Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
  • Cited by 795
Publisher:
Cambridge University Press
Online publication date:
December 2009
Print publication year:
2003
Online ISBN:
9780511613609

Book description

Languages differ in how they describe space, and such differences between languages can be used to explore the relation between language and thought. This 2003 book shows that even in a core cognitive domain like spatial thinking, language influences how people think, memorize and reason about spatial relations and directions. After outlining a typology of spatial coordinate systems in language and cognition, it is shown that not all languages use all types, and that non-linguistic cognition mirrors the systems available in the local language. The book reports on collaborative, interdisciplinary research, involving anthropologists, linguists and psychologists, conducted in many languages and cultures around the world, which establishes this robust correlation. The overall results suggest that thinking in the cognitive sciences underestimates the transformative power of language on thinking. The book will be of interest to linguists, psychologists, anthropologists and philosophers, and especially to students of spatial cognition.

Reviews

"A fundamental part of this new-Whorfian movement has been Stephen Levinson's thinking about spatial language and cognition. It is valuable to have most of his arguments and data gathered together in this thought-provoking book."
-Nora S. Newcombe, Human Development

"Levinson's book will certainly stand as a textbook in the study of the relationship between language and cognition, and is rich and challenging reading...Those with an interest in the issue of linguistic variation will find an unusually well-documented and well-argued presentation of the actual variation in the domain of spatial reference."
-Ingjerd Hoem, Institute for Pacific Archaeology and Cultural History, The Kon-Tiki Museum, Anthropological Linguistics

Refine List

Actions for selected content:

Select all | Deselect all
  • View selected items
  • Export citations
  • Download PDF (zip)
  • Save to Kindle
  • Save to Dropbox
  • Save to Google Drive

Save Search

You can save your searches here and later view and run them again in "My saved searches".

Please provide a title, maximum of 40 characters.
×

Contents

List of references
References
Acredolo, L. 1988, Infant mobility and spatial development, in J. Stiles-Davis, M. Kritchevsky and U. Bellugi (eds.), Spatial cognition: Brain bases and development, pp. 157–66. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Google Scholar
Ameka, F. and Levinson, S. C. in preparation, Positional verbs and locative predicates, Special issue of Linguistics Google Scholar
Anderson, S. and Keenan, E. 1985, Deixis, in T. Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, Vol. III: Grammatical categories and the lexicon, pp. 259–307. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Aoki, K. and Feldman, M. W. 1991, Recessive hereditary deafness, assortative mating, and persistence of a sign language, Theoretical Population Biology 39 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(3): 358–72
Argyle, M. 1967, The psychology of interpersonal behaviour, Harmondsworth: Penguin Google Scholar
Argyle, M. and Trower, P. 1979, Person to person: Ways of communicating, London: Harper and Row Google Scholar
Asch, S. E. and Witkin, H. A. 1948, Studies in space orientation II: Perception of the upright with displaced visual fields and with body tilted, Journal of Experimental Psychology 38: 455–77. [Reprinted in: Journal of Experimental Psychology, General, 1992, 121 CrossRef | Google Scholar(4): 407–18.]
Austin, P. and Bresnan, J. 1996, Non-configurationality in Australian Aboriginal languages, Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 14 CrossRef | Google Scholar(2): 215–68
Baayen, H. and Danziger, E. (eds.) 1994, Annual Report of the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics 1993, Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. 1990, Human memory: Theory and practice, Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon Google Scholar
Baker, R. R. 1989, Human navigation and magnetoreception, Manchester: University of Manchester Press Google Scholar
Batschelet, E. 1981, Circular statistics, New York: Academic Press Google Scholar
Beattie, G. 1983, Talk: An analysis of speech and non-verbal behaviour in conversation, Stony Stratford: Open University Press Google Scholar
Berg, R. van den 1997, Spatial deixis in Muna (Sulawesi), in G. Senft (ed.), Referring to space, pp. 197–220. Oxford: Clarendon Press Google Scholar
Berlin, B. 1968, Tzeltal numeral classifiers, Berkeley: University of California Press Google Scholar
Berlin, B. 1992, Ethnobiological classification: Principles of categorization of plants and animals in traditional societies, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press Google Scholar
Berlin, B. and Kay, P. 1969, Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution, Berkeley: University of California Press Google Scholar
Berlin, B., Breedlove, D. and Raven, P. 1974, Principles of Tzeltal plant classification, New York: Academic Press Google Scholar
Berthoz, A. 1991, Reference frames for the perception and control of movement, in J. Paillard (ed.), Brain and space, pp. 81–111. Oxford: Oxford Science Publications Google Scholar
Beusmans, J. 1996, Two strategies for route learning in a driving simulator. Talk given at the 3rd Annual CBR Workshop on ‘Mental Representations in Navigation’. Cambridge Basic Research, Cambridge, MA Google Scholar
Bickel, B. 1997, Spatial operations in deixis, cognition, and culture: Where to orient oneself in Belhare, in J. Nuyts and E. Pederson (eds.), Language and conceptualization, pp. 46–83. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Bierwisch, M. 1967, Some semantic universals of German adjectivals, Foundations of Language 3 Google Scholar: 1–36
Bierwisch, M. and Lang, E. 1989, Somewhat longer – much deeper – further and further. Epilogue to the Dimension Adjective Project, in M. Bierwisch and E. Lang (eds.), Dimensional adjectives: Grammatical structure and conceptual interpretation, pp. 471–514. Berlin: Springer Google Scholar
Bisiach, E. and Luzzatti, C. 1978, Unilateral neglect of representational space, Cortex 14 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 129–33
Blake, B. J. 1994, Case, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Bloom, P., Peterson, M., Nadel, L. and Garrett, M. (eds.) 1996, Language and space, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. 1984, Possible causes of sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees, Journal of Human Evolution 13 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 415–40
Bohnemeyer, J. 1998, Temporal reference from a radical pragmatics perspective. Why Yucatec doesn't need to express ‘after’ and ‘before’, Cognitive Linguistics 9 CrossRef | Google Scholar(3): 239–82
Bohnemeyer, J. and Stolz, C. in preparation, The expression of spatial reference in Yukatek Maya: A survey, in S. C. Levinson and D. P. Wilkins (eds.), Grammars of space Google Scholar
Bourdieu, P. 1977, Outline of a theory of practice, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Bowden, J. 1991, Grammaticalization of locatives in Oceanic languages. MA thesis, University of Auckland Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. 1996, Learning how to structure space for language: A cross-linguistic perspective, in P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel and M. Garrett (eds.), Language and space, pp. 385–436. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. 2000, Where do children's word meanings come from? Rethinking the role of cognition in early semantic development, in L. Nucci, G. Saxe and E. Turiel (eds.), Culture, thought, and development, pp. 199–230. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. and Choi, S. 2001, Shaping meanings for language: Universal and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semantic categories, in M. Bowerman and S. Levinson (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, pp. 475–511. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S. C. (eds.) 2001, Language acquisition and conceptual development, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Bowerman, M., de León, L. and Choi, S. 1995, Verbs, particles and spatial semantics. Learning to talk about spatial actions in typologically different languages, in E. V. Clark (ed.), Proceedings of the 27th Child Language Research Forum, pp. 101–10. Stanford: CSLI Google Scholar
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. 1985, Culture and the evolutionary process, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Brewer, B. and Pears, J. 1993, Frames of reference, in N. Eilan, R. McCarthy and B. Brewer (eds.), Spatial representation: Problems in philosophy and psychology, pp. 25–30. Oxford: Blackwell Google Scholar
Brown, C. H. 1983, Where do cardinal direction terms come from?Anthropological Linguistics 25 Google Scholar(2): 121–61
Brown, P. 1994, The INs and ONs of Tzeltal locative expressions: The semantics of static descriptions of location, in J. B. Haviland and S. C. Levinson (eds.), Space in Mayan languages, Special issue of Linguistics 32(4/5): 743–90 Google Scholar
Brown, P. 1998, Children's first verbs in Tzeltal: Evidence for an early verb category, Linguistics 36 Google Scholar(4): 713–53
Brown, P. 2001, Learning to talk about motion UP and DOWN in Tzeltal: Is there a language-specific bias for verb learning, in M. Bowerman and S. Levinson (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, pp. 512–43. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Brown, P. and Levinson, S. C. 1993a, ‘Uphill’ and ‘downhill’ in Tzeltal, Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 3 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 46–74
Brown, P. and Levinson, S. C. 1993b, Explorations in Mayan cognition. Working paper 24, Cognitive Anthropology Research Group, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen Google Scholar
Brown, P. and Levinson, S. C. 2000, Frames of spatial reference and their acquisition in Tenejapan Tzeltal, in L. Nucci, G. Saxe and E. Turiel (eds.), Culture, thought, and development, pp. 167–98. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Google Scholar
Brown, P. and Levinson, S. C. in preparation, Tilted worlds: The language and cognition of space in a Mayan community Google Scholar
Brown, P., Senft, G. and Wheeldon, L. (eds.) 1993, Annual Report of the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics 1992. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics Google Scholar
Brugman, C. 1983, The use of body-part terms as locatives in Chalcatongo Mixtec, Survey of California and Other Indian Languages 4 Google Scholar: 235–90
Brugman, C. and Macaulay, M. 1986, Interacting semantic systems: Mixtec expressions of location, Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistics Society Meeting 12 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 315–27
Bühler, K. 1982 [1934], The deictic field of language and deictic words, in R. J. Jarvella and W. Klein (eds.), Speech, place and action, pp. 9–30. New York: Wiley Google Scholar
Bülthoff, H. H. 1991, Shape from X: Psychophysics and computation, in M. S. Landy and J. A. Movshon (eds.), Computational models of visual processing, pp. 305–30. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Burgess, N., Jeffery, K. and O'Keefe, J. 1999, The hippocampal and parietal foundations of spatial cognition, Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Cablitz, G. H. 2002, The acquisition of an absolute system: learning to talk about space in Marquesan (Oceanic, French Polynesia). In the Proceedings of the 31st Child Language Research Forum, Stanford, 2002, pp. 40–9. http://csli-publications.stanford.edu Google Scholar
Calbris, G. 1990, The semiotics of French gesture, Bloomington: University of Indiana Press Google Scholar
Campbell, A. 1987, Portolan charts from the late thirteenth century to 1500, in J. B. Harley and D. Woodward (eds.), The history of cartography, vol. 1, pp. 371–463. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Campbell, J. 1993, The role of physical objects in spatial thinking. In N. Eilan, R. McCarthy and B. Brewer (eds.), Spatial representation: Problems in philosophy and psychology, pp. 65–95. Oxford: Blackwell Google Scholar
Carlson-Radvansky, L. A. and Irwin, D. A. 1993, Frames of reference in vision and language: Where is above?Cognition 46 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 223–44
Casey, E. 1997, The fate of place, Berkeley: University of California Press Google Scholar
Cassell, J., McNeill, D. and McCullough, K.-E. 1999, Speech-gesture mismatches: Evidence for one underlying representation of linguistic and nonlinguistic information, Pragmatics & Cognition 7 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 1–33
Cassirer, E. 1923, Das Erkenntnisproblem in der Philosophie und Wissenschaft der neueren Zeit, Berlin: Cassirer Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. and Feldman, M. W. 1981, Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach, Monographs in population biology, 16. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press Google Scholar
Cheng, K. and Spetch, M. 1998, Mechanisms of landmark use in mammals and birds, in S. Healy (ed.), Spatial representation in animals, pp. 1–17. Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Choi, S., McDonough, L., Mandler, J. and Bowerman, M. in preparation, Development of language-specific semantic categories of spatial relations: From prelinguistic to linguistic stage. Paper presented at the Workshop ‘Finding the Words’. Stanford University, May 2000 Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1965, Aspects of the theory of syntax, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. 1973, Space, time, semantics, and the child, in T. E. Moore (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language, pp. 28–64. New York: Academic Press Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. and Wilkes-Gibbs, D. 1986, Referring as a collaborative process, Cognition 22 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(1): 1–39
Cohen, D. and Kubovy, M. 1993, Mental rotation, mental representation, and flat slopes, Cognitive Psychology 25 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 351–82
Colby, C. 1999, Spatial perception. Entry in MIT Encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences, pp. 784–7. Boston, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Collett, T. S. and Zeil, J. 1998, Places and landmarks: An Arthropod perspective, in S. Healy (ed.), Spatial representations in animals, pp. 18–53. Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Cowan, N. 2001, The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(1): 87–114
Danziger, E. 1994, Out of sight, out of mind: Person, perception and function in Mopan Maya spatial deixis, in J. B. Haviland and S. C. Levinson (eds.), Space in Mayan languages, Special issue of Linguistics 32(4): 885–907 Google Scholar
Danziger, E. 1996, Parts and their counter-parts: Social and spatial relationships in Mopan Maya, The Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, inc. Man 2 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 67–82
Danziger, E. 1999, Language, space and sociolect: Cognitive correlates of gendered speech in Mopan Maya, in C. Fuchs and S. Robert (eds.), Language diversity and cognitive representations, pp. 85–106. Amsterdam: Benjamins Google Scholar
Danziger, E. and Pederson, E. 1998, Through the looking glass: Literacy, writing systems and mirror-image discrimination, Written Language and Literacy 1 CrossRef | Google Scholar(2): 153–67
Davidoff, J., Davies, I. and Robertson, D. 1999, Colour categories in a stone-age tribe, Nature 398 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 203–4
Dawkins, R. 1976, The selfish gene, Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Deacon, T. 1997, The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain, New York: Norton Google Scholar
Dennett, D. 1991, Consciousness explained, Boston, MA: Little Brown Google Scholar
Dennett, D. 1995, Darwin's dangerous idea, London: Penguin Google Scholar
Dilke, O. A. W. 1987, Itineraries and geographical maps in the early and late Roman empires, in J. B. Harley and D. Woodward (eds.), The history of cartography, vol. 1, pp. 234–57. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Duhem, P. 1985, Medieval cosmology, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Durham, W. 1991, Coevolution, Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press Google Scholar
Eilan, N. 1993, Molyneux's question and the idea of an external world, in N. Eilan, R. McCarthy and B. Brewer (eds.), Spatial representation: Problems in philosophy and psychology, pp. 236–55. Oxford: Blackwell Google Scholar
Eilan, N., McCarthy, R. and Brewer, B. (eds.) 1993, Spatial representation: Problems in philosophy and psychology, Oxford: Blackwell Google Scholar
Einstein, A. 1954, Foreword, in M. Jammer (ed.), Concepts of space: The history of theories of space in physics, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press Google Scholar
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D. and Plunkett, K. 1996, Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Emmorey, K. 2001, Language, cognition and the brain: Insights from sign language research, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Google Scholar
Etienne, A. S., Berlie, J., Georgakopoulos, J. and Maurer, R. 1998, Role of dead reckoning in navigation, in S. Healy (ed.), Spatial representation in animals, pp. 54–68. Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Ettlinger, G. 1987, Cross-model sensory integration, in R. Gregory (ed.), The Oxford companion to the mind, pp. 173–4. Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Evans, N. 1995, A grammar of Kayardild, Berlin: Mouton Grammar Library Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. 1971, Towards a theory of deixis. Paper read at the Pacific Conference on Contrastive Linguistics and Language Universals, University of Hawaii Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. 1975, Santa Cruz lectures on deixis. Mimeo, Indiana University Linguistics Club Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. 1982, Towards a descriptive framework for spatial deixis, in R. J. Jarvella and W. Klein (eds.), Speech, place and action: Studies in deixis and related topics, pp. 31–59. New York: Wiley Google Scholar
Fodor, J. 1975, The language of thought, New York: Crowell Google Scholar
Fodor, J. 1983, The modularity of mind, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Fodor, J. D., Fodor, J. A. and Garrett, M. 1975, The unreality of semantic representations, Linguistic Inquiry 4 Google Scholar: 515–31
Fortescue, M. 1988, Eskimo orientation systems, Meddr Gr⊘nland: Man and Society 11 Google Scholar: 3–30
Friederici, A. and Levelt, W. J. M. 1990, Spatial reference in weightlessness: Perceptual factors and mental representations, Perception & Psychophysics 47 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(3): 253–66
Friedrich, P. 1971, The Tarascan suffixes of locative space: Meaning and morphotactics, Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press Google Scholar
Frisch, K. von 1967, The dance language and orientation of bees, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press Google Scholar
Furby, C. E. and Furby, E. S. 1976, Garawa compass directions, Talanya 3 Google Scholar: 1–13
Gage, J. 1999, Colour and meaning, London: Thames and Hudson Google Scholar
Galea, L. M. and Kimura, D. 1993, Sex differences in route-learning, Personality and Individual Differences 14 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 53–65
Gallistel, C. R. 1990, The organization of learning, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. 1996, Insect navigation: The brain as symbol processing organ or knowledge and purpose as the mechanisms of habit. Talk given at the 3rd Annual CBR Workshop on ‘Mental Representations in Navigation’. Cambridge, MA Google Scholar
Gardner, H. 1985, The mind's new science, New York: Basic Books Google Scholar
Geertz, C. C. 1972, Deep play: Notes on the Balinese cockfight, Daedalus 101 Google Scholar: 1–37
Gillner, S. and Mallot, H. 1997, Navigation and acquisition of spatial knowledge in a virtual maze. Technical Report 45, Max-Planck-Institut für biologische Kybernetik, Tübingen Google Scholar
Gladwin, T. 1970, East is a big bird: Navigation and logic on Puluwat Atoll, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press Google Scholar
Goddard, C. and Wierzbicka, A. 1994, Semantic and lexical universals, Amsterdam: Benjamins Google Scholar
Golledge, R. G. 1995, Path selection and route preference in human navigation: A progress report, in A. U. Frank and W. Kuhn (eds.), Spatial information theory, pp. 207–22. Berlin: Springer Google Scholar
Golledge, R. G., Dougherty, V. and Bell, S. 1995, Acquiring spatial knowledge – Survey versus route-based knowledge in unfamiliar environments, Annals of the Association of American Geographers 85 Google Scholar(1): 134–58
Golledge, R. G., Ruggles, A., Pellegrino, J. and Gale, N. 1993, Integrating route knowledge in an unfamiliar neighborhood: Along and across route experiments, Journal of Environmental Psychology 13 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 293–307
Goodenough, W. H. 1956, Componential analysis and the study of meaning, Language 32 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 195–216
Gregory, R. L. 1987, Oxford companion to the mind, Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Gumperz, J. J. and Levinson, S. C. (eds.) 1996, Rethinking linguistic relativity, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Haiman, J. 1980, Hua: A Papuan language of the Eastern Highlands, Papua New Guinea, Amsterdam: Benjamins Google Scholar
Hanks, W. 1990, Referential practice: Language and lived space in a Maya community, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Harley, J. B. and Woodward, D. (eds.) 1987, The history of cartography, Vol. 1, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Hatfield, G. 1990, The natural and the normative: Theories of spatial perception from Kant to Helmholtz, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Hauser, M. 1997, The evolution of communication, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Haviland, J. B. 1979a, Guugu Yimidhirr, in R. M. W. Dixon and B. Blake (eds.), Handbook of Australian languages, Vol. I, pp. 27–182. Canberra: Australian National University Press Google Scholar
Haviland, J. B. 1979b, Guugu Yimidhirr brother-in-law language, Language in Society 8 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 365–93
Haviland, J. B. 1985, The evolution of a speech community: Guugu Yimidhirr at Hopevale, Aboriginal History 9 Google Scholar(1/2): 170–204
Haviland, J. B. 1986, Complex referential gestures. Unpublished MS Google Scholar
Haviland, J. B. 1993, Anchoring, iconicity and orientation in Guugu Yimithirr pointing gestures, Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 3 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 3–45
Haviland, J. B. 1996, Projections, transpositions, and relativity, in J. J. Gumperz and S. C. Levinson (eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity, pp. 269–323. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Haviland, J. B. 1998, Guugu Yimithirr cardinal directions, Ethos 26 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 7–24
Haviland, J. B. 2000, Pointing, gesture spaces, and mental maps, in D. McNeill (ed.), Language and gesture, pp. 13–46. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Haviland, J. B. and Hart, R. 1998, Old man fog and the last Aborigines of Barrow Point, Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press Google Scholar
Haviland, J. B. and Haviland, L. 1980, ‘How much food will there be in Heaven?’ Aborigines and Lutherans around Cooktown before 1900, Aboriginal History 4 Google Scholar(1–2): 118–49
Healy, S. (ed.) 1998, Spatial representation in animals, Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Heeschen, V. 1982, Some systems of spatial deixis in Papuan languages, in J. Weissenborn and W. Klein (eds.), Here and there: Cross-linguistic studies on deixis and demonstration, pp. 81–110. Amsterdam: Benjamins Google Scholar
Heine, B. 1997, Cognitive foundations of grammar, Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Heine, B., Claudi, U. and Hünnemeyer, F. 1991, Grammaticalization: A conceptual framework, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Hermer, L. and Spelke, E. 1994, A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children, Nature 370 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 57–9
Herrmann, T. 1990, Vor, hinter, rechts und links: Das 6H-Modell, Zeitschrift für Literatur und Linguistik 78 Google Scholar: 117–40
Herskovits, A. 1986, Language and spatial cognition: An interdisciplinary study of the prepositions in English, Studies in natural language processing, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Hertz, R. 1960 [1909], La prééminence de la main droite: Etude sur la polarité religieuse, in R. Needham and C. Needham (eds.), ‘Death’ and ‘the right hand’, pp. 89–113. London: Cohen and West Google Scholar
Hill, C. 1982, Up/down, front/back, left/right: A contrastive study of Hausa and English, in J. Weissenborn and W. Klein (eds.), Here and there: Cross-linguistic studies on deixis and demonstration, pp. 11–42. Amsterdam: Benjamins Google Scholar
Hill, D. 1994, Spatial configurations and evidential propositions. Working Paper No. 25, Cognitive Anthropology Research Group, Max Planck Institute, Nijmegen Google Scholar
Hill, D. 1997, Finding your way in Longgu: geographical reference in a Solomons Islands language, in G. Senft (ed.), Referring to space: Studies in Austronesian and Papuan languages, pp. 101–26. Oxford: Clarendon Press Google Scholar
Hockett, C. F. 1960, The origin of speech, Scientific American 203 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 89–96
Howard, I. P. 1987, Spatial coordination of the senses, in R. L. Gregory (ed.), The Oxford companion to the mind, pp. 727–32. Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Hughes, H. C. 1999, Sensory exotica: A world beyond human experience, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Hunn, E. 1996, Columbia Plateau Indian place names: What can they teach us?Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 6 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 3–26
Hutchins, E. 1995, Cognition in the wild, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J. 1968, Constructing spatial images: A strategy in reasoning, Psychological Review 75 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 550–60
Jackendoff, R. 1983, Semantics and cognition, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. 1991, Parts and boundaries, Cognition 41 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 9–45
Jackendoff, R. 1992, Languages of the mind: Essays on mental representation, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. 1996, The architecture of the linguistic-spatial interface, in P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel and M. Garrett (eds.), Language and space, pp. 1–30. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Jacobson, S. A. 1984, Semantics and morphology of demonstratives in Central Yup'ik Eskimo, Etudes/Inuit/Studies 8 Google Scholar: 185–92
Jammer, M. 1954, Concepts of space: The history of theories of space in physics, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press Google Scholar
Jarvella, R. and Klein, W. (eds.) 1982, Speech, place and action: Studies in deixis and related topics, New York: Wiley Google Scholar
Jeannerod, M. 1997, The cognitive neuroscience of action, Oxford: Blackwell Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. 1983, Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. 1996, Space to think, in P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel and M. Garrett (eds.), Language and space, pp. 437–62. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Johnston, J. R. and Slobin, D. 1979, The development of locative expressions in English, Italian, Serbo-Croatian and Turkish, Journal of Child Language 6 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 529–45
Just, M. and Carpenter, P. 1985, Cognitive coordinate systems: Accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability, Psychological Review 92 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(2): 137–72
Kahr, J. 1975, Adpositions and locationals: typology and diachronic development, Working Papers on Language Universals 19 Google Scholar: 21–54. Stanford University
Kant, E. 1991 [1768], Von dem ersten Grunde des Unterschiedes der Gegenden im Raume. [Translated as: ‘On the first ground of the distinction of regions in space’.] In J. van Cleve and R. E. Frederick (eds.), The philosophy of right and left: Incongruent counterparts and the nature of space, pp. 27–34. Dordrecht: Kluwer Google Scholar
Kari, J. 1989, Some principles of Alaskan Athabaskan toponymic knowledge, in M. R. Key and H. Hoenigswald (eds.), General and Amerindian ethnolinguistics, pp. 129–50. Berlin: Mouton Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1992, Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Kay, P. and Kempton, W. 1984, What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis?American Anthropologist 86 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 65–79
Kay, P. and Maffi, L. 1999, Color appearance and the emergence and evolution of basic color lexicons, American Anthropologist 101 CrossRef | Google Scholar(4): 743–60
Kempson, R. 1977, Semantic theory, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Kendon, A. 1995, Gestures as illocutionary and discourse structure markers in Southern Italian conversation, Journal of Pragmatics 23 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 247–79
Kendon, A. 2000, Language and gesture: Unity or duality?, in D. McNeill (ed.), Language and gesture: Window into thought and action, pp. 47–63. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Kendon, A. and Versante, L. in press, Pointing by hand, in S. Kita (ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture and cognition meet, pp. 109–37. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Google Scholar
Kenesei, I., Vago, R. M. and Fenyvesi, A. 1997, Hungarian, London: Routledge Google Scholar
Kita, S. 1993, Language and thought interface: A study of spontaneous gestures and Japanese mimetics. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Chicago Google Scholar
Kita, S. 1998, Expressing a turn at an invisible location in route direction, in E. Hess-Lüttich, J. Müller and A. van Zoest (eds.), Signs and space; Raum und Zeichen, pp. 160–72. Tübingen: Gunter Narr Google Scholar
Kita, S. in press a, Interplay of gaze, hand, torso orientation and language in pointing, in Kita in press b, pp. 307–28 Google Scholar
Kita, S. (ed.) in press b, Pointing: Where language, culture and cognition meet, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Google Scholar
Kita, S., Danziger, E. and Stolz, C. 2001, Cultural specificity of spatial schemas, as manifested in spontaneous gestures, in M. Gattis (ed.), Spatial schemas and abstract thought, pp. 115–46. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Klatsky, R. and Lederman, S. 1993, Spatial and non-spatial avenues to object recognition by the human haptic system, in N. Eilan, R. McCarthy and B. Brewer (eds.), Spatial representation, pp. 191–205. Oxford: Blackwell Google Scholar
Kolb, B. and Whishaw, I. 1990, Fundamentals of human neuropsychology, New York: Freeman Google Scholar
Kölver, U. 1984, Local prepositions and serial verb constructions in Thai, Arbeiten des Kölner universalien Projekts 56 Google Scholar
Kornfilt, J. 1997, Turkish, ‘Croom Helm Descriptive Grammars’, London: Routledge Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. 1980, Image and mind, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press Google Scholar
Krauss, R. M., Morrel-Samuels, P. and Colasante, C. 1991, Do conversational hand gestures communicate?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(5): 743–54
Kuhl, P. 1991, Perception, cognition and the ontogenetic and phylogenetic emergence of human speech, in S. E. Brauth, W. S. Hall and R. J. Dooling (eds.), Plasticity of development, pp. 73–106. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Lahiri, T. K. 1965, Tracking techniques of Australian Aborigines. MS, Australian Institute for Aboriginal Studies, Canberra Google Scholar
Landau, B. and Gleitman, L. 1985, Language and experience: Evidence from the blind child, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press Google Scholar
Landau, B. and Jackendoff, R. 1993, ‘What’ and ‘where’ in spatial language and spatial cognition, Behavioural and Brain Sciences 16 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 217–38
Landau, B., Spelke, E. and Gleitman, L. 1984, Spatial knowledge in a young blind child, Cognition 16 CrossRef | Google Scholar(3): 225–60
Lang, E. 1989, The semantics of dimensional designation of spatial objects, in M. Bierwisch and E. Lang (eds.), Dimensional adjectives: Grammatical structure and conceptual interpretation, pp. 263–417. Berlin: Springer Google Scholar
Lang, E. 1995, Basic dimension terms: A first look at universal features and typological variation. Fachbereich Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft Working paper No. 1, Berlin Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. 1987, Foundations of cognitive grammar, Vol. I: Theoretical perspectives, Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. 1991, Foundations of cognitive grammar, Vol. II: Descriptive application, Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press Google Scholar
Langhorne, M. C. 1948, The effects of maze rotation on learning, Journal of General Psychology 38 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 191–205
Laughlin, R. 1975, The great Tzotzil dictionary of San Lorenzo Zinacantán, Washington, DC: Smithsonian Google Scholar
Laughren, M. 1978, Directional terminology in Warlpiri, in Th. Le and M. McCausland (eds.), Working papers in language and linguistics, 8: 1–16. Launceston: Tasmanian College of Advanced Education Google Scholar
Lee, D. N. and Thompson, J. 1982, Vision in action: The control of locomotion, in D. Ingle, M. A. Goodale and R. J. W. Mansfield (eds.), Analysis of visual behavior, pp. 411–36. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Leech, G. 1969, Towards a semantic description of English, London: Longmans Google Scholar
Leer, J. 1989, Directional systems in Athabaskan and Na-Dene, in E. Cook and K. D. Rice (eds.), Trends in linguistics, Vol. 15, pp. 575–622. Berlin: Mouton Google Scholar
León, L. de 1991, Space games in Tzotzil: Creating a context for spatial reference. Working Paper No. 4, Cognitive Anthropology Research Group, Max Planck Institute, Nijmegen Google Scholar
León, L. de 1993, Shape, geometry and location: The case of Tzotzil body part terms, in K. Beals et al. (eds.), CLS 29: Papers from the parasession on conceptual representations, pp. 77–90. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
León, L. de 1994, Exploration in the acquisition of location and trajectory in Tzotzil, in J. B. Haviland and S. C. Levinson (eds.), Space in Mayan Languages, Special issue of Linguistics 32(4/5): 857–84 Google Scholar
León, L. de 1996, The development of geocentric location in young speakers of Guugu Yimithirr. Working Paper No. 33, Cognitive Anthropology Research Group, Max Planck Institute, Nijmegen Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. 1984, Some perceptual limitations on talking about space, in A. J. van Doorn, W. A. van der Grind and J. J. Koenderink (eds.), Limits in perception, pp. 323–58. Utrecht: VNU Science Press Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. 1989, Speaking: From intention to articulation, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. 1996, Perspective taking and ellipsis in spatial descriptions, in P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel and M. Garrett (eds.), Language and space, pp. 77–108. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Levine, S. and Carey, S. 1982, Up front: The acquisition of a concept and a word, Journal of Child Language 9 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 645–57
Levinson, S. C. 1983, Pragmatics, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 1984/1986, The semantics/pragmatics/kinesics of space in Guugu Yimidhirr. Talk given to the International Conference of the Sociology of Language, Plymouth, June 1984, and to the University of Bamberg psychology department, 1986 Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 1987a, Minimization and conversational inference, in M. Pappi and J. Verschueren (eds.), The pragmatic perspective, pp. 61–129. Amsterdam: Benjamins Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 1987b, Pragmatics and the grammar of anaphora, Journal of Linguistics 23 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 379–434
Levinson, S. C. 1992a, Primer for the field investigation of spatial description and conception, Pragmatics 2 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 5–47
Levinson, S. C. 1992b, Language and cognition: The cognitive consequences of spatial descriptions in Guugu Yimithirr. Working Paper No. 13, Cognitive Anthropology Research Group, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 1994, Vision, shape and linguistic description: Tzeltal body-part terminology and object description, in J. B. Haviland and S. C. Levinson (eds.), Space in Mayan Languages, Special issue of Linguistics 32(4): 791–855 Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 1996a, Language and space, Annual Review of Anthropology 25 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 353–82
Levinson, S. C. 1996b, Frames of reference and Molyneux's question: Cross-linguistic evidence, in P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel and M. Garrett (eds.), Language and space, pp. 109–69. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 1996c, The body in space: Cultural differences in the use of body-schema for spatial thinking and gesture. Working Paper No. 39, Cognitive Anthropology Research Group, Max Planck Institute, Nijmegen Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 1996d, The role of language in everyday human navigation. Working Paper No. 38, Cognitive Anthropology Research Group, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 1997a, Language and cognition: The cognitive consequences of spatial description in Guugu Yimithirr, Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 7 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 98–131
Levinson, S. C. 1997b, From outer to inner space: Linguistic categories and non-linguistic thinking, in E. Pederson and J. Nuyts (eds.), Language and conceptualization, pp. 13–45. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 1999, H. P. Grice on location on Rossel Island, Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 25 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 210–24
Levinson, S. C. 2000a, Presumptive meanings, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 2000b, Yélî Dnye and the theory of basic color terms, Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 10 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 1–53
Levinson, S. C. 2000c, Language as nature and language as art, in R. Hide, J. Mittelstrass and W. Singer (eds.), Changing concepts of nature at the turn of the millennium, pp. 257–87. Vatican City: Pontifical Academy of Science Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 2001, Covariation between language and cognition, and its implications for language learning, in M. Bowerman and S. Levinson (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, pp. 566–88. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. in press, The body in space: Cultural differences in the use of body-schema for spatial thinking and gesture, in G. Lewis and F. Sigaut (eds.), Culture and the body, Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. and Brown, P. 1994, Immanuel Kant among the Tenejapans: Anthropology as empirical philosophy, Ethos 22 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 3–41
Levinson, S. C. and Wilkins, D. P. in preparation, Grammars of space Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C., Kita, S., Haun, D. and Rasch, B. 2002, Returning the tables: Language affects spatial reasoning, Cognition 84 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 155–88
Levy, P. 1994, La base verbal en Totonaco, in Carolyn J. MacKay and Veronica Vazquez (eds.), Investigaciones linguisticas en mesoamerica, Estudios sobre lenguas americanas 1, Mexico City: Seminario de Lenguas Google Scholar
Levy, P. 1999, From ‘part’ to ‘shape’: Incorporation in Totonac and the issue of classification by verbs, International Journal of American Linguistics 65 CrossRef | Google Scholar(2): 127–75
Lewis, D. 1972, We, the navigators, Canberra: ANU Press Google Scholar
Lewis, D. 1976, Route finding by desert aborigines in Australia, Journal of Navigation 29 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 21–38
Li, P. and Gleitman, L. 1999, Language and spatial reasoning. MS, University of Pennsylvania Google Scholar
Li, P. and Gleitman, L. 2002, Turning the tables: Language and spatial reasoning, Cognition 83 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 265–94
Lucy, J. 1992a, Language diversity and thought: A reformulation of the linguistic relativity hypothesis, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Lucy, J. 1992b, Grammatical categories and cognition: A case study of the linguistic relativity hypothesis, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Lucy, J. and Gaskins, S. 2001, Grammatical categories and the development of classification preferences: a comparative approach, in M. Bowerman and S. Levinson (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, pp. 257–83. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Lyons, J. 1977, Semantics, Vols. I and II, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Lyons, J. 1995, Linguistic semantics: An introduction, London and New York: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
MacDonald, J. 1998, The arctic sky, Toronto: Nunuavut Research Institute, Royal Ontario Museum Google Scholar
MacLaury, R. 1989, Zapotec body-part locatives: Prototypes and metaphoric extensions, International Journal of American Linguistics 55 CrossRef | Google Scholar(2): 119–54
Maddieson, I. 1984, Patterns of sounds, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Maguire, E., Gadian, D., Johnsrude, I., Good, C., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. and Frith, C. 2000, Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(8): 4398–403
Mallot, H. A. 1996, Action-perception learning: A scheme for learning cognitive maps from sequences of movements and views. Talk given at the 3rd Annual CBR Workshop on ‘Mental Representations in Navigation’. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge Basic Research Google Scholar
Marascuilo, L. A. and Serlin, R. C. 1988, Statistical methods for the social and behavioral sciences, New York: Freeman Google Scholar
Marr, D. 1982, Vision, New York: Freeman Google Scholar
McCarthy, R. 1993, Assembling routines and addressing representations: An alternative conceptualization of ‘what’ and ‘where’ in the human brain, in N. Eilan, R. McCarthy and B. Brewer (eds.), Spatial representation: Problems in philosophy and psychology, pp. 373–99. Oxford: Blackwell Google Scholar
McCullough, K.-E. 1993, Spatial information and cohesion in the gesticulation of English and Chinese speakers. Paper presented to the American Psychological Society, Chicago Google Scholar
McDonough, L., Choi, S. and Mandler, J. in press, Understanding spatial relations: Flexible infants, lexical adults. Cognitive Psychology Google Scholar
McNaughton, B., Chen, L. and Markus, E. 1990, ‘Dead reckoning’, landmark learning and the sense of direction: A neurophysiological and computational hypothesis, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 3 Google Scholar(2): 191–202
McNeill, D. 1992, Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (ed.) 2000, Language and gesture: Window into thought and action, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
McNeill, D. and Levy, E. 1982, Conceptual representations in language activity and gesture, in R. Jarvella and W. Klein (eds.), Speech, place, and action, pp. 271–95. Chichester: Wiley Google Scholar
Meira, S. in preparation, Approaching space in Tiriyó grammar, in S. C. Levinson and D. Wilkins (in preparation), Grammars of space Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. 1993, Molyneux's babies: Cross-modal perception, imitation and the mind of the preverbal infant, in N. Eilan, R. McCarthy and B. Brewer (eds.), Spatial representation, pp. 219–35. Oxford: Blackwell Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. 1956, The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information, Psychological Review 63 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(2): 81–97
Miller, G. A. 1982, Some problems in the theory of demonstrative reference, in R. Jarvella and W. Klein (eds.), Speech, place and action, pp. 61–72. New York: Wiley Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. and Johnson-Laird, P. N. 1976, Language and perception, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press Google Scholar
Mithun, M. 1999, The languages of native North America, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Moar, I. and Bower, G. 1983, Inconsistency in spatial knowledge, Memory & Cognition 11 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(2): 107–13
Moar, I. and Carleton, L. R. 1982, Memory for routes, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 34A CrossRef | Google Scholar: 381–94
Morrel-Samuels, P. and Krauss, R. 1992, Word familiarity predicts temporal asychrony of hand gestures and speech, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 18 Google Scholar(3): 615–22
Müller, C. 1998, Redebegleitende Gesten: Kulturgeschichte – Theorie Sprachvergleich, Berlin: Arno Spitz Verlag Google Scholar
Müller, M. and Wehner, R. 1994, The hidden spiral: Systematic search and path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis, Journal of Comparative Physiology A175 Google Scholar: 525–30
Munn, N. 1986, Walbiri iconography, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Nash, D. 1993, Notes on way-finding in Australia's deserts by Warlpiri and their neighbours. Paper presented at the Conference on Spatial Representation, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen Google Scholar
Needham, R. 1973, Left and right, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Neumann, S. and Widlok, T. 1996, Rethinking some universals of spatial language using controlled comparison, in R. Dirven and M. Pütz (eds.), The construal of space in language and thought, pp. 345–72. Berlin: de Gruyter Google Scholar
Norman, J. 1988, Chinese, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Nunberg, G. 1978, The pragmatics of reference. PhD dissertation, University of New York Google Scholar
O'Keefe, J. 1991, The hippocampal cognitive map and navigational strategies, in J. Paillard (ed.), Brain and space, pp. 273–95. Oxford: Oxford Science Publications Google Scholar
O'Keefe, J. 1993, Kant and the sea-horse: An essay in the neurophilosophy of space, in N. Eilan, R. McCarthy and B. Brewer (eds.), Spatial representation, pp. 43–64. Oxford: Blackwell Google Scholar
O'Keefe, J. 1996, The spatial preposition in English, Vector grammar, and the cognitive map theory, in P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel and M. Garrett (eds.), Language and space, pp. 277–316. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
O'Keefe, J. and Nadel, L. 1978, The hippocampus as a cognitive map, Oxford: Clarendon Press Google Scholar
Ozanne-Rivièrre, F. 1987, L'expression linguistique de l'espace: Quelques exemples oceaniens, Cahiers du Lacito 2 Google Scholar: 129–55
Ozanne-Rivièrre, F. 1997, Spatial reference in New Caledonian languages, in G. Senft (ed.), Referring to space: Studies in Austronesian and Papuan languages, pp. 83–100. Oxford: Clarendon Press Google Scholar
Özyürek, A. and Kita, S. 1999, Expressing manner and path in English and Turkish: Differences in speech, gesture, and conceptualization, in M. Hahn and S. C. Stoness (eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-first annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 507–12. Mahwah, NJ and London: Lawrence Erlbaum Google Scholar
Paillard, J. (ed.) 1991, Brain and space, Oxford: Oxford Science Publications Google Scholar
Pederson, E. 1993, Geographic and manipulable space in two Tamil linguistic systems, in A. U. Frank and I. Campari (eds.), Spatial information theory, pp. 294–311. Berlin: Springer Google Scholar
Pederson, E. 1995, Language as context, language as means: Spatial cognition and habitual language use, Cognitive Linguistics 6 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 33–62
Pederson, E. and Roelofs, A. (eds.) 1995, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics annual report 1994. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute Google Scholar
Pederson, E., Danziger, E., Wilkins, D., Levinson, S., Kita, S. and Senft, G. 1998, Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization, Language 74 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 557–89
Peterson, M., Nadel, L., Bloom, P. and Garrett, M. 1996, Space and language. In P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel and M. Garrett (eds.), Language and space, pp. 553–77. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1928, Judgment and reasoning in the child, London: Routledge Google Scholar
Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. 1956 [1948], The child's conception of space, London: Routledge and Kegan Paul Google Scholar
Pick, H. L., Jr 1988, Perceptual aspects of spatial cognitive development, in J. Stiles-Davis, M. Kritchevsky and U. Bellugi (eds.), Spatial cognition: Brain bases and development, pp. 145–56. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Google Scholar
Pick, H. L., Jr 1993, Organization of spatial knowledge in children, in N. Eilan, R. McCarthy and B. Brewer (eds.), Spatial representation, pp. 31–42. Oxford: Blackwell Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 1989, Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure, Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 1994, The language instinct, New York: Morrow Google Scholar
Pinker, S. and Bloom, P. 1992, Natural language and natural selection, in J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides and J. Tooby (eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, pp. 451–94. New York: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Poincaré, H. 1946, The foundations of science (Trans. G. B. Halsted), Lancaster, PA: Science Press Google Scholar
Potter, M. 1990, Remembering, in D. N. Osherson and E. S. Smith (eds.), Thinking: An invitation to cognitive science, Vol. III, pp. 3–32. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Putnam, H. 1988, Representation and reality, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G. and Svartvik, J. 1991, A comprehensive grammar of the English language (rev. edn), London: Longman Google Scholar
Reichenbach, H. 1958, The philosophy of space and time, New York: Dover Google Scholar
Renzi, E. de 1982, Disorders of space exploration and cognition, New York: Wiley Google Scholar
Rock, I. 1990, The frame of reference, in I. Rock (ed.), The legacy of Soloman Asch, pp. 243–68. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Google Scholar
Rock, I. 1992, Comment on Asch and Witkin's ‘Studies in space orientation II’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 121 CrossRef | Google Scholar(4): 404–6
Rock, I., Wheeler, D. and Tudor, L. 1989, Can we imagine how objects look from other viewpoints?Cognitive Psychology 21 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 185–210
Ruiter, J. P. A. de and Wilkins, D. P. 1998, The synchronization of gesture and speech in Dutch and Arrernte, in S. Santi, I. Guaïtella, C. Cavé and G. Konopczynski (eds.), Oralité et gestualité: Communication multimodale, interaction, pp. 603–7. Paris: L'Harmattan Google Scholar
Save, E., Poucet, B. and Thinus-Blanc, C. 1998, Landmark use and the cognitive map in the rat, in S. Healy (ed.), Spatial representation in animals, pp. 119–32. Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. 1984, On some gestures' relation to speech, in J. Atkinson and J. Heritage (eds.), Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis, pp. 266–96. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Schöne, H. 1984, Spatial orientation, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press Google Scholar
Schultze-Berndt, E. 2000, Simple and complex verbs in Jaminjung (Max Planck Institute Series, Doctoral Dissertations in Psycholinguistics), Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute Google Scholar
Schultze-Berndt, E. in preparation, Sketch of Jaminjung grammar of space, in S. C. Levinson and D. Wilkins (in preparation), Grammars of space Google Scholar
Searle, J. 1969, Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Senft, G. 1994a, Spatial reference in Kilivila: The Tinkertoy matching games– A case study, Language and Linguistics in Melanesia 25 Google Scholar: 98–9
Senft, G. 1994b, Ein Vorschlag, wie man standardisiert Daten zum Thema ‘Sprache, Kognition und Konzepte des Raumes’ in verschiedenen Kulturen erheben kann, Linguistische Berichte 154 Google Scholar: 413–29
Senft, G. 1995, Sprache, Kognition und Konzepte des Raumes in verschiedenen Kulturen, Kognitionswissenschaft 4 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 166–70
Senft, G. 2001, Frames of spatial reference in Kilivila, Studies in Language 25 CrossRef | Google Scholar(3): 521–55
Senft, G. (ed.) 1997, Referring to space: Studies in Austronesian and Papuan languages, Oxford: Clarendon Press Google Scholar
Senft, G. and Wilkins, D. P. 1994, A man, a tree, and forget about the pigs: Space games, spatial reference and an attempt to identify functional equivalents across languages. Talk given at the 19th LAUD Conference, Duisburg, March Google Scholar
Senghas, A. 2000, Differences between first and second cohort signers in communicating orientation. Poster session at the 7th International Conference on Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research, Amsterdam, 23–7 July Google Scholar
Shephard, R. and Metzler, J. 1971, Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects, Science 171 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 701–3
Sherry, D. and Healy, S. 1998, Neural mechanisms of spatial representation, in S. Healy (ed.), Spatial representation in animals, pp. 133–58. Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 1996, From ‘thought and language’ to ‘thinking for speaking’, in J. Gumperz and S. Levinson (eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity, pp. 70–96. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 2001, Form-function relations: How do children find out what they are?, in M. Bowerman and S. Levinson (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, pp. 406–49. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (ed.) 1985, The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. II: The data, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Google Scholar
Sorabji, R. 1988, Matter, space and motion: Theories in antiquity and their sequel, London: Duckworth Google Scholar
Spelke, E. and Tsivkin, S. 2001, Initial knowledge and conceptual change: Space and number, in M. Bowerman and S. C. Levinson (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, pp. 70–97. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Sperber, D. 1996, Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach, Oxford: Blackwell Google Scholar
Squire, L. R. 1992, Declarative and nondeclarative memory – multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 4 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(3): 232–43
Staden, M. van 2000, Tidore: a linguistic description of a language of the North Moluccas. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Leiden Google Scholar
Stassen, L. 1997, Intransitive predication (Oxford studies in typology and linguistic theory), Oxford: Clarendon Press Google Scholar
Stein, J. F. 1992, The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(4): 691–700
Steinhauer, H. 1991, Demonstratives in the Blagar language of Dolap, in T. Dutton (ed.), Papers in papuan linguistics, Vol. I, pp. 177–221. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics A73 Google Scholar
Stephens, D. 1983, Hemispheric language dominance and gesture hand preference. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Chicago Google Scholar
Stolz, C. 1996, Spatial dimensions and orientation of objects in Yucatec Maya. PhD dissertation, University of Bielefeld Google Scholar
Stross, B. 1991, Classic Maya directional glyphs, Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 1 CrossRef | Google Scholar(1): 97–114
Stutterheim, C. and Carroll, M. 1993, Raumkonzepte in Produktions-prozessen, Kognitionswissenschaft 3 Google Scholar: 1–13
Sutton, P. 1998, Aboriginal maps and plans, in D. Woodward and G. M. Lewis (eds.), Cartography in the traditional African, American, Arctic, Australian and Pacific societies, Vol. II(3), pp. 387–418. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Svorou, S. 1994, The grammar of space, Amsterdam: Benjamins Google Scholar
Takano, Y. 1989, Perception of rotated forms: A theory of information types, Cognitive Psychology 21 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 1–59
Talmy, L. 1983, How language structures space, in H. Pick and L. Acredolo (eds.), Spatial orientation: Theory, research and application, pp. 225–82. New York: Plenum Press Google Scholar
Talmy, L. 1985, Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms, in T. Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, Vol. III: Grammatical categories and the lexicon, pp. 56–149. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Google Scholar
Talmy, L. 1988, Force dynamics in language and cognition, Cognitive Science 12 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 49–100
Talmy, L. 2000, Toward a cognitive semantics, Vols. I and II, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Tarr, M. and Pinker, S. 1989, Mental rotation and orientation-dependence in shape recognition, Cognitive Psychology 21 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed: 233–82
Tindale, N. 1974, Aboriginal tribes of Australia: Their terrain, environmental controls, distributions, limits and proper names, Berkeley, CA: University of California Press Google Scholar
Tinkelpaugh, O. L. 1928, An experimental study of representative factors in monkeys, Journal of Comparative Psychology 8 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 197–236
Tolman, E. C. 1948, Cognitive maps in rats and men, The Psychological Review 55 CrossRef | Google Scholar | PubMed(4): 109–45
Tomasello, M. and Call, J. 1997, Primate cognition, New York: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. 1992, The psychological foundations of culture, in J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides and J. Tooby (eds.), The adapted mind, pp. 19–136. Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Tversky, B. 1981, Distortions in memory for maps, Cognitive Psychology 13 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 407–33
Tversky, B. 1991, Spatial mental models, in G. H. Bower (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. XXVII, pp. 109–45. New York: Academic Press Google Scholar
Tversky, B. 1996, Language and spatial cognition, International Journal of Psychology 31 Google Scholar(3–4): 3010
Tversky, B. 1998, Three dimensions of spatial cognition, in M. A. Conway, S. E. Gathercole and C. Cornoldi (eds.), Theories of memory, pp. 259–76. Hove: Psychology Press Google Scholar
Tversky, B. and Taylor, H. A. 1998, Acquiring spatial and temporal knowledge from language, in M. J. Egenhofer and R. G. Golledge (eds.), Spatial and temporal reasoning in geographic information systems, pp. 155–66. Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Tye, M. 1991, The imagery debate: Representation and mind, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Ulltan, R. 1978, Some general characteristics of interrogative systems, in J. Greenberg (ed.), Universals of human language, Vol. IV, pp. 211–48. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press Google Scholar
Ungerer, F. and Schmid, H. 1996, An introduction to cognitive linguistics, London: Longman Google Scholar
Ungerleider, L. G. and Mishkin, M. 1982, Two cortical visual systems, in D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale and R. J. W. Mansfield (eds.), Analysis of visual behavior, pp. 549–86. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Van Cleve, J. and Frederick, R. 1991, The philosophy of right and left, Dordrecht: Kluwer Google Scholar
Vandeloise, C. 1991, Spatial prepositions: A case study from French, Chicago: University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. 1986, Thought and language (rev. edn), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Wassmann, J. and Dasen, P. 1998, Balinese spatial orientation: Some empirical evidence of moderate linguistic relativity, Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (inc. MAN) 4 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 689–711
Waterman, T. H. 1989, Animal navigation, Scientific American Library. Houndmills, UK: Palgrave MacMillan Google Scholar
Wehner, R. 1983, Celestial and terrestrial navigation: Human strategies – insect strategies, in F. Huber and H. Markl (eds.), Neuroethology and behavioral physiology, pp. 366–81. Berlin: Springer Google Scholar
Weissenborn, J. 1984, La genèse de la référence spatiale en langue maternelle et en langue seconde: Similarités et différences, in G. Extra and M. Mittner (eds.), Studies in second language acquisition by adult immigrants, Proceedings of the ESF/AILA symposium, pp. 262–72. Tilburg: Tilburg University Google Scholar
Weissenborn, J. and Klein, W. (eds.) 1982, Here and there: Cross-linguistic studies on deixis and demonstration, Amsterdam: Benjamins Google Scholar
Weissenborn, J. and Stralka, R. 1984, Das Verstehen von Missverständnissen: Eine ontogenetische Studie, Zeitschrift für Literaturwissenschaft und Linguistik Google Scholar, pp. 113–34
Werker, J. and Tees, R. 1984, Cross language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization in the first year of life, Infant Behavior and Development 7 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 49–63
Whorf, B. L. 1956, Language, thought and reality, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Google Scholar
Widlok, T. 1994, The social relationships of changing Hai//om hunter/gatherers in Northern Namibia, 1990–1994. PhD dissertation, University of London Google Scholar
Widlok, T. 1996, Topographical gossip and the indexicality of Hai//om environmental knowledge. Working Paper No. 37, Cognitive Anthropology Research Group, Max Planck Institute, Nijmegen Google Scholar
Widlok, T. 1997, Orientation in the wild: the shared cognition of Hai//om Bushpeople, Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 3 CrossRef | Google Scholar: 317–32
Widlok, T. 1999, Living in Mangetti, Oxford: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Wilkins, D. P. 1989, Mparntwe Arrente (Aranda): Studies in the structure and semantics of grammar. PhD dissertation, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australia Google Scholar
Wilkins, D. P. 1997, Alternative representations of space: Arrernte narratives in sand and sign, in M. Biemans and J. v.d. Weijer (eds.), Proceedings of the CLS Opening Academic Year '97–'98, pp. 133–62. Nijmegen: Centre for Language Studies Google Scholar
Wilkins, D. P. 1999, Spatial deixis in Arrernte speech and gesture: On the analysis of a species of composite signal as used by a Central Australian Aboriginal group, in E. Andre, M. Poesio and H. Riser (eds.), Proceedings of the Workshop on Deixis, Demonstration and Deictic Belief in Multimedia Contexts, pp. 30–42. Utrecht: ESSLLI XI/FOLLI Google Scholar
Wilkins, D. P. in press, Why pointing with the index finger is not a universal, in S. Kita (ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture and cognition meet, pp. 171–215. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Google Scholar
Woodward, D. 1987, Medieval Mappaemundi, in J. B. Harley and D. Woodward (eds.), The history of cartography, Vol. I, pp. 286–370. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Yates, F. A. 1966, The art of memory, Chicago: The University of Chicago Press Google Scholar
Zee, E. van der 1996, Spatial knowledge and spatial language: A theoretical and empirical investigation (Ruimtelijke kennis en ruimtelijk taalgebruik: Een empirisch onderzoek). PhD dissertation, University of Utrecht Google Scholar

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 7333 *
Loading metrics...

Book summary page views

Total views: 16922 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between September 2016 - 29th April 2025. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.