Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T07:56:51.649Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bilingual Development in Childhood

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 April 2021

Annick De Houwer
Affiliation:
Harmonious Bilingualism Network

Summary

In the first decade of life, children become bilingual in different language learning environments. Many children start learning two languages from birth (Bilingual First Language Acquisition). In early childhood hitherto monolingual children start hearing a second language through daycare or preschool (Early Second Language Acquisition). Yet other hitherto monolingual children in middle childhood may acquire a second language only after entering school (Second Language Acquisition). This Element explains how these different language learning settings dynamically affect bilingual children's language learning trajectories. All children eventually learn to speak the societal language, but they often do not learn to fluently speak their non-societal language and may even stop speaking it. Children's and families' harmonious bilingualism is threatened if bilingual children do not develop high proficiency in both languages. Educational institutions and parental conversational practices play a pivotal role in supporting harmonious bilingual development.
Get access
Type
Element
Information
Online ISBN: 9781108866002
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication: 13 May 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aarssen, J. (1996). Relating events in two languages: Acquisition of cohesive devices by Turkish-Dutch bilingual children at school age. Tilburg: Tilburg University Press.Google Scholar
Águila, E., Ramon-Casas, M., Pons, F., & Bosch, L. (2007). La medida del léxico productivo inicial: Aplicación de un cuestionario para la población bilingüe. In Diez-Itza, E. (Ed.), Estudios de desarrollo del lenguaje y educación (Vol. 32). Oviedo, Spain: ICE Monografias de Aula Abierta, 163–71.Google Scholar
Ahrenholz, B., Hövelbrinks, B., Maak, D., & Zippel, W. (2013). Mehrsprachigkeit an Thüringer Schulen (MaTS) – Ergebnisse einer Fragebogenerhebung zu Mehrsprachigkeit an Erfurter Schulen. In Dirim, İ. & Oomen-Welke, I. (Eds.), Mehrsprachigkeit in der Klasse: Wahrnehmen – Aufgreifen – Fördern. Stuttgart: Fillibach bei Klett, 4358.Google Scholar
Andruski, J. E., Casielles, E., & Nathan, G. (2014). Is bilingual babbling language-specific? Some evidence from a case study of Spanish-English dual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 660–72. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000655Google Scholar
Argyri, E., & Sorace, A. (2007). Crosslinguistic influence and language dominance in older bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 7999. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728906002835Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., & Meir, N. (2019). The nature of exposure and input in early bilingualism. In De Houwer, A. & Ortega, L. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 193212. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/9781316831922.011Google Scholar
Autorengruppe Bildungsberichterstattung. (2018). Bildung in Deutschland 2018. Ein indikatorengestützter Bericht mit einer Analyse zu Wirkungen und Erträgen von Bildung. Bielefeld: wby Media. doi: http://doi.org/10.3278/6001820fwGoogle Scholar
Babatsouli, E., & Ball, M. J. (Eds.) (2020). An anthology of bilingual child phonology. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. doi: http://doi.org/10.21832/9781788928427Google Scholar
Baralt, M., Darcy-Mahoney, A., & Brito, N. H. (2020). Háblame Bebé: A phone application intervention to support Hispanic children’s early language environments and bilingualism. Child Language Teaching & Therapy, 36 (1), 3357. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0265659020903779CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayram, F., Miller, D., Rothman, J., & Serratrice, L. (2018). Studies in bilingualism: 25 years in the making. In Miller, D., Bayram, F., Rothman, J., & Serratrice, L. (Eds.), Bilingual cognition and language: The state of the science across its subfields. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 112. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.54.01bayGoogle Scholar
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. PNAS, 109(9), 32533258. doi: http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113380109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhatia, T. K., & Ritchie, W. C. (2013). The handbook of bilingualism and multilingualism (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell. doi: http://doi.org/10.1002/9781118332382Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 525–31. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990423CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bijeljac-Babic, R., Höhle, B., & Nazzi, T. (2016). Early prosodic acquisition in bilingual infants: The case of the perceptual trochaic bias. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 210. doi: http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00210CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blom, E. (2010). Effects of input on the early grammatical development of bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 14(4), 422446. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/1367006910370917CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blom, E., & Paradis, J. (2015). Sources of individual differences in the acquisition of tense inflection by English second language learners with and without specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 953976. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641300057XGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2009). Toward a model of culture↔parent↔child transactions. In Sameroff, A. (Ed.), The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 139161. doi: http://doi.org/10.1037/11877-008Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2019). A developmentalist’s viewpoint: “It’s about time!” Ecological systems, transaction, and specificity as key developmental principles in children’s changing worlds. In Parke, R. D. & Elder, G. H. Jr. (Eds.), Children in changing worlds. Sociocultural and temporal perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 277286. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/9781108264846.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. & Haynes, O. M. (1998). Vocabulary competence in early childhood: Measurement, latent construct, and predictive validity. Child Development, 69(3), 654671. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06235.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L., Figueras, F., Teixidó, M., & Ramon-Casas, M. (2013). Rapid gains in segmenting fluent speech when words match the rhythmic unit: Evidence from infants acquiring syllable-timed languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 106. doi: http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00106Google Scholar
Bosch, L., & Ramon-Casas, M. (2011). Variability in vowel production by bilingual speakers: Can input properties hinder the early stabilization of contrastive categories? Journal of Phonetics, 39, 514526. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.02.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brojde, C. L., Ahmed, S., & Colunga, E. (2012). Bilingual and monolingual children attend to different cues when learning new words. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 155. doi: http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00155Google Scholar
Budde-Spengler, N., Sachse, S., & Rinker, T. (2018). The relationship between input factors and early lexical knowledge in Turkish–German children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2018.1543647CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burchinal, M., Vernon-Feagans, L., & Cox, M. (2008). Cumulative social risk, parenting, and infant development in rural low-income communities. Parenting: Science and Practice, 8, 4169. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/15295190701830672Google Scholar
Burns, T., Yoshida, K., Hill, K., & Werker, J. T. (2007). The development of phonetic representation in bilingual and monolingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 455474. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070257Google Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K. (2017). Bilingualism affects 9-month-old infants’ expectations about how words refer to kinds. Developmental Science, 20, e12486. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12486CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byers-Heinlein, K., Tsui, A., Bergmann, C., Black, A. K., Brown, A., Carbajal, M. J., … Wermelinger, S. (2021). A multi-lab study of bilingual infants: Exploring the preference for infant-directed speech. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 4(1), 130 [Registered report]. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245920974622Google Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Burns, T. C., & Werker, J. F. (2010). The roots of bilingualism in newborns. Psychological Science, 21(3), 343348. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609360758Google Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Esposito, A. G., Winsler, A., Marian, V., Castro‖, D. C., & Luk, G. (2019). The case for measuring and reporting bilingualism in developmental research. Collabra: Psychology, 5(1), 37. doi: http://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.233Google Scholar
Cantone, K. F. (2007). Code-switching in bilingual children. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer. doi: http://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-5784-7Google Scholar
Carbajal, M. J., & Peperkamp, S. (2020). Dual language input and the impact of language separation on early lexical development. Infancy, 25(1), 2245. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12315Google Scholar
Chang, F., Crawford, G., Early, D., Bryant, D., Howes, C., Burchinal, M., Barbarin, O., Clifford, R., & Pianta, R. (2007). Spanish-speaking children’s social and language development in pre-kindergarten classrooms. Early Education and Development, 18(2), 243–69. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/10409280701282959Google Scholar
Chevalier, S. (2015). Trilingual language acquisition: Contextual factors influencing active trilingualism in early childhood. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.16Google Scholar
Chevrot, J.-P., & Ghimenton, A. (2019). Bilingualism and bidialectalism. In De Houwer, A. & Ortega, L. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 510–23. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/9781316831922.026Google Scholar
Chilla, S., & Niebuhr-Siebert, S. (2017). Mehrsprachigkeit in der KiTa. Grundlagen – Konzepte – Bildung. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
Chondrogianni, V., & Marinis, T. (2011). Differential effects of internal and external factors on the development of vocabulary, morphology and complex syntax in successive bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 223248. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.05choCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chondrogianni, V, & Schwartz, R. G. (2020). Case and word order in Greek heritage children. Journal of Child Language, 47, 766795. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000849CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chumak-Horbatsch, R. (2012). Linguistically Appropriate Practice: A guide for working with young immigrant children. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511554377Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (2003). Critical periods, time, and practice. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 9(2), 3948.Google Scholar
Collier, V., & Thomas, W. P. (2017). Validating the power of bilingual schooling: Thirty-two years of large-scale, longitudinal research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 37, 203–17. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190517000034Google Scholar
Collins, B., & Toppelberg, C. (2021). The role of socioeconomic and sociocultural predictors of Spanish and English proficiencies of young Latino children of immigrants. Journal of Child Language, 48(1), 129156. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000920000203Google Scholar
Comeau, L., & Genesee, F. (2001). Bilingual children’s repair strategies during dyadic communication. In Cenoz, J. & Genesee, F. (Eds.), Trends in bilingual acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 231256. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.1.11comGoogle Scholar
Conboy, B. T., & Thal, D. (2006). Ties between the lexicon and grammar: Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies of bilingual toddlers. Child Development, 77, 712735. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00899.xGoogle Scholar
Cote, L. R., & Bornstein, M. H. (2014). Productive vocabulary among three groups of bilingual American children: Comparison and prediction. First Language, 34, 467485. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0142723714560178Google Scholar
Cruz-Ferreira, M. (2006). Three is a crowd? Acquiring Portuguese in a trilingual environment. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. doi: http://doi.org/10.21832/9781853598395Google Scholar
Czinglar, C., Rüdiger, J. O., Korecky-Kröll, K., Uzunkaya-Sharma, K., & Dressler, W. U. (2017). Inputfaktoren im DaZ-Erwerb von sukzessiv bilingualen Kindern mit L1 Türkisch. In Fuchs, I., Jeuk, S., & Knapp, W. (Eds.), Mehrsprachigkeit: Spracherwerb, Unterrichtsprozesse, Seiteneinstieg: Beiträge aus dem 11. Workshop “Kinder und Jugendliche mit Migrationshintergrund.” Stuttgart: Fillibach bei Klett, 1534.Google Scholar
Dahoun, Z. K. S. (1995). Les couleurs du silence. Le mutisme des enfants de migrants. Paris: Calmann-Lévy.Google Scholar
David, A., & Li, W. (2008). Individual differences in the lexical development of French–English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism and Bilingual Education, 11(5), 598618. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/13670050802149200Google Scholar
Davidson, D., Jergovic, D., Imami, Z., & Theodos, V. (1997). Monolingual and bilingual children’s use of the mutual exclusivity constraint. Journal of Child Language, 24, 324. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000996002917Google Scholar
DeAnda, S., Arias-Trejo, N., Poulin-Dubois, D., Zesiger, P., & Friend, M. (2016a). Minimal second language exposure, SES, and early word comprehension: New evidence from a direct assessment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 162180. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000820Google Scholar
DeAnda, S., Poulin-Dubois, D., Zesiger, P., & Friend, M. (2016b). Lexical processing and organization in bilingual first language acquisition: Guiding future research. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 655667. doi: http://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000042Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (1990). The acquisition of two languages from birth: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511519789CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2003). Home languages spoken in officially monolingual Flanders: A survey. Plurilingua, 24, 7996.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2007). Parental language input patterns and children’s bilingual use. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(3), 411424. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2009). Bilingual First Language Acquisition. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. doi: http://doi.org/10.21832/9781847691507Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2014). The absolute frequency of maternal input to bilingual and monolingual children: A first comparison. In Grüter, T. & Paradis, J. (Eds.), Input and experience in bilingual development. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 3758. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.13.03dehGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2015a). Early foreign language teaching: Some critical remarks and some recommendations. Babylonia, 01(14), 1421.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2015b). Integration und Interkulturalität in Kindertagesstätten und in Kindergärten: Die Rolle der Nichtumgebungssprache für das Wohlbefinden von Kleinkindern. In Reichert-Garschhammer, E., Kieferle, C., Wertfein, M., & Becker-Stoll, F. (Eds.), Inklusion und Partizipation. Vielfalt als Chance und Anspruch. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht, 113125.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2017a). Bilingual language input environments, intake, maturity and practice. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 1920. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000298Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2017b). Early multilingualism and language awareness. In Cenoz, J., Gorter, D. & May, S. (Eds.), Language awareness and multilingualism. Encyclopedia of language and education (3rd ed.). Cham: Springer, 8397. doi: http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-02240-6_6Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2017c). Minority language parenting in Europe and children’s well-being. In Cabrera, N. & Leyendecker, B. (Eds.), Handbook on positive development of minority children and youth. Cham: Springer, 231246. doi: http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-43645-6_14Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2018a). Input, context and early child bilingualism: Implications for clinical practice. In Bar-On, A. & Ravid, Dorit (Eds.), Handbook of communication disorders: Theoretical, empirical, and applied linguistic perspectives. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, 599616. doi: http://doi.org/10.1515/9781614514909-030Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2018b). The role of language input environments for language outcomes and language acquisition in young bilingual children. In Miller, D., Bayram, F., Rothman, J., & Serratrice, L. (Eds.), Bilingual cognition and language: The state of the science across its subfields. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 127153. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.54.07houCrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2019a). Equitable evaluation of bilingual children’s’s language knowledge using the CDI: It really matters who you ask. Journal of Monolingual and Bilingual Speech, 1(1), 3254. doi: http://doi.org/10.1558/jmbs.11184Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2019b). Language choice in bilingual interaction. In De Houwer, A. & Ortega, L. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 324348. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/9781316831922.018Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2019c). Uninstructed language acquisition in multiple language learners. In Darquennes, J., Salmons, J., & Vandenbussche, W. (Eds.), Language contact: An international handbook. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 183196. doi: http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110435351-016Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2020a). Harmonious Bilingualism: Well-being for families in bilingual settings. In Eisenchlas, S. & Schalley, A. (Eds.), Handbook of home language maintenance and development. Social and affective factors. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 6383. doi: http://doi.org/10.1515/9781501510175-004Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (2020b). Why do so many children who hear two languages speak just a single language? Zeitschrift für Interkulturellen Fremdsprachenunterricht, 25(1), 726.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A., & Bornstein, M. H. (2016a). Balance patterns in early bilingual acquisition: A longitudinal study of word comprehension and production. In Silva-Corvalán, C. & Treffers-Daller, J. (Eds.), Language dominance in bilinguals. Issues of measurement and operationalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 134155. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107375345.007Google Scholar
De Houwer, A., & Bornstein, M. H. (2016b). Bilingual mothers’ language choice in child-directed speech: Continuity and change. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 37(7), 680693. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2015.1127929Google Scholar
De Houwer, A., Bornstein, M. H., & De Coster, S. (2006). Early understanding of two words for the same thing: A CDI study of lexical comprehension in infant bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 10, 331347. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/13670069060100030401Google Scholar
De Houwer, A., Bornstein, M. H., & Putnick, D. L. (2014). A bilingual-monolingual comparison of young children’s vocabulary size: Evidence from comprehension and production. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 11891211. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000744Google Scholar
De Houwer, A., & Nakamura, J. (accepted for publication 2021). Developmental perspectives on parents’ use of discourse strategies with bilingual children. Routledge.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A., & Ortega, L. (2019). Introduction: Learning, using, and unlearning more than one language. In De Houwer, A. & Ortega, L. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 112. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/9781316831922.001Google Scholar
Dicataldo, R., & Roch, M. (2020). Are the effects of variation in quantity of daily bilingual exposure and socioeconomic status on language and cognitive abilities independent in preschool children? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17, 4570. doi: http://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17124570Google Scholar
Drury, R. (2007). Young bilingual learners at home and school: Researching multilingual voices. Stoke on Trent: Trentham Books.Google Scholar
Dubiel, B., & Guilfoyle, E. (2017). Language strength in bilingual children: The Child HALA test. Heritage Language Journal, 14(1), 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Durrant, S., Delle Luche, C., Cattani, A., & Floccia, C. (2014). Monodialectal and multidialectal infants’ representation of familiar words. Journal of Child Language, 42, 447465. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000914000063Google Scholar
Eilers, R. E., Oller, D. K., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (2002). Bilingualism and cultural assimilation in Miami Hispanic children. In Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters, 4363.Google Scholar
Eilers, R. E., Pearson, B. Z., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (2006). Social factors in bilingual development: The Miami experience. In McCardle, P. & Hoff, E. (Eds.), Childhood bilingualism: Research on infancy through school age. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters, 6890.Google Scholar
Ertanir, B., Kratzmann, J., Frank, M., Jahreiss, S., & Sachse, S. (2018). Dual language competencies of Turkish–German children growing up in Germany: Factors supportive of functioning dual language development. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(2261). doi: http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02261Google Scholar
Ertanir, B., Kratzmann, J., & Sachse, S. (2019). Sozio-emotionale Kompetenzen mehrsprachiger Kindergartenkinder und deren Wechselwirkungen mit den Sprachleistungen im Deutschen. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 51(1), 3144. doi: http://doi.org/10.1026/0049-8637/a000207Google Scholar
Fennell, C. T., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2014). You sound like Mommy: Bilingual and monolingual infants learn words best from speakers typical of their language environments. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 309316. doi: http://doi.org/10.1026/0049-8637/a00020710.1177/0165025414530631Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., Pethick, S. J., & Reilly, J. (1993). MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Fish, M. S., García-Sierra, A., Ramírez-Esparza, N., & Kuhl, P. K. (2017). Infant-directed speech in English and Spanish: Assessments of monolingual and bilingual caregiver VOT. Journal of Phonetics, 63, 1934. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2017.04.003Google Scholar
Fuller, J. M. (2012). Bilingual pre-teens: Competing ideologies and multiple identities in the U.S. and Germany. New York, NY: Routledge. doi: http://doi.org/10.4324/9780203110645Google Scholar
Fuller, J. M. (2019). Ideologies of language, bilingualism, and monolingualism. In De Houwer, A. & Ortega, L. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 119134. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/9781316831922.007Google Scholar
Gagarina, N., & Klassert, A. (2018). Input dominance and development of home language in Russian-German bilinguals. Frontiers in Communication, 3(40). doi: http://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2018.00040Google Scholar
Gampe, A., Hartmann, L., & Daum, M. M. (2020). Dynamic interaction patterns of monolingual and bilingual infants with their parents. Journal of Child Language, 47(1), 4563. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000631Google Scholar
Gampe, A., Wermelinger, S., & Daum, M. M. (2019). Bilingual children adapt to the needs of their communication partners, monolinguals do not. Child Development, 90(1), 98107. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13190Google Scholar
Garcia-Sierra, A., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Percaccio, C. R., Conboy, B. T., Romo, H., Klarman, L., & Kuhl, P. K. (2011). Bilingual language learning: An ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, language input, and later word production. Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 546557. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.07.002Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M. (Ed.) (2007). Language transmission in bilingual families in Wales. Cardiff: Welsh Language Board.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., & Thomas, E. M. (2009). Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 213237. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909004015Google Scholar
Gatt, D., Baldacchino, R., & Dodd, B. (2020). Which measure of socioeconomic status best predicts bilingual lexical abilities and how? A focus on four-year-olds exposed to two majority languages. Journal of Child Language, 47(4), 737765. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000886Google Scholar
Genesee, F., Boivin, I., & Nicoladis, E. (1996). Talking with strangers: A study of bilingual children’s communicative competence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 427442. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400008183Google Scholar
Genesee, F., & Delcenserie, A. (Eds.) (2016). Starting over – The language development in internationally-adopted children. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.18Google Scholar
Gibson, T. A., Oller, D. K., Jarmulowicz, L., & Ethington, C. A. (2012). The receptive-expressive gap in the vocabulary of young second-language learners: Robustness and possible mechanisms. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 102116. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728910000490Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Starr, J., & Ferreira, V. S. (2015). More than use it or lose it: The number-of-speakers effect on heritage language proficiency. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22(1), 147–55. doi: http://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0649-7Google Scholar
Granfeldt, J., Schlyter, J., & Kihlstedt, M. (2007). French as cL2, 2L1 and L1 in pre-school children. In Granfeldt, J. (Ed.), Studies in Romance bilingual acquisition – age of onset and development of French and Spanish. Lund: Lund University, 742.Google Scholar
Groba, A., De Houwer, A., Obrig, H., & Rossi, S. (2019). Bilingual and monolingual first language acquisition experience differentially shapes children’s property term learning: Evidence from behavioral and neurophysiological measures. Brain Sciences, 9(2), 40. doi: http://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9020040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosjean, F. (1989). Neurolinguists beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language, 36, 315. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(89)90048-5Google Scholar
Grosjean, F., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2018). The listening bilingual: Speech perception, comprehension, and bilingualism. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. doi: http://doi.org/10.1002/9781118835722Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Kreiter, J. (2003). Understanding child bilingual acquisition using parent and teacher reports. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 267288. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716403000158Google Scholar
Haberzettl, S., Dimroth, C., Wulff, N., & Czinglar, C. (2013). Erwerb des Deutschen als Zweitsprache im Grundschulalter. In Berndt, A. (Ed.), Fremdsprachen in der Perspektive lebenslangen Lernens. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang, 143161.Google Scholar
Haman, E., Łuniewska, M., & Pomiechowska, B. (2015). Designing Cross-Linguistic Lexical Tasks (CLTs) for bilingual preschool children. In Armon-Lotem, S., de Jong, J., & Meir, M. (Eds.), Methods for assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 194238. doi: http://doi.org/10.21832/9781783093137-010Google Scholar
Hammer, C. S., Komaroff, E., Rodriguez, B. L., Lopez, L. M., Scarpino, S. E., & Goldstein, B. (2012). Predicting Spanish-English bilingual children’s language abilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(5), 12511264. doi: http://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0016)Google Scholar
Han, W.-J. (2010). Bilingualism and socioemotional well-being. Children and Youth Services Review, 32, 720–31. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.01.009Google Scholar
Han, W.-J., & Huang, C.-C. (2010). The forgotten treasure: Bilingualism and Asian children’s emotional and behavioral health. American Journal of Public Health, 100(5), 831838. doi: http://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2009.174219Google Scholar
Hatch, E. M. (Ed.) (1978). Second language acquisition: A book of readings. Rowley, MA: Newbury House Publishers.Google Scholar
Havy, M., Bouchon, C., & Nazzi, T. (2016). Phonetic processing when learning words: The case of bilingual infants. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40, 4152. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415570646Google Scholar
Haznedar, B. (1997). L2 acquisition by a Turkish-speaking child: Evidence for L1 influence. In Hughes, E., Hughes, M., & Greenhill, A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st annual Boston University conference on language development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, 245–56.Google Scholar
Head Zauche, L., Darcy Mahoney, A., Thul, T., Zauche, M., Weldon, A., & Stapel-Wax, J. (2017). The power of language nutrition for children’s brain development, health, and future academic achievement. Journal of Pediatric Health Care 31: 493503. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedhc.2017.01.007Google Scholar
Henderson, A. M. E., & Scott, J. C. (2015). She called that thing a mido, but should you call it a mido too? Linguistic experience influences infants’ expectations of conventionality. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 332. doi: http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00332Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39, 127. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000910000759Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., & Shanks, K. F. (2020). The quality of child-directed speech depends on the speaker’s language proficiency. Journal of Child Language, 47, 132145. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S030500091900028XGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., & Ribot, K. M. (2017). Language growth in English monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual children from 2.5 to 5 years. Journal of Pediatrics, 190, 241245.e1. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.06.071Google Scholar
Höhle, B., Bijeljac-Babic, R., & Nazzi, T. (2020). Variability and stability in early language acquisition: Comparing monolingual and bilingual infants” speech perception and word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23, 5671. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000348Google Scholar
Holowka, S., Brosseau-Lapré, F., & Petitto, L. A. (2002). Semantic and conceptual knowledge underlying bilingual babies’ first signs and words. Language Learning, 52, 205–62. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/0023-8333.00184Google Scholar
Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2014). Relative language exposure, processing efficiency and vocabulary in Spanish–English bilingual toddlers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(01), 189202. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891300014XGoogle Scholar
Idiazábal, I. (1984). Conciencia bilingüe del niño bilingüe. In Siguan, M. (Ed.), Adquisición precoz de una segunda lengua. Barcelona: Publicacions i edicions de la universitat de Barcelona, 5563.Google Scholar
Itani-Adams, Y., Iwasaki, J., & Kawaguchi, S. (2017). Similarities and differences between simultaneous and successive bilingual children: Acquisition of Japanese morphology. International Journal of Applied Linguistics & English Literature, 6(7), 268276. doi: http://doi.org/10.7575/aiac.ijalel.v.6n.7p.268Google Scholar
Itoh, H., & Hatch, E. (1978). Second Language Acquisition: A case study. In Hatch, E. (Ed.), Second Language Acquisition: A book of readings. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, 7788.Google Scholar
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Marchman, V., Bates, E., & Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. (1993). Early lexical development in Spanish-speaking infants and toddlers. Journal of Child Language, 20, 523549. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900008461Google Scholar
Jahreiß, S., Ertanir, B., Sachse, S., & Kratzmann, J. (2018). Sprachliche Interaktionen in Kindertageseinrichtungen mit hohem Anteil an mehrsprachigen Kindern. Forschung Sprache, 2, 3241.Google Scholar
Jardak, A., & Byers-Heinlein, B. (2019). Labels or concepts? The development of semantic networks in bilingual two-year-olds. Child Development, 90(2), e212e229. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13050Google Scholar
Jia, G., Chen, J., Kim, H. Y., Chan, P.-S., & Jeung, C. (2014). Bilingual lexical skills of school-age children with Chinese and Korean heritage languages in the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 350358. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0165025414533224Google Scholar
Juan-Garau, M., & Lyster, R. (2019). Becoming bilingual through additive immersive programs. In De Houwer, A. & Ortega, L. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 213232. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/9781316831922.012Google Scholar
Kalashnikova, M., Escudero, P., & Kidd, E. (2018). The development of fast-mapping and novel word retention strategies in monolingual and bilingual infants. Developmental Science, 21(6), e12674. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12674Google Scholar
Kan, P. F., & Kohnert, K. (2005). Preschoolers learning Hmong and English: Lexical-semantic skills in L1 and L2. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 48(2), 372383. doi: http://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/026)Google Scholar
Karlsen, J., Halaas Lyster, S.-A., & Lervåg, A. (2017). Vocabulary development in Norwegian L1 and L2 learners in the kindergarten–school transition. Journal of Child Language, 44(2), 402426. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000106Google Scholar
Karrebaek, M. S. (2013). “Don’t speak like that to her!”: Linguistic minority children’s socialization into an ideology of monolingualism. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 17(3), 355375. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/josl.12035Google Scholar
Kaufman, D. (2001). Tales of L1 attrition – Evidence from pre-puberty children. In Ammerlaan, T., Hulsen, M., Strating, H. & Yağmur, K. (Eds.), Sociolinguistic and psycholinguistic perspectives on maintenance and loss of minority languages. Münster: Waxmann, 185202.Google Scholar
Kaufman, D., & Aronoff, M. (1991). Morphological disintegration and reconstruction in first language attrition. In Seliger, H. & Vago, R. (Eds.), First language attrition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 175–88. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620720.012Google Scholar
Keller, K., Troesch, L., & Grob, A. (2013). Shyness as a risk factor for local language skills of immigrant preschoolers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 34, 328335. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2013.07.001Google Scholar
Kibler, A. K., Palacios, N., & Simpson Baird, A. (2014). The influence of older siblings on language use among second generation Latino preschoolers. TESOL Quarterly, 48, 164175. doi: http://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.151Google Scholar
Kim, Y. K., Curby, T. W., & Winsler, A. (2014). Child, family, and school characteristics related to English proficiency development among low-income, dual language learners. Developmental Psychology, 50(12), 26002613. doi: http://doi.org/10.1037/a0038050Google Scholar
Kim, Y. K., Hutchison, L. A., & Winsler, A. (2015). Bilingual education in the United States: An historical overview and examination of two-way immersion. Educational Review, 67(2), 236252. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/00131911.2013.865593CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köppe, R. (1996). Language differentiation in bilingual children: The development of grammatical and pragmatic competence. Linguistics, 34, 927954. doi: http://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1996.34.5.927Google Scholar
Korecky-Kröll, K., Czinglar, C., Uzunkaya-Sharma, K., Sommer-Lolei, S., & Dressler, W. U. (2016). Das INPUT-Projekt: Wortschatz-und Grammatikerwerb von ein-und zweisprachigen Wiener Kindergartenkindern. logoTHEMA, 13(1), 1622.Google Scholar
Korecky-Kröll, K., Dobek, N., Blaschitz, V., Sommer-Lolei, S., Boniecki, M., Uzunkaya-Sharma, K., & Dressler, W. U. (2019). Vocabulary as a central link between phonological working memory and narrative competence: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual 4-year olds from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Language and Speech, 62, 546569. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0023830918796691Google Scholar
Kuo, E. C.-Y. (1974). The family and bilingual socialization: A sociolinguistic study of a sample of Chinese children in the United States. The Journal of Social Psychology, 92, 181191.Google Scholar
Lanza, E. (1992). Can bilingual two-year-olds code-switch? Journal of Child Language, 19(3), 633658. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900011600Google Scholar
Lauro, J., Core, C., & Hoff, E. (2020). Explaining individual differences in trajectories of simultaneous bilingual development: Contributions of child and environmental factors. Child Development, 91(6), 20632082. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13409Google Scholar
Legacy, J., Zesiger, P., Friend, M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2016). Vocabulary size, translation equivalents, and efficiency in word recognition in very young bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 43(4), 760783. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000252Google Scholar
Legacy, J., Zesiger, P., Friend, M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2018). Vocabulary size and speed of word recognition in very young French–English bilinguals: A longitudinal study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(1), 137149. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000833Google Scholar
Leist-Villis, A. (2004). Zweisprachigkeit im Kontext sozialer Netzwerke. Unterstützende Rahmenbedingungen zweisprachiger Entwicklung und Erziehung am Beispiel griechisch-deutsch. Münster: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. (1939–1949). Speech development of a bilingual child: A linguist’s record (4 volumes). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Levine, D., Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., de Villiers, J., Iglesias, A., & Wilson, M. S. (2020). Evaluating socioeconomic gaps in preschoolers’ vocabulary, syntax, and language process skills with the Quick Interactive Language Screener (QUILS). Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 114128. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.11.006Google Scholar
Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2017). Exposure to multiple languages enhances communication skills in infancy. Developmental Science, 20, e12420. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12420Google Scholar
Linberg, T., Schneider, T., Waldfogel, J., & Wang, Y. (2019). Socioeconomic status gaps in child cognitive development in Germany and the United States. Social Science Research, 79, 131. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2018.11.002Google Scholar
Lindquist, H., & Gram Garmann, N. (2021). Toddlers and their translingual practicing homes. International Journal of Multilingualism, 18(1), 5972. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/14790718.2019.1604712Google Scholar
Liu, L., & Kager, R. (2017). Perception of tones by bilingual infants learning non-tone languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 561575. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000183Google Scholar
Li, Wei. (2011). The early acquisition of English as a second language: The case of young Chinese learners of English in Britain. In De Houwer, A. & Wilton, A. (Eds.), English in Europe today: Educational and sociocultural perspectives. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 105122. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/aals.8.07weiGoogle Scholar
Luo, R., Escobar, K., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2020). Heterogeneity in the trajectories of US Latine mothers” dual-language input from infancy to preschool. First Language, 40(3), 275–99. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0142723720915401Google Scholar
MacLeod, A. A. N., Fabiano-Smith, L., Boegner-Pagé, S., & Fontolliet, S. (2013). Simultaneous bilingual language acquisition: The role of parental input on receptive vocabulary development. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 29, 131142. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0265659012466862Google Scholar
MacSwan, J., & Pray, L. (2005). Learning English bilingually: Age of onset of exposure and rate of acquisition among English language learners in a bilingual education program. Bilingual Research Journal, 29(3), 653678. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/15235882.2005.10162857Google Scholar
Mancilla‐Martinez, J., & Lesaux, N. (2011). The gap between Spanish speakers” word reading and word knowledge: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 82(5),15441560. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01633.xGoogle Scholar
Maneva, B., & Genesee, F. (2002). Bilingual babbling: Evidence for language differentiation in dual language acquisition. In Skarbela, B., Fish, S., & Do, A H.-J (Eds.), Boston University Conference on Language Development 26 Proceedings. Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, 383392.Google Scholar
Manigand, A. (1999). Le silence des enfants turcs à l’école. Psychologie & Éducation, 37, 5773.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Bermúdez, V. N., Bang, J. Y., & Fernald, A. (2020). Off to a good start: Early Spanish‐language processing efficiency supports Spanish‐and English‐language outcomes at 4½ years in sequential bilinguals. Developmental Science, 23(6) e12973. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12973Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Fernald, A., & Hurtado, N. (2010). How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young Spanish–English bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 37(04), 817840. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000909990055Google Scholar
Marchman, V., Martínez, L., Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., & Fernald, A. (2017). Caregiver talk to young Spanish-English bilinguals: Comparing direct observation and parent-report measures of dual-language exposure. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12425. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12425Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Martínez-Sussmann, C., & Dale, P. S. (2004). The language-specific nature of grammatical development: Evidence from bilingual language learners. Developmental Science, 7, 212224. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00340.xGoogle Scholar
Marecka, M., Wrembel, M., Otwinowska, A., Szewczyk, J., Banasik-Jemielniak, N., & Wodniecka, Z. (2020). Bilingual children’s phonology shows evidence of transfer, but not deceleration in their L1. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 42(1), 89114. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263119000408Google Scholar
Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 121157. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5Google Scholar
Marinis, T., Armon-Lotem, S., & Pontikas, G. (Eds.) (2017). Language impairment in bilingual children: State of the art 2017. Special Issue. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7 (3–4). doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.7.3-4Google Scholar
Mayr, R., & Montanari, S. (2015). Cross-linguistic interaction in trilingual phonological development: The role of the input in the acquisition of the voicing contrast. Journal of Child Language, 42(5), 10061035. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000914000592Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (1989). Early differentiation of languages in bilingual children. In Hyltenstam, K. & Obler, L. K. (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan: Aspects of acquisition, maturity and loss. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1340. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511611780.003Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2008). Âge du début de l’acquisition successive du bilinguisme. Effets sur le développement grammatical. In Kail, M., Fayol, M., & Hickman, M. (Eds.), Apprentissage des langues premières et secondes. Paris: Editions du CNRS, 245272.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M., Elsig, M., & Bonnesen, M. (2011). Delayed grammatical acquisition in first language development: Subject-verb inversion and subject clitics in French interrogatives. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(4), 347390. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.4.01meiGoogle Scholar
Mirvahedi, S. H., & Cavallaro, F. (2020). Siblings’ play and language shift to English in a Malay-English bilingual family. World Englishes, 39(1), 183197. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/weng.12417Google Scholar
Mishina, S. (1999). The role of parental input and discourse strategies in the early language mixing of a bilingual child. Multilingua, 18(4), 343367. doi: http://doi.org/10.1515/mult.1999.18.4.317Google Scholar
Mobaraki, M., Vainikka, A., & Young-Scholten, M. (2008). The status of subjects in early child L2 English. In Haznedar, B. & Gavruseva, E. (Eds.), Current trends in child second language acquisition: A generative perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 209236. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/lald.46.11mobGoogle Scholar
Montanari, S. (2010). Translation equivalents and the emergence of multiple lexicons in early trilingual development. First Language, 30, 102125. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0142723709350528Google Scholar
Müller, L.-M., Howard, K., Wilson, E., Gibson, J., & Katsos, N. (2020). Bilingualism in the family and child well-being: A scoping review. International Journal of Bilingualism, 24(5–6), 10491070. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/1367006920920939Google Scholar
Muñoz, C., & Spada, N. (2019). Foreign language learning from early childhood to young adulthood. In De Houwer, A. & Ortega, L. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 233249. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/9781316831922.013Google Scholar
Murphy, V. A. (2018). Literacy development in linguistically diverse pupils. In Miller, D., Bayram, F., Rothman, J., & Serratrice, L. (Eds.), Bilingual cognition and language: The state of the science across its subfields. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 155–82. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.54.08murGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, J. (2015). Nonnative maternal input : Language use and errors in a Thai mother’s interactions in Japanese with her child. Japan Journal of Multilingualism and Multiculturalism, 21, 1026.Google Scholar
Nakamura, J. (2020). Language regrets: Mixed-ethnic children’s lost opportunity for minority language acquisition in Japan. Multilingua, 39(2), 213237. doi: http://doi.org/10.1515/multi-2019-0040Google Scholar
Nakamura, J., & Quay, S. (2012). The impact of caregivers’ interrogative styles in English and Japanese on early bilingual development. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 15(4), 417434. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2012.665827Google Scholar
Navarro, A. M., Pearson, B. Z., Cobo-Lewis, A., & Oller, K. D. (1998). Identifying the language spoken by 26-month-old monolingual- and bilingual-learning babies in a no-context situation. In Greenhill, A., Hughes, M., Littlefield, H., & Walsh, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Vol. 2. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, 557–68.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E. (1998). First clues to the existence of two input languages: Pragmatic and lexical differentiation in a bilingual child. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 105116. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728998000236Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (Eds.) (2002). Language and literacy in bilingual children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Oppenheim, G. M., Griffin, Z. M., Peña, E. D., & Bedore, L. M. (2020). Longitudinal evidence for simultaneous bilingual language development with shifting language dominance, and how to explain it. Language Learning, 70, 2044. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12398Google Scholar
Orena, A., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Polka, L. (2020). What do bilingual infants actually hear? Evaluating measures of language input to bilingual-learning 10-month-olds. Developmental Science 23(2), e12901. doi: http://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/2qnhwGoogle Scholar
Orena, A. J., & Polka, L. (2019). Monolingual and bilingual infants” word segmentation abilities in an inter‐mixed dual‐language task. Infancy, 24(5), 718737. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R.M. (2017). Identifying pathways between socio-economic status and language development. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 285308. doi: http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011516-034226Google Scholar
Palacios, N., Kibler, A. K., Simpson Baird, A., Parr, A., & Bergey, R. (2015). An examination of language practices during mother-child play activities among Mexican immigrant families. International Multilingual Research Journal, 9(3), 197219. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/19313152.2015.1048543Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (2011). Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(3), 213237. doi: http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.01parGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Navarro, S. (2003). Subject realization and crosslinguistic interference in the bilingual acquisition of Spanish and English: What is the role of the input? Journal of Child Language, 30, 371393. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000903005609Google Scholar
Paradis, J., & Nicoladis, E. (2007). The influence of dominance and sociolinguistic context on bilingual preschoolers” language choice. The International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10(3), 277297. doi: http://doi.org/10.2167/beb444.0Google Scholar
Park, H., Tsai, K. M., Liu, L. L., & Lau, A. S. (2012). Transactional associations between supportive family climate and young children’s heritage language proficiency in immigrant families. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 36(3), 226236. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0165025412439842Google Scholar
Parra, M., Hoff, E., & Core, C. (2011). Relations among language exposure, phonological memory, and language development in Spanish-English bilingually developing two year olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 113125. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.011Google Scholar
Patterson, J. L. (1998). Expressive vocabulary development and word combinations of Spanish–English bilingual toddlers. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 7, 4656. doi: http://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360.0704.46Google Scholar
Patterson, J. L. (2002). Relationships of expressive vocabulary to frequency of reading and television experience among bilingual toddlers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(4), 493508. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716402004010Google Scholar
Patterson, J. L, & Rodríguez, B. L. (Eds.) (2016). Multilingual perspectives on child language disorders. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. doi: http://doi.org/10.21832/9781783094738Google Scholar
Pavlovitch, M. (1920). Le langage enfantin. Acquisition du serbe et du français. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., & Fernández, S. C. (1994). Patterns of interaction in the lexical growth in two languages of bilingual infants and toddlers. Language Learning, 44, 617653.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 4158. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400009863Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in simultaneous bilingual infants: comparison to monolinguals. Language Learning, 43, 93120.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S., & Oller, D. K. (1995). Cross-language synonyms in the lexicons of bilingual infants: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 22, 345368. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S030500090000982XGoogle Scholar
Pearson, P. D. (2019). Time, complexity, and the enduring importance of words: Key themes in language learning in the middle years. In Grøver, V., Ucelli, P., Rowe, M. L., & Lieven, E. (Eds.), Learning through language. Towards an educationally informed theory of language learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 159169. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/9781316718537.015Google Scholar
Pease-Alvarez, L., & Winsler, A. (1994). Cuando el maestro no habla Español: Children’s bilingual languages practices in the classroom. TESOL Quarterly, 28, 507535.Google Scholar
Persici, V., Vihman, M. M., Burroa, R., & Majoranoa, M. (2019). Lexical access and competition in bilingual children: The role of proficiency and the lexical similarity of the two languages. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 179, 103125. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.10.002Google Scholar
Pfaff, C. (1994). Early bilingual development of Turkish children in Berlin. In Extra, G. & Verhoeven, L. (Eds.), The cross-linguistic study of bilingualism. Amsterdam: Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, 7597.Google Scholar
Pienemann, M. (1981). Der Zweitspracherwerb ausländischer Arbeiterkinder. Bonn: Bouvier.Google Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2011). Properties of dual language exposure that influence two-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82 (6), 18341849. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01660.xGoogle Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2016). Effects and noneffects of input in bilingual environments on dual language skills in 2½-year-olds. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 10231041. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728915000322Google Scholar
Polka, L., Orena, A. J., Sundara, M., & Worrall, J. (2017). Segmenting words from fluent speech during infancy – challenges and opportunities in a bilingual context. Developmental Science, 20, e12419. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12419Google Scholar
Porsché, D. (1983). Die Zweisprachigkeit während des primären Spracherwerbs. Tübingen: Gunter Narr.Google Scholar
Prevoo, M. J. L., Malda, M., Mesman, J., Emmen, R.A.G., Yeniad, N., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Linting, M. (2014). Predicting ethnic minority children’s vocabulary from socioeconomic status, maternal language and home reading input: Different pathways for host and ethnic language. Journal of Child Language, 41(5), 963984. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000913000299Google Scholar
Prevoo, M., Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M., & Pieper, S. (2011). Bilingual toddlers reap the language they sow: Ethnic minority toddlers’ childcare attendance increases maternal host language use. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 32, 561576. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2011.609279Google Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., Bialystok, E., Blaye, A., Polonia, A., & Yott, J. (2013). Lexical access and vocabulary development in very young bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(1), 5770. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/1367006911431198Google Scholar
Ramírez-Esparza, N., García-Sierra, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2017). The impact of early social interactions on later language development in Spanish–English bilingual infants. Child Development, 88(4), 12161234. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12648Google Scholar
Reich, H. H. (2009). Zweisprachige Kinder. Sprachenaneignung und sprachliche Fortschritte im Kindergartenalter. Münster: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Ribot, K., & Hoff, E. (2014). “¿Cómo estas?” “I’m good.” Conversational code-switching is related to profiles of expressive and receptive proficiency in Spanish-English bilingual toddlers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(3), 333341. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0165025414533225Google Scholar
Ribot, K. M., Hoff, E., & Burridge, A. (2018). Language use contributes to expressive language growth: evidence from bilingual children. Child Development, 89(3), 929940. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12770Google Scholar
Rinker, T., Budde-Spengler, N., & Sachse, S. (2017). The relationship between first language (L1) and second language (L2) lexical development in young Turkish-German children. International Journal of Bilingualism and Bilingual Education, 20, 218233. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2016.1179260Google Scholar
Roesch, A., & Condrogianni, V. (2016). “Which mouse kissed the frog?” Effects of age of onset, length of exposure, and knowledge of case marking on the comprehension of wh-questions in German-speaking simultaneous and early sequential bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 43(3), 635661. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000015Google Scholar
Ronjat, J. (1913). Le développement du langage observé chez un enfant bilingue. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Rothweiler, M. (2016). Zum Erwerb der deutschen Grammatik bei früh sequentiell zweisprachigen Kindern mit Türkisch als Erstsprache – Ergebnisse aus einem Forschungsprojekt. Diskurs Kindheits- und Jugendforschung, 1, 9-25.Google Scholar
Scheele, A., Leseman, P., & Mayo, A. (2010). The home language environment of monolingual and bilingual children and their language proficiency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 117140. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716409990191Google Scholar
Schulz, P. (2013). Wer versteht wann was? Sprachverstehen im frühen Zweitspracherwerb des Deutschen am Beispiel der w-Fragen. In Deppermann, A. (Ed.), Das Deutsch der Migranten. Berlin: de Gruyter, 313337. doi: http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110307894.313Google Scholar
Schulz, P., & Tracy, R. (2011). LiSe-DaZ. Linguistische Sprachstandserhebung – Deutsch als Zweitsprache. (In Verbindung mit der Baden-Württemberg Stiftung). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schwartz, M. (2020). Strategies and practices of home language maintenance. In Eisenchlas, S. & Schalley, A. (Eds.), Handbook of home language maintenance and development. Social and affective factors. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 194217. doi: http://doi.org/10.1515/9781501510175-010Google Scholar
Schwartz, M., Deeb, I., & Hijazy, S. (2020). “How do you say it in Arabic, in Hebrew, in English?” Towards a better understanding of children’s agentic behaviour in novel language learning. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2020.1711768Google Scholar
Schwartz, M., Minkov, M., Dieser, E., Protassova, E., Moin, V., & Polinsky, M. (2015). Acquisition of Russian gender agreement by monolingual and bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19, 726752. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/1367006914544989Google Scholar
Serratrice, L. (2007). Cross-linguistic influence in the interpretation of anaphoric and cataphoric pronouns in English–Italian bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 225238. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907003045Google Scholar
Serratrice, L. (2019). Becoming bilingual in early childhood. In De Houwer, A. & Ortega, L. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1535. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/9781316831922.002Google Scholar
Sierens, S., Slembrouck, S., Van Gorp, K., & Ağırdağ, O. (2019). Linguistic interdependence of receptive vocabulary skills in emergent bilingual preschool children: Exploring a factor-dependent approach. Applied Psycholinguistics, 50(5), 12691297. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716419000250Google Scholar
Silvén, M., Voeten, M., Kouvo, A., & Lundén, M. (2014). Speech perception and vocabulary growth: A longitudinal study of Finnish-Russian bilinguals and Finnish monolinguals from infancy to three years. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(4), 323332. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/0165025414533748Google Scholar
Singh, L., Fu, C.S.L., Tay, Z., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Novel word learning in bilingual and monolingual infants: Evidence for a bilingual advantage. Child Development, 89(3), e183e198. doi: http://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12747Google Scholar
Singh, L., Poh, F. L. S., & Fu, C. S. L. (2016). Limits on monolingualism? A comparison of monolingual and bilingual infants’ abilities to integrate lexical tone in novel word learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 667. doi: http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00667Google Scholar
Sinka, I., & Schelletter, C. (1998). Morphosyntactic development in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 2(3), 301326. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/136700699800200303Google Scholar
Slavkov, N. (2015). Language attrition and reactivation in the context of bilingual first language acquisition. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 18, 715734. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2014.941785Google Scholar
Smithson, L., Paradis, J., & Nicoladis, E. (2014). Bilingualism and receptive vocabulary achievement: Could sociocultural context make a difference? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(4), 810821. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000813Google Scholar
Song, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Yoshikawa, H., Kahana-Kalman, R., & Wu, I. (2012). Language experiences and vocabulary development in Dominican and Mexican infants across the first 2 years. Developmental Psychology, 48, 11061123. doi: http://doi.org/10.1037/a0026401Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., Bornstein, M. H., Vandell, D. L., & Rook, K. (2011). Life-span development: Infancy through adulthood. Boston, MA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Sun, H., Ng, S. C., O’Brien, B. A., & Fritzsche, T. (2020). Child, family, and school factors in bilingual preschoolers’ vocabulary development in heritage languages. Journal of Child Language, 47(4), 817843. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000904Google Scholar
Sun, H., Yussof, N., Mohamed, M., Rahim, A., Cheung, W. L., Cheong, S. A., & Bull, R. (2021). Bilingual language experience and social-emotional wellbeing: A cross-sectional study of Singapore preschoolers. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 24(3), 324339. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2018.1461802Google Scholar
Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Genesee, F. (2006). Language-experience facilitates discrimination of /d-δ/ in monolingual and bilingual acquisition of English. Cognition, 100(2), 369388. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.04.007Google Scholar
Sundara, M., & Scutellaro, A. (2011). Rhythmic distance between languages affects the development of speech perception in bilingual infants. Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 505513. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2010.08.006Google Scholar
Sundara, M., Ward, N., Conboy, B., & Kuhl, P. K. (2020). Exposure to a second language in infancy alters speech production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(5), 978–991. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000853Google Scholar
Tang, G., & Sze, F. (2019). Bilingualism and sign language research. In De Houwer, A. & Ortega, L. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 483509. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/9781316831922.025Google Scholar
Tare, M., & Gelman, S. A. (2010). Can you say it another way? Cognitive factors in bilingual children’s pragmatic language skills. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 137158.Google Scholar
Thompson, L. (2000). Young bilingual children in nursery school. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Uccelli, P., & Páez, M. (2007). Narrative and vocabulary development of bilingual children from kindergarten to first grade: Developmental changes and associations among English and Spanish skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 225236. doi: http://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2007/024)Google Scholar
Uchikoshi, Y. (2006). English vocabulary development in bilingual kindergarteners: What are the best predictors? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 3349. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728905002361Google Scholar
UNESCO. (2016). If you don’t understand, how can you learn? Global Education Monitoring Report. Policy Paper 24. bit.ly/MLD2016Google Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2013). Assessing age of onset effects in (early) child L2 acquisition. Language Acquisition, 20, 7492. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2013.766739Google Scholar
Unsworth, S., Argyri, F., Cornips, L., Hulk, A., Sorace, A., & Tsimpli, I. (2014). The role of age of onset and input in early child bilingualism in Greek and Dutch. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 765805. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000574Google Scholar
Unsworth, S., Brouwer, S., de Bree, E. & Verhagen, J. (2019). Predicting bilingual preschoolers’ patterns of language development: Degree of non-native input matters. Applied Psycholinguistics, 40(5), 11891219. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716419000225Google Scholar
Vaahtoranta, E., Suggate, S., Lenhart, J., & Lenhard, W. (2020). Language exposure and phonological short-term memory as predictors of majority language vocabulary and phonological awareness in dual language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(2), 319332. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000541Google Scholar
van de Weijer, J. (1997). Language input to a prelingual infant. In Sorace, A., Heycock, C. & Shillcock, R. (Eds.), Proceedings of the GALA ‘97 Conference on Language Acquisition. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh, 290293.Google Scholar
van de Weijer, J. (1998). Language input for word discovery. Max Planck Series in Psycholinguistics 9. Wageningen: Ponsen & Looijen.Google Scholar
van de Weijer, J. (2000). Language input and word discovery. In Beers, M., van den Bogaerde, B., Bol, G., de Jong, J., & Rooijmans, C. (Eds.), From sound to sentence: Studies on first language acquisition. Groningen: Centre for Language and Cognition, 155162.Google Scholar
van de Weijer, J. (2002). How much does an infant hear in a day? In Costa, J. & João Freitas, M. (Eds.), Proceedings of the GALA2001 Conference on Language Acquisition. Lisbon, Portugal: Associação Portuguesa de Linguistíca, 279282.Google Scholar
Verdon, S., McLeod, S., & Winsler, A. (2014). Language maintenance and loss in a population study of young Australian children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(2), 168181. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.12.003Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1999). The transition to grammar in a bilingual child: Positional patterns, model learning, and relational words. International Journal of Bilingualism, 3(2–3), 267301. doi: http://doi.org/10.1177/13670069990030020801Google Scholar
Vihman, M. (2016). Prosodic structures and templates in bilingual phonological development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 6988. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000790Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Thierry, G., Lum, J., Keren-Portnoy, T., & Martin, P. (2007). Onset of word form recognition in English, Welsh, and English-Welsh bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 475493. doi: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070269Google Scholar
Vila, I. (1984). Yo siempre hablo catalan y castellano: datos de una investigacion en curso sobre la adquisicion del lenguaje en niños bilingües familiares. In Siguan, M. (Ed.), Adquisición precoz de una segunda lengua. Barcelona: Publicacions i edicions de la Universitat de Barcelona, 3151.Google Scholar
von Grünigen, R., Kochenderfer-Ladd, B., Perren, S., & Alsaker, F. (2012). Links between local language competence and peer relations among Swiss and immigrant children: The mediating role of social behaviour. Journal of School Psychology, 50(2), 195213. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2011.09.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner-Gough, J. (1975). Comparative studies in second language learning. CAL-ERIC/CCL Series on Language and Linguistics, 26. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Weikum, W. M., Vouloumanos, A., Navarra, J., Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Visual language discrimination in infancy. Science, 316(5828), 1159. doi: http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1137686Google Scholar
Willard, J. A., Kohl, K., Bihler, L.-M., Agache, A., & Leyendecker, B. (2020). Family literacy activities and their interplay with family and preschool language environments: Links to gains in dual language learners’ German vocabulary. Early Education and Development, 32(2), 189208. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2020.1744404Google Scholar
Winsler, A., Burchinal, M., Tien, H.-C., Peisner-Feinberg, E., Espinosa, L., Castro, D., LaForett, D., Kim, Y., & De Feyter, J. (2014a). Early developmental skills of diverse dual language learners: The roles of home language use, cultural heritage, maternal immigration, and socio-demographic variables. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 750764. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.02.008Google Scholar
Winsler, A., Kim, Y., & Richard, E. (2014b). Socio-emotional skills, behavior problems, and Spanish competence predict the acquisition of English among English language learners in poverty. Developmental Psychology, 50, 22422254. doi: http://doi.org/10.1037/a0037161Google Scholar
Wölck, W. (1987/1988). Types of natural bilingual behavior: A review and revision. The Bilingual Review/La Revista bilingüe, 14, 316.Google Scholar
Yamamoto, M. (2001). Language use in interlingual families: A Japanese–English sociolinguistic study. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Yow, W. Q., & Markman, E. M. (2011). Young bilingual children’s heightened sensitivity to referential cues. Journal of Cognitive Development, 12, 1231. doi: http://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2011.539524Google Scholar
Yu, S. (2014). How much can migrant parents influence their children’s language choice. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, 12(1), 81105.Google Scholar
Zdorenko, T., & Paradis, J. (2007). The role of the first language in child second language acquisition of articles. In Belikova, A., Meroni, L., & Umeda, M. (Eds.), Galana 2: Proceedings of the Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America 2. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, 483-490.Google Scholar
Zentella, A. C. (1997). Growing up bilingual: Puerto Rican children in New York. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar

Save element to Kindle

To save this element to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Bilingual Development in Childhood
Available formats
×

Save element to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Bilingual Development in Childhood
Available formats
×

Save element to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Bilingual Development in Childhood
Available formats
×