Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T19:01:38.115Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The flexible brain. On mind and brain, neural darwinism and psychiatry

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2015

Summary

A theoretical introduction is given in which several theoretical viewpoints concerning the mind-brain problem are discussed. During the last decade philosophers like Searle, Dennett and the Churchlands have taken a more or less pure materialistic position in explaining mental phenomena.

Investigators in biological psychiatry have hardly ever taken a clear position in this discussion, whereas we believe it is important that the conclusions drawn from biological research are embedded in a theoretical framework related to the mind-brain problem.

In this article the thesis is defended that the theory of neural darwinism represents a major step forward and may bridge previous distinctions between biological, clinical and social psychiatry.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology 1997

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Literatuur

1.Gontsjarov, IA. Oblomov (1859). Nederlandse vertaling: Amsterdam: G.A. van Oorschot, 1966.Google Scholar
2.Lycan, WG. Mind and Cognition; a Reader. Oxford: Blackwell, 1992.Google Scholar
3.Warner, R, Szuba, T, red. The Mind-body Problem; a Guide to the current Debate. Oxford: Blackwell, 1994.Google Scholar
4.Kim, J. Philosophy of Mind. Oxford: Westview Press, 1996.Google Scholar
5.Cairns-Smith, AG. Evolving the Mind; on the Nature of Matter and the Origin of Consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press, 1996.Google Scholar
6.Godfrey-Smith, P. Complexity and the Function of Mind in Nature. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press, 1996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7.Popper, KR, Eccles, JC. The self and its Brain; an Argument for Interactionism. Berlin: Springer Verlag, 1977.Google Scholar
8.Searle, JR. The rediscovery of the Mind. Cambridge: Bradford Books, MIT Press, 1992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9.Churchland, PS. Neurophilosophy; toward a unified Science of the Mind/brain. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1986.Google Scholar
10.Foster, J. The immaterial Self; a Defence of the cartesian Dualist conception of the Mind, London: Routledge, 1991.Google Scholar
11.Lindahl, BIB, Arhem, P. Mind as a force field: comments on a new interactionistic hypothesis. J theor Biol 1994; 171: 111–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Dennett, D. Het Bewustzijn verklaard. Amsterdam: Contact, 1993.Google Scholar
13.Dennett, D. Aspecten van Bewustzijn. Amsterdam: Contact, 1996.Google Scholar
14.Damasio, AR. De Vergissing van Descartes; Gevoel Verstand en het menselijk Brein. Amsterdam: Wereldbibliotheek, 1995.Google Scholar
15.Crane, HD, Piantanida, TP. On seeing reddish-green and yellowish-blue. Science 1983; 221: 1078–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16.Churchland, PS, Ramachadran, VS. Filling in: Why Dennett is wrong. In: Revonsuo, A, Kamppinen, M, eds. Consciousness in Philosophy and cognitive Neuroscience. Hillsdale New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Ass, 1994:6591.Google Scholar
17.Krauskopf, J. Effect of the retinal image stabilization on the appearance of hetero-chromatic targets. J optic Soc Am 1963; 53: 741–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18.Kousbroek, R. Einsteins Poppenhuis. Amsterdam: Meulenhof, 1990.Google Scholar
19.Lockwood, M. Mind, Brain and the Quantum; the Compound ‘I’. Oxford: Blackwell, 1989.Google Scholar
20.Guichard, L. The causal efficacy of propositional attitudes. Poznan studies in the philosophy of the sciences and the humanities 1995; 45:373–92.Google Scholar
21.Pinker, S. Het Taalinstinct; het taalscheppend Vermogen van de Mens. Amsterdam: Contact, 1995.Google Scholar
22.Praag, HM van. Make believes in Psychiatry, or the Perils of Progress. New York: Brunner Mazel, 1993.Google Scholar
23.Jobe, TH, Fichtner, CG, Port, JD, Gaviria, MM. Neuropoiesis: Proposal for a connectionistic neurobiology. Med Hypotheses 1995; 45: 147–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Putnam, H. Mind, Language and Reality: Philosophic Papers, vol. 2, Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press, 1975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
25.Baker, LR. Explaining Attitudes; a practical Approach to the Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press, 1995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26.Churchland, PM. The Engine of Reason the Seat of the Soul. A philosophical Journey into the Brain. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1995.Google Scholar
27.Coolen, M. De Machine voorbij; over het Zelfbegrip van de Mens in het Tijdperk van de Informatietechniek. Meppel: Boom, 1992.Google Scholar
28.Draaisma, D. De Metaforenmachine; een Geschiedenis van het Geheugen. Groningen: Historische Uitgeverij, 1995.Google Scholar
29.Kolk, B van der. The Body keeps the Score; the evolving psychobiology of posttraumatic stress-disorder. In: Westenberg, HGM, Den Boer, JA, Murphy, DL, eds. Advances in the Neurobiology of Anxiety Disorders. Chichester: Wiley 1996: 361–82.Google Scholar
30.Dijken, HH van. Once is enough; an animal Study on temporal Aspects of stress-induced behavioral and neuroendocrine Changes. Monnickendam: Gravo groep, 1992.Google Scholar
31.Gould, SJ. De gok van de evolutie. Misvattingen over Evolutie, Vooruitgang en Honkbal. Amsterdam: Contact, 1996.Google Scholar
32.Gould, SJ. De Mens gemeten. De Geschiedenis van de Intelligentietest. Amsterdam: Contact, 1996.Google Scholar
33.Leaky, R, Lewin, R. Onze Oorsprong. Een Speurtocht naar het Ontstaan van de Mensheid. Amsterdam: Contact, 1993.Google Scholar
34.Dennett, D. Darwins gevaarlijke idee. Amsterdam: Contact, 1995.Google Scholar
35.Penrose, R. Shadows of the Mind. A Search for the missing Science of Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press, 1994.Google Scholar
36.Edelman, GM. Neural Darwinism; the Theory of neural Group Selection. New York: Basic Books, 1987.Google Scholar
37.Edelman, GM. Topobiology, an Introduction to molecular Embryology. New York: Basic Books, 1988.Google Scholar
38.Edelman, GM. The remembered Present; a biological Theory of Consciousness. New York: Basic Books, 1989.Google Scholar
39.Edelman, GM. Klare Lucht, louter Vuur; over de stoffelijke Oorsprong van Denken en Bewustzijn. Amsterdam: Bert Bakker, 1993.Google Scholar
40.Mehler, J, Jusczyk, PW, Lambertz, G. A precursor to language development in young infants. Cognition 1988; 29: 143–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41.Friston, KJ, Tononi, G, Reeke, GN, Sporns, O, Edelman, GM. Value-dependent selection in the brain: simulation in a synthetic neural model. Neurosci 1994; 59: 229–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
42.Wray, J, Edelman, GR. A model of color vision based on cortical reentry. Cereb Cortex 1996; 6: 701–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
43.Rucci, M, Tononi, G, Edelman, GM. Registration of neural maps through value dependent learning: modelling the alignment of auditory and visual maps in the barn owl's optic tectum. J Neurosci 1997; 17: 334–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44.Povinelli, DJ, Preuss, TM. Theory of mind: evolutionary history of a cognitive specialization. Trends Neurosci 1995; 18:418–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Nitecki, MH, Nitecki, DB, eds. Evolutionary Ethics. New York: The Free Press, 1993.Google Scholar
46.Waal, F de. Van Nature Goed. Over de Oorsprong van Goed en Kwaad bij Mensen en andere Dieren. Amsterdam: Contact, 1996.Google Scholar
47.Wilson, JO. The moral Sense. New York: The Free Press, 1993.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
48.Wright, R. The moral Animal; Evolutionary Psychology and everyday Life. New York: Pantheon Books, 1994.Google Scholar
49.Sober, E. From a biological Point of View; Essays in evolutionary Philosophy. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press, 1994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
50.Hundert, EM. Lessons from an optical Illusion. On Nature and Nurture, Knowledge and Values. Cambridge: Harvard Univ Press, 1995.Google Scholar
51.Eisenberg, L. The social construction of the human brain. Am J Psychiat 1995; 152: 1563–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52.Ross, C, Pam, A. Pseudoscience in biological Psychiatry; Blaming the Body. New York: Wiley, 1995.Google Scholar
53.Hoofdakker, RH van den. De biologie van het geluk. Maandblad voor Geestelijke Gezondheidszorg 1996: 227–44.Google Scholar
54.Buytendijk, FJJ. Prolegomena van een antropologische fysiologie. Utrecht/Antwerpen: Aula-boeken, Het Spectrum, 1965.Google Scholar
55.Elbert, T, Pantev, C, Wienbruch, C, Rockstroh, B, Taub, E. Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science 1995; 270:305–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
56.Lewis, B. Psychotherapy, neuroscience, and philosophy of mind. Am J Psychotherapy 1994; 48: 8593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57.Kortmann, FAM. Over het raadsel van lichaam en geest en de invloed ervan op de hedendaagse psychiatric Tijdschr Psychiat 1997; ter perse.Google Scholar
58.Gillett, GR. Neuropsychology and meaning in psychiatry. J med Philosophy 1990; 15: 2139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
59.Bolton, D, Hill, J. Mind, Meaning and mental Disorder. The Nature of causal Explanation in Psychiatry and Psychology. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press, 1996.Google Scholar