Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T04:06:02.316Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Biological and Social Signaling Systems

from Part I - Conceptual Models of Social Signals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 July 2017

Kory Floyd
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
Valerie Manusov
Affiliation:
University of Washington
Judee K. Burgoon
Affiliation:
University of Arizona
Nadia Magnenat-Thalmann
Affiliation:
Université de Genève
Maja Pantic
Affiliation:
Imperial College London
Alessandro Vinciarelli
Affiliation:
University of Glasgow
Get access

Summary

As complex beings, humans communicate in complex ways, relying on a range of faculties to encode and decode social messages. Some aptitudes are innate, based on one's biological characteristics, whereas others are acquired, varying according to one's social and cultural experiences. As we explain in this chapter, each of us uses a combination of biological and sociocultural processes to produce and interpret social signals. Our goal is to introduce some of the forms that these processes can take.

We begin this chapter with an overview of social signals and a comparison between the biological and sociocultural processes underlying their production and interpretation. Next, we explore three examples of biologically processed social signals, and then examine sociocultural processing of the same signals. We conclude the chapter by discussing some ways in which biological and sociocultural processes interact.

The Nature of Social Signals

Communicators depend on a wide variety of social signals to make sense of the world around them. Poggi and D'Errico (2011) define a signal as “any perceivable stimulus from which a system can draw some meaning” and a social signal as “a communicative or informative signal which, either directly or indirectly, provides information about ‘social facts,’ that is, about social interactions, social attitudes, social relations and social emotions” (Poggi & D'Errico, 2011: 189). Social interactions are situations in which people perform reciprocal social actions, such as a game, a surgical procedure, an orchestral performance, or a conflict. Social attitudes are people's tendencies to behave in a particular way toward another person or group and include elements such as beliefs, opinions, evaluations, and emotions. Social relations are relationships of interdependent goals between two or more people. Finally, social emotions include those emotions that (1) we feel toward someone else, such as admiration and envy; (2) are easily transmitted from one person to another, such as enthusiasm and panic; and/or (3) are self-conscious, such as pride and shame.

As noted, humans use both biological and sociocultural processes to produce and interpret social signals.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbey, A. & Melby, C. (1986). The effect of nonverbal cues on gender differences in perceptions of sexual intent. Sex Roles, 15, 283–298.Google Scholar
Altmann, J., Hausfater, G., & Altmann, S. A. (1988). Determinants of reproductive success in savannah baboons, Papio cynocephalus. In T. H., Clutton-Brock (Ed.), Reproductive Success: Studies of Individual Variation in Contrasting Breeding Systems (pp. 403–418). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Aune, K, S. (2005). Assessing display rules in relationships. In V.Manusov (Ed.), The Sourcebook of Nonverbal Measures: Going BeyondWords (pp. 151–161).Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bennett, S. & Montero-Diaz, L. (1982). Children's perceptions of speaker sex. Journal of Phonetics, 10, 113–121.Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., & Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology, 45, 602–607. doi: 10.1111/j.1469- 8986.2008.00654.x.Google Scholar
Brown, D. E. & Perrett, D. I. (1993). What gives a face its gender? Perception, 22, 829–840. doi: 10.1068/p220829.Google Scholar
Buck, R. (1989). Emotional communication in personal relationships: A developmentalinteractionist view. In C. D, Hendrick (Ed.), Close Relationships: Review of Personality and Social Psychology (vol. 10, pp. 144–163). Newbury Park: SAGE.
Bull, R. & Shead, G. (1979). Pupil dilation, sex of stimulus, and age and sex of observer. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49, 27–30. doi: 10/2466/pms.1979.49.1.27.Google Scholar
Coupland, J. (2003). Ageist ideology and discourses of control in skincare product marketing. In J., Coupland & R., Gwyn (Eds), Discourse, the Body and Identity (pp. 127–150). Basingstoke, England: Palgrave Macmillan.
Darwin, C. R. (1873). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray.
Duke, M. & Nowicki, S. (2005). The Emory Dissemia Index. In V., Manusov (Ed.), The Sourcebook of Nonverbal Measures: Going Beyond Words (pp. 25–46). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Egland, K. L., Spitzberg, B. H., & Zormeier, M. M. (1996). Flirtation and conversational competence in cross-sex platonic and romantic relationships. Communication Reports, 9, 105–117.Google Scholar
Ekins, R. (1997). Male Femaling: A Grounded Theory Approach to Cross-dressing and Sex Changing. New York: Routledge.
Ekman, P. (1977). Biological and cultural contributions to body and facial movement. In J., Blacking (Ed.), Anthropology of the Body (pp. 34–84). London: Academic Press.
Galati, D., Scherer, K. R., & Ricci-Bitti, P. E. (1997). Voluntary facial expression of emotion: Comparing congenitally blind with normally sighted encoders. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1363–1379.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1976). Gender Advertisements. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Goldberg, L. S. & Grandey, A. A. (2007). Display rules versus display autonomy: Emotion regulation, emotional exhaustion, and task performance in a call center situation. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12, 301–318. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.12.3.301.Google Scholar
Gray, H. & Goss, C. M. (1966). Anatomy of the Human Body, (28th edn). Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.
Hall, J. A. (2006).Women's and men's nonverbal communication: Similarities, differences, stereotypes, and origins. In V., Manusov & M. L., Patterson (Eds), The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication (pp. 201–218). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Hall, S. (1997). Representation: Cultural Representations and Signifying Practices. London: SAGE.
Hancock, A. B. & Garabedian, L. M. (2013). Transgender voice and communication treatment: A retrospective chart review of 25 cases. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 48, 54–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00185.x.Google Scholar
Herman-Giddens, M. E. (2006). Recent data on pubertal milestones in United States children: The secular trend toward earlier development. International Journal of Andrology, 29, 241– 246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2605.2005.00575.x.Google Scholar
Houle, R. & Feldman, R. S. (1991). Emotional displays in children's television programming. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 261–271.Google Scholar
Janssen, E. & Everaerd, W. (1993). Determinants of male sexual arousal. Annual Review of Sex Research, 4, 211–245. doi: 10.1080/10532528.1993.10559888.Google Scholar
Jaworski, A. (2003). Talking bodies: Representations of norm and deviance in the BBCNaked programme. In J, Coupland & R, Gwyn (Eds), Discourse, the Body and Identity (pp. 151–176). Basingstoke, England: Palgrave Macmillan.
Johnson, K. L. & Tassinary, L. G. (2005). Perceiving sex directly and indirectly: Meaning in motion and morphology. Psychological Science, 16, 890–897. doi: 10.1111/j.1467- 9280.2005.01633.x.Google Scholar
Koppensteiner, M. & Grammer, K. (2011). Body movements of male and female speakers and their influence on perceptions of personality.Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 743– 747. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.014.Google Scholar
Laan, E., Everaerd, W., & Evers, A. (1995). Assessment of female sexual arousal: Response specificity and construct validity. Psychophysiology, 32, 476–485. doi: 10/1111/j.1469- 8986.1995.tb02099.x.Google Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (2003). Autonomic specificity and emotion. In R. J., Davidson, K. R., Scherer, & H. H, Goldsmith (Eds), Handbook of Affective Sciences (pp. 212–224). New York: Oxford University Press.
Mah, K. & Binik, Y. M. (2001). The nature of human orgasm: A critical review of major trends. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 823–856. doi: 10/1016/S0272.7358(00)00069-6.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., Keltner, D., Shiota, M., Frank, M., & O'Sullivan, M. (2008). Facial expressions of emotion. In M., Lewis, J., Haviland, & L., Feldman-Barrett (Eds), Handbook of Emotion (pp. 211–234). New York: Guilford Press.
Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., & Fontaine, J. (2008). Mapping expressive differences around the world: The relationship between emotional display rules and individualism versus collectivism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 55–74. doi: 10.1177/0022022107311854.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., Hirayama, S., & Petrova, G. (2005). Validation of an individuallevel measure of display rules: The display rule assessment invesntory (DRAI). Emotions, 5, 23–40.Google Scholar
Monstrey, S. J., Buncamper, M., Bouman, M.-B., & Hoebeke, P. (2014). Surgical interventions for gender dysphoria. In B. P. C., Kreukels, T. D., Steensma, & A. L. C. de, Vries (Eds), Gender Dysphoria and Disorders of Sex Development (pp. 299–318). New York: Springer.
Panksepp, J., Knutson, B., & Pruitt, D. L. (1998). Toward a neuroscience of emotion: The epigenetic foundations of emotional development. In M. F., Mascolo &S., Griffin (Eds), What Develops in Emotional Development? Emotions, Personality, and Psychotherapy (pp. 53–84). New York: Plenum Press.
Parr, L. A., Waller, B. M., & Fugate, J. (2005). Emotional communication in primates: Implications for neurobiology. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 716–720. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.017.Google Scholar
Perper, T. & Weis, D. L. (1987). Proceptive and rejective strategies of US and Canadian college women. Journal of Sex Research, 23, 455–480.Google Scholar
Philipsen, G. (2009). Researching culture in contexts of social interaction: An ethnographic approach, a network of scholars, illustrative moves. In D., Carbaugh & P. M., Buzzanell (Eds), Distinctive Qualities in Communication Research (pp. 87–105). New York: Routledge.
Poggi, I. & D'Errico, F. (2011). Social signals: A psychological perspective. In A. A., Salah & T., Gevers (Eds), Computer Analysis of Human Behavior (pp. 185–225). London: Springer.
Pusch, R. S. (2005). Objects of curiosity: Transgender college students' perceptions of the reactions of others. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Issues in Education, 3, 45–61. doi: 10.1300/J367v03n01_06.Google Scholar
Riggio, R. E. & Riggio, H. R. (2005). Self-report measures of emotional and nonverbal expressiveness. In V., Manusov(Ed.), The Sourcebook of Nonverbal Measures: Going Beyond Words (pp. 105–111). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Roy, A. (2005). The “male gaze” in Indian television commercials: A rhetorical analysis. In T., Carilli & J., Campbell (Eds), Women and the Media: National and Global Perspectives (pp. 3–18). Lanham, MD: University Press of America.
Schegloff, E. (1984). On some gestures' relation to talk. In J. M., Atkinson & J., Heritage (Eds), Structures of Social Action (pp. 266–296). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sherar, L. B., Baxter-Jones, A. D. G., & Mirwald, R. L. (2004). Limitations to the use of secondary sex characteristics for gender comparisons. Annals of Human Biology, 31, 586–593. doi: 10.1080/03014460400001222.Google Scholar
Sugden, K., Arseneault, L., Harrington et al. (2010). The serotonin transporter gene moderates the development of emotional problems among children following bullying victimization. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 830–840. doi: 10/1016/j.jaac.2010.01.024.
Susman, E. J., Houts, R. M., Steinberg, L., et al. (2010). Longitudinal development of secondary sexual characteristics in girls and boys between ages 91/2 and 151/2 years. JAMA Pediatrics, 164, 166–173. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.261.Google Scholar
Tombs, S. & Silverman, I. (2004). Pupillometry: A sexual selection approach. Evolution & Human Behavior, 25, 221–228. doi: 10/1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.05.001.Google Scholar
Tucker, D. M., Derryberry, D., & Luu, P. (2000). Anatomy and physiology of human emotions: Vertical integration of brainstem, limbic, and cortical systems. In J. C., Borod (Ed.), The Neuropsychology of Emotions (pp. 80–105). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., & Chung, J. (2010). The expression of anger across cultures. In M., Potegal, G., Stemmler, & C., Spielberger (Eds), International Handbook of Anger (pp. 125–137). New York, NY: Springer.
Simpson, B. S. (1997). Canine communication. The Veterinary Clinics of North America, Small Animal Practice, 27, 445–464.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×