the white compartment (A), number of entries into the white compartment (B) and time spent in the white compartment (C) in the light-dark test. The test duration was 900 s. Given in the figure are the results of two-way ANOVAs with treatment and sex as predictive variables. Error bars indicate SEM. CTRL = control. Group sizes: n = 18 (males, control), 34 (males, ESC) 13 (females, control) and 26 (females, ESC) in Experiment III, 30 (males) and 17 (females) in Experiment IV and 26 (males) and 16 (females) in Experiment V.

Experiment IV: As in Experiment III, a significant treatment effect on time spent in the white compartment was observed (Figure 4, panel 2C). A sex effect was present for all three recorded indices of anxiety-like behaviour, with males displaying less anxiety-like behaviour. There were no significant interactions (Figure 4, panel 2A-C).

Experiment V: Again, a treatment effect on time spent in the white compartment was present, with animals having received fenfluramine spending more time there (Figure 4, panel 3C). As in Experiment IV, a sex effect was observed, although only for number of entries to the white compartment. As in the earlier experiments, no significant interactions were observed (Figure 4, panel 3B).

Discussion

Recalling the aims of the study as stated in the introduction, we would argue that our results (i) support the notion that acute administration of serotonin-releasing agents exerts an anxiolytic-like influence in the light-dark test in zebrafish and (ii) that this is not unique to reuptake inhibitors and (iii) that neither sex nor earlier exposure to the apparatus has any appreciable influence on treatment effects while (iv) earlier work strongly supports the existence of an anxiolytic-like effect of agents that increase synaptic serotonin in the novel-tank diving test while being more equivocal regarding the light-dark test.

Taken together, our findings would then indicate that the general effect of acute serotonin elevation, irrespective of whether this is achieved through blockage (escitalopram) or reversal (fenfluramine) of the serotonin transporter is anxiolytic, rather than anxiogenic, in zebrafish, as assayed by both the scototaxis and geotaxis tests. This is the opposite of the situation not only in humans (Grillon et al., Reference Grillon, Levenson and Pine2007; Sinclair et al., Reference Sinclair, Christmas, Hood, Potokar, Robertson, Isaac, Srivastava, Nutt and Davies2009; Näslund et al., Reference Näslund, Hieronymus, Emilsson, Lisinski, Nilsson and Eriksson2017), other mammals (Griebel et al., Reference Griebel, Moreau, Jenck, Misslin and Martin1994) and birds (Warnick et al., Reference Warnick, Huang, Acevedo and Sufka2009) but also in more distantly related metazoans such as crayfish (Fossat et al., Reference Fossat, Bacqué-Cazenave, De Deurwaerdère, Delbecque and Cattaert2014) (but note the anxiolytic-like effect in shore crabs [Hamilton et al., Reference Hamilton, Kwan, Gallup and Tresguerres2016]).

The neurobiology underlying a heightened capacity for anxiety/anxiety-like behaviour in humans and other mammals is only partly known, as are the long-term neurobiological effects of SRI administration beyond the immediate effects at the serotonergic synapse. It can nevertheless be noted that imaging studies indicate the 5-HT system in patients with anxiety disorders to be overactive (Frick et al., Reference Frick, Åhs, Engman, Jonasson, Alaie, Björkstrand, Frans, Faria, Linnman, Appel, Wahlstedt, Lubberink, Fredrikson and Furmark2015) and that this can be attenuated by long-term SSRI treatment (Frick et al., Reference Frick, Åhs, Appel, Jonasson, Wahlstedt, Bani, Merlo Pich, Bettica, Långström, Lubberink, Fredrikson and Furmark2016). Similarly, rodents displaying high anxiety-like behaviour have increased levels of the enzyme catalysing the rate-limiting step in serotonin synthesis, tryptophan hydroxylase 2 (TPH2), in the raphe nuclei, where serotonergic cell bodies reside, as well as higher serotonin levels in the amygdala as compared to less ‘anxious’ compatriots (Näslund et al., Reference Näslund, Studer, Pettersson, Hagsäter, Nilsson, Nissbrandt and Eriksson2015). Finally, interventions that render rodents more ‘anxious’ also tend to raise raphe TPH2 levels (Chamas et al., Reference Chamas, Serova and Sabban1999; Gardner et al., Reference Gardner, Hale, Lightman, Plotsky and Lowry2009; Sidor et al., Reference Sidor, Amath, MacQueen and Foster2010).

While differences, as compared to mammals, in ethopharmacological responses to SRIs conceivably could be related to the drugs having a partly different mode of action in teleosts, this does not seem to be the case (Winberg & Thörnqvist, Reference Winberg and Thörnqvist2016); acute SSRI treatment appears to increase serotonin turnover also in zebrafish. To our knowledge, no studies directly investigating the effects of fenfluramine on serotonin turnover in cyprinid brains, as measured by, for example, microdialysis, have been conducted. Nevertheless, it has been demonstrated to be a potent serotonin-releasing agent in mammals (Laferrere & Wurtman, Reference Laferrere and Wurtman1989; Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Hernandez and Hoebel1989), as well as in rainbow trout (Ruibal et al., Reference Ruibal, Soengas and Aldegunde2002).

When considering our results in relation to previously conducted studies identified in the systematic review (summarised in Tables 1 and 2), there is a clear agreement with results obtained from studies employing the novel-tank diving/geotaxis test (Table 1). Only one paper (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Puty, Matos Oliveira and Herculano2013b) reports an anxiogenic-like effect of acute SSRI administration and then only for one of several strains investigated. The addition of, for example, chronic unpredictable stress beforehand, or the presence of robotic predators or conspecifics does not seem to change this general picture (Clément et al., Reference Clément, Macrì and Porfiri2020; Costa de Melo et al., Reference de Melo, Sánchez-Ortiz, dos Santos Sampaio, Matias Pereira, da Silva Neto FL, da Silva, Alves Soares Cruz, Keita, Soares Pereira and Tavares Carvalho2019; Karakaya et al., Reference Karakaya, Scaramuzzi, Macrì and Porfiri2021).

It has been suggested that the observed anxiolytic-like effect of high (e.g. 100 mg/l of citalopram) doses of SSRIs in the novel-tank diving test partly reflects an effect on the swim bladder, unrelated to any effects related to affective behaviour (Finney et al., Reference Finney, Robertson, McGee, Smith and Croll2006; Sackerman et al., Reference Sackerman, Donegan, Cunningham, Nguyen, Lawless, Long, Benno and Gould2010), and while such an influence cannot be discounted without further investigation, similar behavioural responses are, as mentioned above, also seen at lower doses. It can be noted that in order to achieve brain tissue concentrations comparable to those in rodents and humans given standard doses of at least citalopram/escitalopram, comparatively high doses seem to be necessary; Sackerman and co-workers (Sackerman et al., Reference Sackerman, Donegan, Cunningham, Nguyen, Lawless, Long, Benno and Gould2010) estimate a citalopram concentration of 0.116 mg/kg in the zebrafish brain after a 3 min immersion of 100 mg/l citalopram in water. This can be compared to a study in mice reporting brain tissue escitalopram levels of 1121 nmol/l (0.363 mg/kg) 1 h after the administration of 5 mg/kg (Karlsson et al., Reference Karlsson, Carlsson, Hiemke, Ahlner, Bengtsson, Schmitt and Kugelberg2013) or to post-mortem levels of 1.5–4 mg/kg in patients treated with citalopram and whose deaths were deemed to be unrelated to SSRI treatment (Nedahl et al., Reference Nedahl, Johansen and Linnet2018). It may very well be the case that SSRI doses previously used in the light-dark test have been somewhat low, to the extent that comparisons can be made to rodent models and the clinical situation. In the case of fenfluramine, the maximum dose tested in Experiment I (10 mg/l) was comparable to that used when the drug had been tested as an antiseizure agent in larval models of Dravet syndrome (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Kecskés, Copmans, Langlois, Crawford, Ceulemans, Lagae, de Witte and Esguerra2015); we found no earlier studies investigating the effects of fenfluramine on behaviour in adult zebrafish and only one in fish at all (Weischer, Reference Weischer1966).

The light-dark/scototaxis test data is sparser and also more ambiguous (Table 2). It has been suggested that there exists a real discrepancy between the impact of acute SRI administration in the scototaxis and geotaxis tests and that this is related to a differential influence of serotonin on anxiety and fear/panic (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Puty, Benzecry, Araújo, Lima, de Jesus Oliveira Batista, Renata De Matos Oliveira, Crespo-Lopez and Herculano2013a), in line with the Deakin–Graeff hypothesis; that is, that different subpopulations of serotonergic neurones exert differential effects on fear (or panic) and anxiety, respectively, with increased serotonergic tone inhibiting the former but promoting the latter (Paul et al., Reference Paul, Johnson, Shekhar and Lowry2014).

In our opinion, it is however not immediately clear from an ethological and ethopharmacological point of view why one, but not the other, of the two tests should measure fear/panic rather than anxiety-like behaviour. It can be noted that while azapirones such as buspirone have some use in treating generalised anxiety disorder (Chessick et al., Reference Chessick, Allen, Thase, Batista Miralha da Cunha, Kapczinski, de Lima and dos Santos Souza2006) they are generally ineffective for panic disorder (Imai et al., Reference Imai, Tajika, Chen, Pompoli, Guaiana, Castellazzi, Bighelli, Girlanda, Barbui, Koesters, Cipriani and Furukawa2014), a state of affairs that appears to be mirrored by rodent models (Graeff et al., Reference Graeff, Guimarães, De Andrade and Deakin1996). In zebrafish they are effective in reducing anxiety-like behaviour in the scototaxis (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, da Silva, Gouveia and Herculano2011, Reference Maximino, Puty, Benzecry, Araújo, Lima, de Jesus Oliveira Batista, Renata De Matos Oliveira, Crespo-Lopez and Herculano2013a), as well as in the geotaxis test (Bencan et al., Reference Bencan, Sledge and Levin2009; Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Puty, Benzecry, Araújo, Lima, de Jesus Oliveira Batista, Renata De Matos Oliveira, Crespo-Lopez and Herculano2013a), providing some support for both models to be regarded as primarily reflecting anxiety, rather than fear or panic, if one accepts the Deakin–Graeff hypothesis. Moreover, if the general influence of an acute increase of synaptic 5-HT levels in the scototaxis test would be anxiogenic, then a dose-response relationship could be expected for SRIs, but studies reporting such an effect of SRI administration find it to be transient and observed at only low or intermediate doses (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Puty, Benzecry, Araújo, Lima, de Jesus Oliveira Batista, Renata De Matos Oliveira, Crespo-Lopez and Herculano2013a; Magno et al., Reference Magno, Fontes, Gonçalves and Gouveia2015). Indeed, studies employing higher doses in the scototaxis test either report anxiolysis or trends theretoward (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Puty, Matos Oliveira and Herculano2013b; Sackerman et al., Reference Sackerman, Donegan, Cunningham, Nguyen, Lawless, Long, Benno and Gould2010).

Finally, the fact that it is fluoxetine in low doses that has been employed in all studies reporting an anxiogenic-like effect of acute SRI administration in the scototaxis test may be of some relevance, as low-dose fluoxetine has been reported to have minor effects on serotonergic neurotransmission in rodents while increasing levels of the GABAA-modulating neurosteroid allopregnanolone (Pinna et al., Reference Pinna, Costa and Guidotti2009). It is thus conceivable that an action on the neurosteroid system that is particular to this SSRI partly could explain these discrepancies. Also, while the clinical profiles of the SSRIs are generally very similar, the fact that fluoxetine is the least selective, in terms of monoamine reuptake inhibition, of the SSRIs (Sánchez & Hyttel, Reference Sánchez and Hyttel1999), could possibly have a greater importance in zebrafish than in humans and rodents, given reported differences as compared to mammals in terms of affinity profiles of other reuptake inhibitors (Severinsen et al., Reference Severinsen, Sinning, Müller and Wiborg2008).

Differences between the serotonin systems of mammals and teleosts

The effect of acute serotonin elevation on indices of anxiety-like behaviour is not the only instance where zebrafish differ from mammals in terms of central serotonergic functioning. Several other examples relating to anatomy, neuroendocrinology and behavioural pharmacology exist:

(i) Zebrafish, as well as other teleosts, have a population of serotonergic neurones in the hypothalamus, their relevance to behavioural pharmacology being unclear (Kaslin & Panula, Reference Kaslin and Panula2001).

(ii) The teleost lineage underwent an early whole-genome duplication event early during its radiation, in addition to the two genome duplications shared by all jawed vertebrates (Volff, Reference Volff2005). This means that zebrafish have an extra copy of also all genes coding for the entire molecular machinery of the serotonin system. Though not all extra copies remain functional, it is unknown if and then how this increase in the genomic complexity of the serotonin system (and the opportunity for specialisation it has afforded) affects ethopharmacological responses.

(iii) Cortisol responses to acute serotonin elevation are divergent, with acute SRI treatment inducing a decrease in blood cortisol in zebrafish (de Abreu et al., Reference de, Koakoski, Ferreira, Oliveira, da, Gusso, Giacomini, Piato and Barcellos2014) while an increase is typically seen in humans and other mammals (Hesketh et al., Reference Hesketh, Jessop, Hogg and Harbuz2005; Ahrens et al., Reference Ahrens, Frankhauser, Lederbogen and Deuschle2007).

(iv) Behavioural responses to a reduction in serotonergic tone differ. Serotonin depletion by way of para-chlorophenylalanine (p-CPA) robustly reduces anxiety-like behaviour while increasing aggression and dominance behaviour in mammals and birds (Miczek et al., Reference Miczek, Altman, Appel and Boggan1975; Vergnes et al., Reference Vergnes, Depaulis and Boehrer1986; Buchanan et al., Reference Buchanan, Shrier and Hill1994; Studer et al., Reference Studer, Näslund, Andersson, Nilsson, Westberg and Eriksson2015) while the effect in zebrafish seems to be the opposite, both in terms of anxiety-like behaviour (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Puty, Benzecry, Araújo, Lima, de Jesus Oliveira Batista, Renata De Matos Oliveira, Crespo-Lopez and Herculano2013a; Müller et al., Reference Müller, Ziani, Fontana, Duarte, Stefanello, Canzian, Santos and Rosemberg2020) and aggression (Mezzomo et al., Reference Mezzomo, Müller, Franscescon, Michelotti, Souza, Rosemberg and Barcellos2020). Some data exist on the effects of p-CPA treatment in other teleost species, and while cichlids (Adams et al., Reference Adams, Liley and Gorzalka1996) also respond with decreased aggressive behaviour, this is not the case with bluebanded gobies (Lorenzi et al., Reference Lorenzi, Carpenter, Summers, Earley and Grober2009) or cleaner wrasses (Paula et al., Reference Paula, Messias, Grutter, Bshary and Soares2015).

(v) Responses to modulation of 5-HT2A/C receptors differ. In rodents, antagonism of these highly similar receptors is known to produce an anxiolytic-like response (Critchley & Handley, Reference Critchley and Handley1987; Griebel et al., Reference Griebel, Perrault and Sanger1997), while the opposite holds for agonism (Setem et al., Reference Setem, Pinheiro, Motta, Morato and Cruz1999). 5-HT2A/C antagonism is also a commonality of several antidepressant and/or anxiolytic drugs such as agomelatine, mianserin and mirtazapine, and though ultimately not marketed due to an unfavourable safety profile, the 5-HT2C antagonist ritanserin was investigated as a treatment for generalised anxiety disorder during the late 80ies, with positive results in several randomised controlled trials (Ceulemans et al., Reference Ceulemans, Hoppenbrouwers, Gelders and Reyntjens1985; Pangalila-Ratu Langi & Jansen, Reference Pangalila-Ratu Langi and Jansen1988). In zebrafish, acute administration of antagonists for these receptors increase (Nathan et al., Reference Nathan, Ogawa and Parhar2015), while agonists attenuate (do Carmo Silva et al., Reference do Carmo Silva, do Nascimento, Gomes, da Silva, Pinheiro, da Silva Chaves, Pimentel, Costa, Herculano, Lima-Maximino and Maximino2021) conspecific alarm substance-elicited avoidant behaviour – but note a weak inhibition of post-exposure avoidant behaviour of antagonists (do Carmo Silva et al., Reference do Carmo Silva, do Nascimento, Gomes, da Silva, Pinheiro, da Silva Chaves, Pimentel, Costa, Herculano, Lima-Maximino and Maximino2021).

(vi) There are indications of differences in how baseline temperament relates to serotonergic functioning. A zebrafish strain displaying high anxiety-like behaviour has been reported to exhibit low levels of central 5-HT and low TPH2 activity (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Puty, Matos Oliveira and Herculano2013b) (but note the more complex picture presented by Tran and co-workers [Tran et al., Reference Tran, Nowicki, Muraleetharan, Chatterjee and Gerlai2016]), while as mentioned earlier, the association between humans and rodents seems to be in the opposite direction.

(vii) Unlike in rodents (Grigoryan et al., Reference Grigoryan, Pavlova and Zaichenko2022), social isolation in zebrafish associates with decreased avoidance/anxiety-like behaviour in the scototaxis test (Shams et al., Reference Shams, Chatterjee and Gerlai2015; Varga et al., Reference Varga, Pejtsik, Biró, Zsigmond, Varga, Tóth, Salamon, Annus, Mikics and Aliczki2020) and is paralleled by increased serotonin release and turnover during the test (Varga et al., Reference Varga, Pejtsik, Biró, Zsigmond, Varga, Tóth, Salamon, Annus, Mikics and Aliczki2020) – but also with lower overall serotonin levels (Shams et al., Reference Shams, Chatterjee and Gerlai2015), although possibly only after short-term isolation (Shams et al., Reference Shams, Amlani, Buske, Chatterjee and Gerlai2018).

Finally, the influence of the serotonin system on aggression deserves some elaboration. Notwithstanding the divergent responses to serotonin depletion, the effects of acute (and chronic) SSRI treatment in zebrafish seem to be similar to those in mammals and birds, that is, a reduction of aggressive behaviour (Sperry et al., Reference Sperry, Thompson and Wingfield2003; Eriksson et al., Reference Eriksson, Ekman, Sinclair, Sörvik, Ysander, Mattson and Nissbrandt2008; Carrillo et al., Reference Carrillo, Ricci, Coppersmith and Melloni2009). However, available studies indicate that the effects of increased synaptic levels of serotonin on aggression may vary among teleosts as toadfish (McDonald et al., Reference McDonald, Gonzalez and Sloman2011) and matrinxã (Wolkers et al., Reference Wolkers, Serra, Barbosa Júnior and Urbinati2017) respond to serotonin elevation with increased aggressive behaviour while Siamese fighting fish (Lynn et al., Reference Lynn, Egar, Walker, Sperry and Ramenofsky2007) and blueheaded wrasses (Perreault et al., Reference Perreault, Semsar and Godwin2003), like zebrafish, respond with a decrease.

The role of sex and habituation

Male zebrafish have been reported to display lower levels of anxiety-like behaviour but higher levels of locomotion as compared to females (Genario et al., Reference Genario, de Abreu, Giacomini, Demin and Kalueff2020; dos Santos et al., Reference dos Santos, Giacomini, Marcon, Demin, Strekalova, de Abreu and Kalueff2021). We could observe the latter, but not the former, in Experiment III; however, a difference in terms of anxiety-like behaviour emerged in Experiment IV, with males being more ‘bold’. That this was observed in Experiment IV, but not V (the latter being conducted after thrice as long time as had elapsed between Experiments III and IV) could indicate that the sexes differ in terms of how they habituate to situations such as the scototaxis test – incidentally the only indication that habituation to the apparatus influenced our results in any way. In Experiment IV, females had markedly longer latencies to the first entry into the white compartment, while spending considerably less time there, as compared to the first session. This was not the case for males, and it was not the case for either sex in Experiment V. In no test did we observe an interaction between sex and treatment, indicating that sex has no substantive influence on the response to acute serotonin elevation. In our systematic review, we could identify only two studies where males and females had been directly compared in terms of behavioural SRI responses, with both employing the geotaxis test (or a variant thereof) and neither reporting a sex difference (Singer et al., Reference Singer, Oreschak, Rhinehart and Robison2016; Macrì et al., Reference Macrì, Clément, Spinello and Porfiri2019). These findings are fully in line with what we observed.

Relatedly, we chose to house animals in sex-segregated aquaria. It has been reported that sex-segregated animals exhibit lower stress levels as indicated by cortisol levels (Reolon et al., Reference Reolon, de Melo, da Rosa JG dos, Barcellos and Bonan2018). In terms of behaviour, some minor effects on activity levels and anxiety-like behaviour have been observed in sex-segregated animals kept in unenriched, but not enriched, aquaria (Soares et al., Reference Soares, Kirsten, Pompermaier, Maffi, Koakoski, Woloszyn, Barreto and Barcellos2020). As our aquaria were enriched, the net effect on behaviour is likely to have been small, if at all present, and overall stress levels should have been lower.

We found that most papers identified in the systematic review included insufficient information to assess the risk of bias (e.g. even though many mentioned randomisation, only two papers (Lima-Maximino et al., Reference Lima-Maximino, Pyterson, do Carmo Silva, Gomes, Rocha, Herculano, Rosemberg and Maximino2020; Alves et al., Reference Alves, Tamagno, Pompermaier, Vanin and Barcellos2023) described the process in any detail) and to sometimes even omit basic information necessary for replication such as sex ratios and group sizes. In general, it would seem that the field would benefit from a more structured reporting of methodology. We recommend looking at, for example, the ARRIVE guidelines (du Sert et al., Reference du Sert, Ahluwalia, Alam, Avey, Baker, Browne, Clark, Cuthill, Dirnagl, Emerson, Garner, Holgate, Howells, Hurst, Karp, Lazic, Lidster, MacCallum, Macleod, Pearl, Petersen, Rawle, Reynolds, Rooney, Sena, Silberberg, Steckler and Würbel2020) and also what is included in the SYRCLE assessment tool for systematic reviews (Hooijmans et al., Reference Hooijmans, Rovers, de Vries, Leenaars, Ritskes-Hoitinga and Langendam2014) not only when designing experiments but also during manuscript preparation in order to make sure that adequate information is provided while possible deviations necessitated by, for example, the condition modelled can be discussed in relation to such general guidelines.

Strengths and limitations

The results of the behavioural experiments appear to be robust; group sizes were large, we controlled for sex and habituation effects and we also employed a potent serotonin-releasing agent not previously used in behavioural zebrafish research: fenfluramine. It has a partly different mode of action as compared to SSRIs and tricyclic antidepressants, and the finding that another class of serotonin-releasing drug produces the same result as agents that block the serotonin transporter strengthens the case that the behavioural effects we and others have observed are due to an acute elevation of synaptic serotonin.

Some limitations also deserve to be mentioned. Though individual differences in baseline behaviour are likely to influence behavioural responses to the agents used, we neither assayed baseline temperament differences nor tagged animals in order to track the performance of individual fish over the three experiments. Also, the choice to re-use animals may have had an influence on subsequent experiments. Nevertheless, the fact that we in Experiment I saw no indications of long-term effects of escitalopram treatment suggests that such fears are largely unfounded and that zebrafish, at least using drugs and setups similar to ours, can be re-used with appropriate drug wash-out times. Also, even if small such effects would be present, the impact then likely manifests itself as an increase in variance which, all else being equal, decreases the risk of false positives and increases the risk of false negatives. Thus, some caution might be advised when interpreting, for example, the absence of effect in Experiments IV and V, but on the other hand, the main effect observed, that is, a general anxiolytic-like effect as indicated by an increase of total time spent in the white compartment in all three experiments (as well as in Experiment I), is then likely very robust.

Conclusion

To summarise, we have in a series of experiments observed a consistent anxiolytic-like effect of serotonin-releasing agents in zebrafish in the light-dark test. After reviewing the available literature, we found that there is robust evidence for an anxiolytic-like effect in the other major zebrafish model of anxiety, the novel-tank diving test. We also found that a number of studies indicate other notable differences in terms of how the serotonin system influences behaviour in zebrafish as compared to mammals. This would appear to be most pronounced in behavioural responses to short-term serotonin elevation or depletion, while responses to 5-HT1A agonists (Maximino et al., Reference Maximino, Puty, Benzecry, Araújo, Lima, de Jesus Oliveira Batista, Renata De Matos Oliveira, Crespo-Lopez and Herculano2013a; Magno et al., Reference Magno, Fontes, Gonçalves and Gouveia2015) and long-term SRI treatment (Egan et al., Reference Egan, Bergner, Hart, Cachat, Canavello, Elegante, Elkhayat, Bartels, Tien, Tien, Mohnot, Beeson, Glasgow, Amri, Zukowska and Kalueff2009; Magno et al., Reference Magno, Fontes, Gonçalves and Gouveia2015) are more similar, but differences also seem to be present in terms of neuroendocrinological responses to manipulation of the serotonin system and, to some extent, the relationship between serotonin and temperament. Whether this extends to related taxa is unclear. It is hard to say how representative zebrafish are in terms of behavioural responses to, for example, acute serotonin elevation in cyprinids, or teleosts, in general – as noted, the effects on aggression seem to vary in the handful of species investigated. In the case of anxiety-like behaviour we are aware of a single study, in piauçu (Barbosa et al., Reference Barbosa, Alves, de Fim Pereira, Ide and Hoffmann2012), where acute SSRI administration has been investigated in fish other than zebrafish; it was found to decrease antipredator behaviour, that is, a finding in line with observations in zebrafish.

Though our investigation has focused on short-term interventions, these (and other) indications of differential functioning in terms of the 5-HT system suggest that caution is prudent also when interpreting the effects of long-term treatment, as the mechanisms underlying the positive therapeutic effects of SRI administration in humans are poorly known. It cannot be taken for granted that a similarity of zebrafish and mammalian behavioural responses to chronic SRI administration also reflects a high degree of similarity in terms of underlying neurobiological mechanisms. In our opinion, the discrepancies we discuss in this paper warrant further investigation as they potentially call into question the translational value of research into affective disorders that employ zebrafish as a model organism. Of lesser practical, but not scientific, value is also further investigation of variation between species in terms of serotonergic functioning and how such variation may relate to ecological roles and evolutionary relationships.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/neu.2024.44.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank our animal husbandry personnel, Adam Alvenfors, Franziska Gerlach and Frida Bjerkås, for providing excellent care to the experimental animals.

Author contributions

JN, JL, FH, RKB and PK participated in the conception and design of the experiments. JN, JL and PK conducted the experiments. Data collection and analysis for the systematic review were conducted by JN, PK and FH. JN drafted the manuscript, and all authors performed a critical revision of the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the final version of the manuscript to be published.

Funding statement

Financial support was obtained from the following sources: Formas (2019-2096), the Carl Trygger Foundation for Scientific Research (CTS 20:2020), the Royal Society of Arts and Sciences in Gothenburg, the Åke Wiberg Foundation, the Wilhelm and Martina Lundgren Scientific Foundation, the Anér Foundation, the Längmanska Culture Foundation, the Mental Health Foundation (Sweden); the Wenner-Gren Foundations, the Åhlén Foundation and the Gothenburg Society of Medicine. None of these entities had any role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Competing interests

None of the authors have any conflict of interest to report.

References

de, Abreu MS, Koakoski, G, Ferreira, D, Oliveira, TA, da, Rosa JGS, Gusso, D, Giacomini, ACV, Piato, AL and Barcellos, LJG (2014) Diazepam and fluoxetine decrease the stress response in zebrafish. PLoS ONE 9(7), e.103232. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone0.103232.Google Scholar
Adams, CF, Liley, NR and Gorzalka, BB (1996) PCPA increases aggression in male firemouth cichlids. Pharmacology 53(5), 328330. DOI: 10.1159/000139446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahrens, T, Frankhauser, P, Lederbogen, F and Deuschle, M (2007) Effect of single-dose sertraline on the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal system, autonomic nervous system, and platelet function. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 27(6), 602606. DOI: 10.1097/jcp.0b013e31815abf0e.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albayrak, Y and Hashimoto, K (2017) Sigma-1 receptor agonists and their clinical implications in neuropsychiatric disorders. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology 964, 153161. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-50174-1_11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alves, C, Tamagno, WA, Pompermaier, A, Vanin, AP and Barcellos, LJG (2023) Neurotoxicological effects of venlafaxine on caenorhabditis elegans and danio rerio. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part C: Toxicology & Pharmacology. 271, 109658. DOI: 10.1016/j.cbpc.2023.109658.Google ScholarPubMed
Arfken, CL, Joseph, A, Sandhu, GR, Roehrs, T, Douglass, AB and Boutros, NN (2014) The status of sleep abnormalities as a diagnostic test for major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 156, 3645. DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.12.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandelow, B and Michaelis, S (2015) Epidemiology of anxiety disorders in the 21st century. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 17(3), 327335 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barbosa, A, Alves, FL, de Fim Pereira, AS, Ide, LM and Hoffmann, A (2012) Behavioral characterization of the alarm reaction and anxiolytic-like effect of acute treatment with fluoxetine in piauçu fish. Physiology & Behavior 105(3), 784790. DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.10.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bencan, Z, Sledge, D and Levin, ED (2009) Buspirone, chlordiazepoxide and diazepam effects in a zebrafish model of anxiety. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 94(1), 7580. DOI: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.07.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benneh, CK, Biney, RP, Mante, PK, Tandoh, A, Adongo, DW and Woode, E (2017) Maerua angolensis stem bark extract reverses anxiety and related behaviours in zebrafish—Involvement of GABAergic and 5-HT systems. Journal of Ethnopharmacology 207, 129145. DOI: 10.1016/j.jep.2017.06.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, CP, Shrier, EM and Hill, WL (1994) Time-dependent effects of PCPA on social aggression in chicks. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 49(3), 483488. DOI: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90059-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrillo, M, Ricci, LA, Coppersmith, GA and Melloni, RH (2009) The effect of increased serotonergic neurotransmission on aggression: a critical meta-analytical review of preclinical studies. Psychopharmacology 205(3), 349368. DOI: 10.1007/S00213-009-1543-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, BJ (1981) A specific laboratory test for the diagnosis of melancholia. Archives of General Psychiatry 38(1), 1517001. DOI: 10.1001/archpsyc.1981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ceulemans, D, Hoppenbrouwers, M-L, Gelders, Y and Reyntjens, A (1985) The influence of ritanserin, a serotonin antagonist, in anxiety disorders: a double-blind placebo-controlled study versus lorazepam. Pharmacopsychiatry 18(05), 303305. DOI: 10.1055/s-2007-1017385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamas, F, Serova, L and Sabban, EL (1999) Tryptophan hydroxylase mRNA levels are elevated by repeated immobilization stress in rat raphe nuclei but not in pineal gland. Neuroscience Letters 267(3), 157160. DOI: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00340-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chessick, CA, Allen, MH, Thase, ME, Batista Miralha da Cunha, AA, Kapczinski, F, de Lima, MSilva and dos Santos Souza, JJ (2006) Azapirones for generalized anxiety disorder. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2006(3), CD006115. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006115.Google ScholarPubMed
Clément, RJG, Macrì, S and Porfiri, M (2020) Design and development of a robotic predator as a stimulus in conditioned place aversion for the study of the effect of ethanol and citalopram in zebrafish. Behavioural Brain Research 378, 2256. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Melo, NCosta, Sánchez-Ortiz, BL, dos Santos Sampaio, TI, Matias Pereira, AC, da Silva Neto FL, Pinheiro, da Silva, HRibeiro, Alves Soares Cruz, R, Keita, H, Soares Pereira, AM and Tavares Carvalho, JC (2019) Anxiolytic and antidepressant effects of the hydroethanolic extract from the leaves of Aloysia polystachya (Griseb.) moldenke: a study on zebrafish (Danio rerio). Pharmaceuticals 12(3), 106. DOI: 10.3390/ph12030106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costall, B, Jones, BJ, Kelly, ME, Naylor, RJ and Tomkins, DM (1989) Exploration of mice in a black and white test box: validation as a model of anxiety. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 32(3), 777785. DOI: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90033-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Critchley, MAE and Handley, SL (1987) Effects in the X-maze anxiety model of agents acting at 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptors. Psychopharmacology 93(4), 502506. DOI: 10.1007/BF00207243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demin, KA, Kolesnikova, TO, Khatsko, SL, Meshalkina, DA, Efimova, EV, YYu, Morzherin and Kalueff, AV (2017) Acute effects of amitriptyline on adult zebrafish: potential relevance to antidepressant drug screening and modeling human toxidromes. Neurotoxicology and Teratology 62, 2733. DOI: 10.1016/j.ntt.2017.04.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
do Carmo Silva, RX, do Nascimento, BG, Gomes, GCV, da Silva, NAH, Pinheiro, JS, da Silva Chaves, SN, Pimentel, AFN, Costa, BPD, Herculano, AM, Lima-Maximino, M and Maximino, C (2021) 5-HT2C agonists and antagonists block different components of behavioral responses to potential, distal, and proximal threat in zebrafish. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 210, 173276. DOI: 10.1016/j.pbb.2021.173276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
dos Santos, BE, Giacomini, ACVV, Marcon, L, Demin, KA, Strekalova, T, de Abreu, MS and Kalueff, AV (2021) Sex differences shape zebrafish performance in a battery of anxiety tests and in response to acute scopolamine treatment. Neuroscience Letters. 759, 135993. DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2021.135993.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
dos Santos Sampaio, TI, de Melo, NC, de Freitas Paiva, BT, da Silva Aleluia, GA, da Silva Neto, FLP, da Silva, HR, Keita, H, Cruz, RAS, Sánchez-Ortiz, BL, Pineda-Peña, EA, Balderas, JL, Navarrete, A and Carvalho, JCT (2018) Leaves of spondias mombin L. a traditional anxiolytic and antidepressant: pharmacological evaluation on zebrafish (Danio rerio). Journal of Ethnopharmacology 224, 563578. DOI: 10.1016/j.jep.2018.05.037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
du Sert, NP, Ahluwalia, A, Alam, S, Avey, MT, Baker, M, Browne, WJ, Clark, A, Cuthill, IC, Dirnagl, U, Emerson, M, Garner, P, Holgate, ST, Howells, DW, Hurst, V, Karp, NA, Lazic, SE, Lidster, K, MacCallum, CJ, Macleod, M, Pearl, EJ, Petersen, OH, Rawle, F, Reynolds, P, Rooney, K, Sena, ES, Silberberg, SD, Steckler, T and Würbel, H (2020) Reporting animal research: explanation and elaboration for the ARRIVE guidelines 2.0. PLOS Biology 18(7), e3000411. DOI: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PBIO.3000411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egan, RJ, Bergner, CL, Hart, PC, Cachat, JM, Canavello, PR, Elegante, MF, Elkhayat, SI, Bartels, BK, Tien, AK, Tien, DH, Mohnot, S, Beeson, E, Glasgow, E, Amri, H, Zukowska, Z and Kalueff, AV (2009) Understanding behavioral and physiological phenotypes of stress and anxiety in zebrafish. Behavioural Brain Research 205(1), 3844. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.06.022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksson, E, Ekman, A, Sinclair, S, Sörvik, K, Ysander, C, Mattson, U-B and Nissbrandt, H (2008) Escitalopram administered in the luteal phase exerts a marked and dose-dependent effect in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 28(2), 195202. DOI: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e3181678a28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
File, SE (1993) The interplay of learning and anxiety in the elevated plus-maze. Behavioural Brain Research 58(1-2), 199202. DOI: 10.1016/0166-4328(93)90103-W.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
File, SE and Guardiola-Lemaitre, BJ (1988) L-fenfluramine in tests of dominance and anxiety in the rat. Neuropsychobiology 20(4), 205211. DOI: 10.1159/000118500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finney, JL, Robertson, GN, McGee, CAS, Smith, FM and Croll, RP (2006) Structure and autonomic innervation of the swim bladder in the zebrafish (Danio rerio). Journal of Comparative Neurology 495(5), 587606. DOI: 10.1002/cne.20948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontana, BD, Alnassar, N and Parker, MO (2022) The zebrafish (Danio rerio) anxiety test battery: comparison of behavioral responses in the novel tank diving and light-dark tasks following exposure to anxiogenic and anxiolytic compounds. Psychopharmacology 239(1), 287296. DOI: 10.1007/S00213-021-05990-W.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fossat, P, Bacqué-Cazenave, J, De Deurwaerdère, P, Delbecque, J-P and Cattaert, D (2014) Anxiety-like behavior in crayfish is controlled by serotonin. Science 344(6189), 12931297. DOI: 10.1126/science.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, A, Åhs, F, Appel, L, Jonasson, M, Wahlstedt, K, Bani, M, Merlo Pich, E, Bettica, P, Långström, B, Lubberink, M, Fredrikson, M and Furmark, T (2016) Reduced serotonin synthesis and regional cerebral blood flow after anxiolytic treatment of social anxiety disorder. European Neuropsychopharmacology 26(11), 17751783. DOI: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.09.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, A, Åhs, F, Engman, J, Jonasson, M, Alaie, I, Björkstrand, J, Frans, Ö., Faria, V, Linnman, C, Appel, L, Wahlstedt, K, Lubberink, M, Fredrikson, M and Furmark, T (2015) Serotonin synthesis and reuptake in social anxiety disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 72(8), 794. DOI: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García-Gutiérrez, MS, Navarrete, F, Sala, F, Gasparyan, A, Austrich-Olivares, A and Manzanares, J (2020) Biomarkers in psychiatry: concept, definition, types and relevance to the clinical reality. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 11, 432. DOI: 10.3389/FPSYT.2020.00432/BIBTEX.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, KL, Hale, MW, Lightman, SL, Plotsky, PM and Lowry, CA (2009) Adverse early life experience and social stress during adulthood interact to increase serotonin transporter mRNA expression. Brain Research 1305, 4763. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genario, R, de Abreu, MS, Giacomini, ACVV, Demin, KA and Kalueff, AV (2020) Sex differences in behavior and neuropharmacology of zebrafish. European Journal of Neuroscience 52(1), 25862603. DOI: 10.1111/ejn.14438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giacomini, ACVV, Abreu, MS, v., Giacomini L, Siebel, AM, Zimerman, FF, Rambo, CL, Mocelin, R, Bonan, CD, Piato, AL and Barcellos, LJG (2016) Fluoxetine and diazepam acutely modulate stress induced-behavior. Behavioural Brain Research 296, 301310. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.09.027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giacomini, ACVV, Piassetta, AS, Genario, R, Bonan, CD, Piato, A, Barcellos, LJG and de Abreu, MS (2020) Tryptophan alleviates neuroendocrine and behavioral responses to stress in zebrafish. Behavioural Brain Research 378, 112264. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graeff, FG, Guimarães, FS, De Andrade, TGCS and Deakin, JFW (1996) Role of 5-HT in stress, anxiety, and depression. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 54(1), 129141. DOI: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02135-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griebel, G, Moreau, J-L, Jenck, F, Misslin, R and Martin, JR (1994) Acute and chronic treatment with 5-HT reuptake inhibitors differentially modulate emotional responses in anxiety models in rodents. Psychopharmacology 113(3-4), 463470. DOI: 10.1007/BF02245224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griebel, G, Perrault, G and Sanger, DJ (1997) A comparative study of the effects of selective and non-selective 5-HT2 receptor subtype antagonists in rat and mouse models of anxiety. Neuropharmacology 36(6), 793802. DOI: 10.1016/S0028-3908(97)00034-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigoryan, GA, Pavlova, IV and Zaichenko, MI (2022) Effects of social isolation on the development of anxiety and depression-like behavior in model experiments in animals. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology 52(5), 5738. DOI: 10.1007/S11055-022-01297-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grillon, C, Levenson, J and Pine, DS (2007) A single dose of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram exacerbates anxiety in humans: a fear-potentiated startle study. Neuropsychopharmacology 32(1), 225231. DOI: 10.1038/sj.npp1301204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, TJ, Kwan, GT, Gallup, J and Tresguerres, M (2016) Acute fluoxetine exposure alters crab anxiety-like behaviour, but not aggressiveness. Scientific Reports 6(1), 19850. DOI: 10.1038/SREP19850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hesketh, S, Jessop, DS, Hogg, S and Harbuz, MS (2005) Differential actions of acute and chronic citalopram on the rodent hypothalamic-pituitary–adrenal axis response to acute restraint stress. Journal of Endocrinology 185(3), 373382. DOI: 10.1677/joe.1.06074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hieronymus, F, Nilsson, S and Eriksson, E (2016) A mega-analysis of fixed-dose trials reveals dose-dependency and a rapid onset of action for the antidepressant effect of three selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Translational Psychiatry 6104(6), e834e837. DOI: 10.1038/tp.2016.104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hooijmans, CR, Rovers, MM, de Vries, RB, Leenaars, M, Ritskes-Hoitinga, M and Langendam, MW (2014) SYRCLE’s risk of bias tool for animal studies. BMC Medical Research Methodology 14(1), 43. DOI: 10.1186/1471-2288-14-43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imai, H, Tajika, A, Chen, P, Pompoli, A, Guaiana, G, Castellazzi, M, Bighelli, I, Girlanda, F, Barbui, C, Koesters, M, Cipriani, A and Furukawa, TA (2014) Azapirones versus placebo for panic disorder in adults. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2014(9), CD010828. DOI: 10.1002/14651858CD0.10828.pub2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iturriaga Vásquez, P, Osorio, F and Herzog, R (2012) Zebrafish: a model for behavioural pharmacology. Revista de farmacología de Chile 5(1), 2732.Google Scholar
Karakaya, M, Scaramuzzi, A, Macrì, S and Porfiri, M (2021) Acute citalopram administration modulates anxiety in response to the context associated with a robotic stimulus in zebrafish. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 108, 110172. DOI: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlsson, L, Carlsson, B, Hiemke, C, Ahlner, J, Bengtsson, F, Schmitt, U and Kugelberg, FC (2013) Altered brain concentrations of citalopram and escitalopram in P-glycoprotein deficient mice after acute and chronic treatment. European Neuropsychopharmacology 23(11), 16361644. DOI: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.01.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaslin, J and Panula, P (2001) Comparative anatomy of the histaminergic and other aminergic systems in zebrafish (danio rerio). Journal of Comparative Neurology 440(4), 342377. DOI: 10.1002/cne.1390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, N, Sacher, J, Geiss-Granadia, T, Attarbaschi, T, Mossaheb, N, Lanzenberger, R, Pötzi, C, Holik, A, Spindelegger, C, Asenbaum, S, Dudczak, R, Tauscher, J and Kasper, S (2006) In vivo imaging of serotonin transporter occupancy by means of SPECT and [123I]ADAM in healthy subjects administered different doses of escitalopram or citalopram. Psychopharmacology 188(3), 263272. DOI: 10.1007/S00213-006-0486-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kulikova, EA, Bazovkina, DV, Evsyukova, VS and Kulikov, AV (2021) Acute administration of imipramine and citalopram increases activity of striatal-enriched tyrosine protein phosphatase (STEP) in brain of zebrafish danio rerio. Bulletin of Experimental Biology and Medicine 170(5), 627630. DOI: 10.1007/s10517-021-05120-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laferrere, B and Wurtman, RJ (1989) Effect of d-fenfluramine on serotonin release in brains of anaesthetized rats. Brain Research 504(2), 258263. DOI: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)91365-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, CA, Mueller, K, Zsido, RG, Reinelt, J, Regenthal, R, Okon-Singer, H, Forbes, EE, Villringer, A and Sacher, J (2021) A single dose of escitalopram blunts the neural response in the thalamus and caudate during monetary loss. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 46(3), E319E327. DOI: 10.1503/jpn.200121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lima-Maximino, M, Pyterson, MP, do Carmo Silva, RX, Gomes, GCV, Rocha, SP, Herculano, AM, Rosemberg, DB and Maximino, C (2020) Phasic and tonic serotonin modulate alarm reactions and post-exposure behavior in zebrafish. Journal of Neurochemistry 153(4), 495509. DOI: 10.1111/jnc.14978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenzi, V, Carpenter, RE, Summers, CH, Earley, RL and Grober, MS (2009) Serotonin, social status and sex change in the bluebanded goby lythrypnus dalli. Physiology & Behavior 97(3-4), 476483. DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynn, SE, Egar, JM, Walker, BG, Sperry, TS and Ramenofsky, M (2007) Fish on prozac: a simple, noninvasive physiology laboratory investigating the mechanisms of aggressive behavior in betta splendens . Advances in Physiology Education 31(4), 358363. DOI: 10.1152/advan.00024.2007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macrì, S, Clément, RJG, Spinello, C and Porfiri, M (2019) Comparison between two- and three-dimensional scoring of zebrafish response to psychoactive drugs: identifying when three-dimensional analysis is needed. PeerJ 7(10), e7893. DOI: 10.7717/PEERJ.7893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magno, LDP, Fontes, A, Gonçalves, BMN and Gouveia, A (2015) Pharmacological study of the light/dark preference test in zebrafish (Danio rerio): Waterborne administration. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 135, 169176. DOI: 10.1016/j.pbb.2015.05.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malyshev, A V, Sukhanova, IA, Zlobin, AS, Gedzun, VR, Pavshintsev, V V, Vasileva, E V, Zalevsky, AO, Doronin, II, Mitkin, NA, Golovin, A V, Lovat, ML, Kovalev, GI, Zolotarev, YA, Kuchumov, AR, Babkin, GA and Luscher, B (2021) In silico Screening and Behavioral Validation of a Novel Peptide, LCGA-17, With Anxiolytic-Like Properties. Frontiers in Neuroscience 15. DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2021.705590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maximino, C, da Silva, AWB, Gouveia, A and Herculano, AM (2011) Pharmacological analysis of zebrafish (Danio rerio) scototaxis. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 35(2), 624631. DOI: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.01.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maximino, C, Gemaque, J, Benzecry, R, Lima, MG, Batista, EDJO, Picanço-Diniz, DW, Oliveira, KRM and Herculano, AM (2015) Role of nitric oxide in the behavioral and neurochemical effects of IB-MECA in zebrafish. Psychopharmacology 232(10), 16711680. DOI: 10.1007/S00213-014-3799-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maximino, C, Lima, MG, Costa, CC, Guedes, IML and Herculano, AM (2014) Fluoxetine and WAY 100,635 dissociate increases in scototaxis and analgesia induced by conspecific alarm substance in zebrafish (Danio rerio Hamilton 1822). Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 124, 425433. DOI: 10.1016/j.pbb.2014.07.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maximino, C, Marques de Brito, T, de Dias C.A.G., M, Gouveia, A and Morato, S (2010) Scototaxis as anxiety-like behavior in fish. Nature Protocols 5(2), 209216. DOI: 10.1038/nprot.2009.225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maximino, C, Puty, B, Benzecry, R, Araújo, J, Lima, MG, de Jesus Oliveira Batista, E, Renata De Matos Oliveira, K, Crespo-Lopez, ME and Herculano, AM (2013a) Role of serotonin in zebrafish (Danio rerio) anxiety: relationship with serotonin levels and effect of buspirone, WAY 100635, SB 224289, fluoxetine and para-chlorophenylalanine (pCPA) in two behavioral models. Neuropharmacology 71, 8397. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.03.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maximino, C, Puty, B, Matos Oliveira, KR and Herculano, AM (2013b), Behavioral and neurochemical changes in the zebrafish leopard strain. Genes, Brain and Behavior 12(5), 576582. DOI: 10.1111/gbb12047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, MD, Gonzalez, A and Sloman, KA (2011) Higher levels of aggression are observed in socially dominant toadfish treated with the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, fluoxetine. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part C: Toxicology & Pharmacology 153(1), 107112. DOI: 10.1016/j.cbpc.2010.09.006.Google ScholarPubMed
Mezzomo, NJ, Müller, TE, Franscescon, F, Michelotti, P, Souza, TP, Rosemberg, DB and Barcellos, LJG (2020) Taurine-mediated aggression is abolished via 5-HT1A antagonism and serotonin depletion in zebrafish. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 199, 173067. DOI: 10.1016/j.pbb.2020.173067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miczek, KA, Altman, JL, Appel, JB and Boggan, WO (1975) Para-chlorophenylalanine, serotonin and killing behavior. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 3(3), 355361. DOI: 10.1016/0091-3057(75)90043-X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mombereau, C, Gur, TL, Onksen, J and Blendy, JA (2010) Differential effects of acute and repeated citalopram in mouse models of anxiety and depression. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 13(03), 321. DOI: 10.1017/S1461145709990630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, TE, Ziani, PR, Fontana, BD, Duarte, T, Stefanello, FV, Canzian, J, Santos, ARS and Rosemberg, DB (2020) Role of the serotonergic system in ethanol-induced aggression and anxiety: a pharmacological approach using the zebrafish model. European Neuropsychopharmacology 32, 6676. DOI: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2019.12.120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Näslund, J, Hieronymus, F, Emilsson, JF, Lisinski, A, Nilsson, S and Eriksson, E (2017) Incidence of early anxiety aggravation in trials of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 136(4), 343351. DOI: 10.1111/acps12784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Näslund, J, Studer, E, Nilsson, K, Westberg, L and Eriksson, E (2013) Serotonin depletion counteracts sex differences in anxiety-related behaviour in rat. Psychopharmacology 230(1), 2935. DOI: 10.1007/s00213-013-3133-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Näslund, J, Studer, E, Pettersson, R, Hagsäter, M, Nilsson, S, Nissbrandt, H and Eriksson, E (2015) Differences in anxiety-like behavior within a batch of wistar rats are associated with differences in serotonergic transmission, enhanced by acute SRI administration, and abolished by serotonin depletion. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 18(8), pyv018. DOI: 10.1093/ijnp/pyv018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nathan, FM, Ogawa, S and Parhar, IS (2015) Kisspeptin1 modulates odorant-evoked fear response via two serotonin receptor subtypes (5-HT1A and 5-HT2) in zebrafish. Journal of Neurochemistry 133(6), 870878. DOI: 10.1111/JNC.13105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nedahl, M, Johansen, SS and Linnet, K (2018) Reference brain/Blood concentrations of citalopram, duloxetine, Mirtazapine and sertraline. Journal of Analytical Toxicology 42(3), 149156. DOI: 10.1093/jat/bkx098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nierenberg, AA and Feinstein, AR (1988) How to evaluate a diagnostic marker test. Lessons from the rise and fall of dexamethasone suppression test. JAMA 259(11), 16991702. DOI: 10.1001/jama.19881036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palagini, L, Baglioni, C, Ciapparelli, A, Gemignani, A and Riemann, D (2013) REM sleep dysregulation in depression: state of the art. Sleep Medicine Reviews 17(5), 377390. DOI: 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.11.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pangalila-Ratu Langi, EA and Jansen, AAI (1988) Ritanserin in the treatment of generalized anxiety disorders: a placebo-controlled trial. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental 3(3), 207212. DOI: 10.1002/hup.470030309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, ED, Johnson, PL, Shekhar, A and Lowry, CA (2014) The Deakin/Graeff hypothesis: focus on serotonergic inhibition of panic. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 46(P3), 379396. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paula, JR, Messias, JP, Grutter, AS, Bshary, R and Soares, MC (2015) The role of serotonin in the modulation of cooperative behavior. Behavioral Ecology 26(4), 10051012. DOI: 10.1093/BEHECO/ARV039.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perreault, H, Semsar, K and Godwin, J (2003) Fluoxetine treatment decreases territorial aggression in a coral reef fish. Physiology & Behavior 79(4-5), 719724. DOI: 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00211-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinna, G, Costa, E and Guidotti, A (2009) SSRIs act as selective brain steroidogenic stimulants (SBSSs) at low doses that are inactive on 5-HT reuptake. Current Opinion in Pharmacology 9(1), 2430. DOI: 10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preskorn, SH (1997) Clinically relevant pharmacology of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Clinical Pharmacokinetics 32(Supplement 1), 121. DOI: 10.2165/00003088-199700321-00003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reolon, GK, de Melo, GM, da Rosa JG dos, S, Barcellos, LJG and Bonan, CD (2018) Sex and the housing: effects on behavior, cortisol levels and weight in zebrafish. Behavioural Brain Research 336, 8592. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.08.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruibal, C, Soengas, J and Aldegunde, M (2002) Brain serotonin and the control of food intake in rainbow trout (oncorhynchus mykiss): effects of changes in plasma glucose levels. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 188(6), 479484. DOI: 10.1007/s00359-002-0320-z.Google ScholarPubMed
Sabadin, GR, Biasuz, E, Canzian, J, Adedara, IA and Rosemberg, DB (2022) A novel behavioral paradigm to measure anxiety-like behaviors in zebrafish by the concomitant assessment of geotaxis and scototaxis. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 118, 110579. DOI: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sackerman, J, Donegan, JJ, Cunningham, CS, Nguyen, NN, Lawless, K, Long, A, Benno, RH and Gould, GG (2010) Zebrafish behavior in novel environments: effects of acute exposure to anxiolytic compounds and choice of danio rerio line. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 23(1), 4361. DOI: 10.46867/IJCP.2010.23.01.06.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez, C and Hyttel, J (1999) Comparison of the effects of antidepressants and their metabolites on reuptake of biogenic amines and on receptor binding. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology 19(4), 467489. DOI: 10.1023/A:1006986824213/METRICS.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, D, Hernandez, L and Hoebel, BG (1989) Fenfluramine administered systemically or locally increases extracellular serotonin in the lateral hypothalamus as measured by microdialysis. Brain Research 482(2), 261270. DOI: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)91189-X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Setem, J, Pinheiro, AP, Motta, VA, Morato, S and Cruz, APM (1999) Ethopharmacological analysis of 5-HT Ligands on the rat elevated plus-maze. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 62(3), 515521. DOI: 10.1016/S0091-3057(98)00193-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Severinsen, K, Sinning, S, Müller, HK and Wiborg, O (2008) Characterisation of the zebrafish serotonin transporter functionally links TM10 to the ligand binding site. Journal of Neurochemistry 105(5), 17941805. DOI: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05285.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shams, S, Amlani, S, Buske, C, Chatterjee, D and Gerlai, R (2018) Developmental social isolation affects adult behavior, social interaction, and dopamine metabolite levels in zebrafish. Developmental Psychobiology 60(1), 4356. DOI: 10.1002/dev.21581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shams, S, Chatterjee, D and Gerlai, R (2015) Chronic social isolation affects thigmotaxis and whole-brain serotonin levels in adult zebrafish. Behavioural Brain Research 292, 283287. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.05.061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sidor, MM, Amath, A, MacQueen, G and Foster, JA (2010) A developmental characterization of mesolimbocortical serotonergic gene expression changes following early immune challenge. Neuroscience 171(3), 734746. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.08.060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinclair, LI, Christmas, DM, Hood, SD, Potokar, JP, Robertson, A, Isaac, A, Srivastava, S, Nutt, DJ and Davies, SJC (2009) Antidepressant-induced jitteriness/anxiety syndrome: systematic review. British Journal of Psychiatry 194(6), 483490. DOI: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.048371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, ML, Oreschak, K, Rhinehart, Z and Robison, BD (2016) Anxiolytic effects of fluoxetine and nicotine exposure on exploratory behavior in zebrafish. PeerJ 4(8), e2352. DOI: 10.7717/peerj.2352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinyakova, NA, Kulikova, EA, Englevskii, NA and Kulikov, AV (2018) Effects of fluoxetine and potential antidepressant 8-trifluoromethyl 1,2,3,4,5-benzopentathiepin-6-amine hydrochloride (TC-2153) on behavior of danio rerio fish in the novel tank test and brain content of biogenic amines and their metabolites. Bulletin of Experimental Biology and Medicine 164(5), 620623. DOI: 10.1007/s10517-018-4045-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soares, SM, Kirsten, K, Pompermaier, A, Maffi, VC, Koakoski, G, Woloszyn, M, Barreto, RE and Barcellos, LJG (2020) Sex segregation affects exploratory and social behaviors of zebrafish according to controlled housing conditions. Physiology & Behavior. 222, 112944. DOI: 10.1016/J.PHYSBEH.2020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Søgaard, B, Mengel, H, Rao, N and Larsen, F (2005) The pharmacokinetics of escitalopram after oral and intravenous administration of single and multiple doses to healthy subjects. The Journal of Clinical Pharmacology 45(12), 14001406. DOI: 10.1177/0091270005280860.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperry, TS, Thompson, CK and Wingfield, JC (2003) Effects of acute treatment with 8-OH-DPAT and fluoxetine on aggressive behaviour in male song sparrows (Melospiza melodia morphna). Journal of Neuroendocrinology 15(2), 150160. DOI: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.00968.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, PK, Carney, RM, Freedland, KE, Skala, JA, Jaffe, AS, Kleiger, RE and Rottman, JN (2000) Severe depression is associated with markedly reduced heart rate variability in patients with stable coronary heart disease. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 48(4-5), 493500. DOI: 10.1016/S0022-3999(99)00085-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, A, Wu, N, Cachat, J, Hart, P, Gaikwad, S, Wong, K, Utterback, E, Gilder, T, Kyzar, E, Newman, A, Carlos, D, Chang, K, Hook, M, Rhymes, C, Caffery, M, Greenberg, M, Zadina, J and Kalueff, AV (2011) Pharmacological modulation of anxiety-like phenotypes in adult zebrafish behavioral models. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 35(6), 14211431. DOI: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.11.035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, AM, Cachat, J, Gaikwad, S, Robinson, KSL, Gebhardt, M and Kalueff, AV (2013) Perspectives on experimental models of serotonin syndrome in zebrafish. Neurochemistry International 62(6), 893902. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuint.2013.02.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Studer, E, Näslund, J, Andersson, E, Nilsson, S, Westberg, L and Eriksson, E (2015) Serotonin depletion-induced maladaptive aggression requires the presence of androgens. Plos One 10(5), e0126462. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Targum, SD and Marshall, LE (1989) Fenfluramine provocation of anxiety in patients with panic disorder. Psychiatry Research 28(3), 295306. DOI: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90210-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tran, S, Nowicki, M, Muraleetharan, A, Chatterjee, D and Gerlai, R (2016) Neurochemical factors underlying individual differences in locomotor activity and anxiety-like behavioral responses in zebrafish. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 65, 2533. DOI: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.08.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varga, ZK, Pejtsik, D, Biró, L, Zsigmond, Á., Varga, M, Tóth, B, Salamon, V, Annus, T, Mikics, É and Aliczki, M (2020) Conserved serotonergic background of experience-dependent behavioral responsiveness in zebrafish (danio rerio). The Journal of Neuroscience 40(23), 45514564. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2178-19.2020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vergnes, M, Depaulis, A and Boehrer, A (1986) Parachlorophenylalanine-induced serotonin depletion increases offensive but not defensive aggression in male rats. Physiology & Behavior 36(4), 653658. DOI: 10.1016/0031-9384(86)90349-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volff, J-N (2005) Genome evolution and biodiversity in teleost fish. Heredity 94(3), 280294. DOI: 10.1038/sj.hdy.6800635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warnick, J, Huang, C, Acevedo, E and Sufka, K (2009) Modelling the anxiety-depression continuum in chicks. Journal of Psychopharmacology 23(2), 143156. DOI: 10.1177/0269881108089805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weischer, ML (1966) [Influence of anorectics of the amphetamine series on the behavior of the siamese warrior fish betta splendens]. Arzneimittel-Forschung 16(10), 13101311.Google ScholarPubMed
Winberg, S and Thörnqvist, P-O (2016) Role of brain serotonin in modulating fish behavior. Current Zoology 62(3), 317323. DOI: 10.1093/cz/zow037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolkers, CPB, Serra, M, Barbosa Júnior, A and Urbinati, EC (2017) Acute fluoxetine treatment increases aggressiveness in juvenile matrinxã (Brycon amazonicus). Fish Physiology and Biochemistry 43(3), 755759. DOI: 10.1007/s10695-016-0329-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Y, Kecskés, A, Copmans, D, Langlois, M, Crawford, AD, Ceulemans, B, Lagae, L, de Witte, PAM and Esguerra, CV (2015) Pharmacological characterization of an antisense knockdown zebrafish model of Dravet syndrome: inhibition of epileptic seizures by the serotonin agonist fenfluramine. PLOS ONE 10(5), e0125898. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone0125898.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. A ‘Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-analyses’ (PRISMA) flowchart illustrating the literature search process (A). Cumulative bar charts displaying risk of bias assessment across 10 items specified in SYRCLE’s assessment tool (B); due to the limited methodological descriptions of most articles, it was not possible to confidently classify any work as clearly not conforming to SYRCLE guidelines. Pie charts illustrating the distribution of relevant categories of sex (C) and substance used (D) in comparisons included in the systematic review. Histogram over number of identified studies published by year since 2010 (E). For items B–E, data are shown separately for the light-dark (LD, left) and novel-tank diving test (NTD, right).

Figure 1

Table 1. Reported effects of acute SRI administration in the novel-tank diving, or geotaxis, test using adult zebrafish

Figure 2

Table 2. Reported effects of acute SRI administration in the scototaxis test using adult zebrafish

Figure 3

Figure 2. Experiment I: effects of escitalopram on latency to first entry into the white compartment (A), number of entries into the white compartment (B) and time spent in the white compartment (C) in the light-dark test. The test duration was 900 s. Given in the figure are the results of a two-way ANOVA with treatment status 6 weeks before and treatment status on the day of the light-dark session, respectively, as predictive variables. Error bars indicate SEM. Group sizes: n = 6 for (CTRL-CTRL), otherwise 7. ESC = escitalopram, CTRL = control.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Experiment II: effects of fenfluramine (FEN) on latency to first entry into the white compartment (A), number of entries into the white compartment (B) and time spent in the white compartment (C) in the light-dark test. The test duration was 900 s. Indicated in the figure are p-values representing comparisons made to the control group. Error bars indicate SEM. Group sizes: n = 10 for FEN 0.5 mg/ml, otherwise 11. CTRL = control, n.s. = non-significant, * = p < 0.05.

Figure 5

Figure 4. Experiments III–V: effects of escitalopram (ESC) (Experiments III and IV) and fenfluramine (FEN) (Experiment V) on latency to first entry into the white compartment (A), number of entries into the white compartment (B) and time spent in the white compartment (C) in the light-dark test. The test duration was 900 s. Given in the figure are the results of two-way ANOVAs with treatment and sex as predictive variables. Error bars indicate SEM. CTRL = control. Group sizes: n = 18 (males, control), 34 (males, ESC) 13 (females, control) and 26 (females, ESC) in Experiment III, 30 (males) and 17 (females) in Experiment IV and 26 (males) and 16 (females) in Experiment V.

Supplementary material: File

Näslund et al. supplementary material

Näslund et al. supplementary material
Download Näslund et al. supplementary material(File)
File 13.8 KB