Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T01:08:21.330Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

24 - Learning by Diffusion

Using Diffusion Experiments and Social Network Analysis to Understand the Dynamics of Cultural Evolution

from Part V - Learning and Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 October 2016

Sukhvinder S. Obhi
Affiliation:
McMaster University, Ontario
Emily S. Cross
Affiliation:
Bangor University
Get access

Summary

Abstract

Culture arises from the interaction of many individuals sharing knowledge and collaborating over time, and, because of this, culture must be studied using different methods to those that are commonly employed in many areas of psychology. The majority of our understanding of the social learning underpinning culture is generalised from ‘dyadic’ experiments, in which a single participant observes a single model, and as a result leaves questions about the relationship between the individual- and group-level unaddressed. Such questions include how different forms of cultural information spread across the population and how individuals work together to produce cultural products. Diffusion experiments present a method for such dynamics to be examined. This chapter reviews the varieties of diffusion methods available and the strengths and weaknesses each type of diffusion design provides in answering questions about cultural evolution. It also reviews recent innovations in studying the spread of culture via social relations using social network analyses. We argue that social network analyses could be especially useful for examining a dynamic which has hitherto not been widely considered in studying cultural evolution; that is, the feedback relationship between social structure (relations) and social learning, with copying behaviour fulfilling both a role of information exchange and a role of affiliation.

Type
Chapter
Information
Shared Representations
Sensorimotor Foundations of Social Life
, pp. 503 - 524
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, J., Weinrich, M., Hoppitt, W., & Rendell, L. (2013). Network-based diffusion analysis reveals cultural transmission of lobtail feeding in humpback whales. Science, 340, 485488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atton, N., Hoppitt, W., Webster, M. M., Galef, B. G., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Information flow through threespine stickleback networks without social transmission. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 42724278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baaren, R. van, Janssen, L., Chartrand, T. L., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2009). Where is love? The social aspects of mimicry. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 23812389. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1932) Remembering. Oxford: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Bergen, Y. van, Coolen, I., & Laland, K. (2004). Nine-spined sticklebacks exploit the most reliable source when public and private information conflict. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 271, 957962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 1091810925. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100290108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. (2008). Experimental models for testing hypotheses about cumulative cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 29, 165171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, C. A., (2009). Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution: Is imitation necessary? Psychological Science, 20(12), 14781483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cantor, M., & Whitehead, H. (2013). The interplay between social networks and culture: Theoretically and among whales and dolphins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368, 14712970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Centola, D., González-Avella, J. C., Eguíluz, V. M., & San Miguel, M. (2007). Homophily, cultural drift, and the co-evolution of cultural groups. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 51, 905929. doi: 10.1177/0022002707307632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chudek, M., Heller, S., Birch, S. & Henrich, J. (2012). Prestige-based cultural learning: Bystander’s differential attention to potential models influences children’s learning. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 33, 4656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claidiere, N., Messer, E., Hoppitt, W., & Whiten, A. (2013). Diffusion dynamics of socially learning foraging techniques in squirrel monkeys. Current Biology, 23, 12511255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335(6072), 11141118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dean, L., Vale, G. L., Laland, K. N., Flynn, E. G., & Kendal, R. L. (2014). Human cumulative culture: A comparative perspective. Biological Reviews, 89, 284301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 629636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeWalt, K. M., DeWalt, B. R. & Wayland, C. B. (1998). Participant observation. In Bernard, H. R. (Ed.), Handbook of methods in cultural anthropology. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press, 259299.Google Scholar
Flynn, E. (2008). Investigating children as cultural magnets: Do young children transmit redundant information along diffusion chains? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 35413551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, E., Laland, K. N., Kendal, R. L., & Kendal, J. R. (2013). Developmental niche construction. Developmental Science, 16(2), 296313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flynn, E., & Siegler, R. (2007). Measuring change: Current trends and future directions in microgenetic research. Infant and Child Development, 16, 135149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, E., & Smith, K. (2012). Investigating the mechanisms of cultural acquisition: How pervasive is overimitation in adults? Social Psychology, 43, 185195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2010). Studying children’s social learning experimentally ‘in the wild’. Learning & Behavior, 38, 284296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2012). Experimental ‘microcultures’ in young children: Identifying biographic, cognitive, and social predictors of information transmission. Child Development, 83(3), 911925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2013). Dissecting children’s observational learning of complex actions through selective video displays. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 247263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forsyth, D. R. (2006). Group dynamics. Belmont, CA: Thomson-Wadworth.Google Scholar
Fu, F., Nowak, M., Christakis, N., & Fowler, J. (2012). The evolution of homophily. Scientific Reports, 2, 845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giraldeau, L.-A., Valone, T., & Templeton, J. (2002). Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 357, 15591566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griskevicius, V., Goldstein, N., Mortensen, C., Cialdini, R. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Going along versus going alone: When fundamental motives facilitate strategic (non)conformity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 281294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heath, C., Bell, C., & Sternberg, E. (2001). Emotional selection in memes: The case of urban legends. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 10281041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 22, 165196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., & Henrich, N. (2010). The evolution of cultural adaptations: Fijian food taboos protect against dangerous marine toxins. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277, 37153724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 123135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernàndez-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 13601366. doi: 10.1126/science.1146282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, K., Barton, M., & Hurtado, A. M. (2009). The emergence of human uniqueness: Characters underlying behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 18, 187200. doi: 10.1002/evan.20224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. (2011). Detecting social learning using networks: A user’s guide. American Journal of Primatology, 73, 834844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoppitt, W., (2013). Social learning: An introduction to mechanisms, methods, and models. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Horner, V., Whiten, A., Flynn, E., & de Waal, F. (2006). Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(37), 1387813883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobs, R. C., & Campbell, D. T. (1961). The perpetuation of an arbitrary tradition through several generations of a laboratory microculture. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 649658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Carvallo, M., & Mirenberg, M. C. (2004). How do I love thee? Let me count the Js: Implicit egotism and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 665683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawai, M. (1965). Newly-acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima islet. Primates, 6, 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunst, L., & Kratzer, J. (2007). Diffusion of innovations through social networks of children. Young Consumers, 8(1), 3651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutsukake, N., Suetsugu, N., & Hasegawa, T. (2006). Pattern, distribution, and function of greeting behavior among black-and-white colobus. International Journal of Primatology, 27, 12711291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioural mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, 334339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laland, K. (2004). Social learning strategies. Learning & Behavior, 32, 414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, H., & Laland, K. (2012). Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367, 21712180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madden, J. R., Drewe, J. A., Pearce, G. P., & Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2011). The social network structure of a wild meerkat population: 3. Position of individuals within networks. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65(10), 18571871.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mann, J., Stanton, M., Patterson, E., Bienenstock, E., & Singh, L. (2012). Social networks reveal cultural behaviour in tool-using dolphins. Nature Communications, 3, 980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McElreath, R., & Henrich, J. (2007). Dual inheritance theory: The evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution. In Dunbar, R. & Barrett, L. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 415444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2008). An experimental simulation of the ‘copy successful individuals’ cultural learning strategy: Adaptive landscapes, producer–scrounger dynamics, and informational access costs. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 350363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2008). The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363, 34893501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2006). A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 405423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, M. (2012). Imitation, pretend play and childhood: Essential elements in the evolution of human culture? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 170181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, M., & Blank, C. (2011). Imitation in young children: When who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Developmental Psychology, 47, 10501053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Putting the social into social learning: Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 182192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Over, H., Carpenter, M., Spears, R., & Gattis, M. (2013). Children selectively trust individuals who have imitated them. Social Development, 22, 215425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pike, T. W., & Laland, K. N. (2010) Conformist learning in nine-spined sticklebacks’ foraging decisions. Biological Letters, 6, 466468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324(5932), 1298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rendell, L., Fogarty, L., Hoppitt, W., Morgan, T., Webster, M., & Laland, K. (2011). Cognitive culture: Theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 6876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, A. (1988). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90, 819831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaik, C. P. van, & Burkart, J. M. (2011). Social learning and evolution: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 10081016. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherif, M. (1935). A study of some social factors in perception. Archives of Psychology, 27(187).Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a hostile world: The evolution of human cognition. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2011). From hominins to humans: How sapiens became behaviourally modern. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 809822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stubbersfield, J., Tehrani, J., & Flynn, E. (2014). Santa crucified, Walt Disney and death by lollypop: Do urban legends exhibit cognitive biases on cultural transmission? British Journal of Psychology, 106(2), 288–307.Google Scholar
Sundaresan, S. R., Fischhoff, I. R., Dushoff, J., & Rubenstein, D. I. (2007). Network metrics reveal differences in social organization between two fission-fusion species, Grevys zebra and onager. Oecologia, 151, 140149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Worchel, S. & Austin, W. G. (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations. Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall, 724.Google Scholar
Toelch, U., Bruce, M. J., Meeus, M. T. H., & Reader, S. M. (2010). Humans copy rapidly increasing choices in a multiarmed bandit problem. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 326333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A., & Ratner, H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 16, 495552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Užgiris, I. C. (1981). Two functions of imitation during infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Webster, M., & Laland, K. (2011). Reproductive state affects reliance on public information in sticklebacks. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 619627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A. (2011). The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and ancestral apes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 9971007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., & Flynn, E. (2010). The transmission and evolution of experimental microcultures in groups of young children. Developmental Psychology, 46, 16941709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., & Mesoudi, A. (2008). Establishing an experimental science of culture: Animal social diffusion experiments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363, 34773488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wisdom, T. N., Song, X., & Goldstone, R. L. (2013). Social learning strategies in a networked group. Cognitive Science, 37, 13831425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, L., Kendal, R., & Flynn, E. (2012). Context-dependent model-based biases in cultural transmission: Children’s imitation is affected by model age over model knowledgeable state. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(4), 387394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, L.A., Kendal, R., & Flynn, E. (2013). Whom do children copy? Model-based biases in learning. Developmental Review, 33, 341356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zentall, T. (2006). Imitation: Definitions, evidence, and mechanisms. Animal Cognition, 9, 335353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zentall, T. (2012). Perspectives on observational learning in animals. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 114128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×