Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T08:24:27.587Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 20 - The Role of Orienting Attention during Perceptual Training in Learning Nonnative Tones and Consonants

from Part V - Cognitive and Psychological Variables

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 January 2021

Ratree Wayland
Affiliation:
University of Florida
Get access

Summary

Previous studies have shown that directing learners’ attention during perceptual training facilitates detection and learning of unfamiliar consonant categories (Guion & Pederson, 2007; Pederson & Guion, 2010). The current study asks whether this attentional directing can also facilitate other types of phonetic learning. Monolingual Mandarin speakers were divided into two groups directed to learn either 1) the consonants or 2) the tones in an identification training task with the same set of Southern Min monosyllabic words containing the consonants /pʰ, p, b, kh, k, ɡ, tɕʰ, tɕ, ɕ/ and the tones (55, 33, 22, 24, 41). All subjects were also tested with an AXB discrimination task (with a distinct set of Southern Min words) before and after the training. Unsurprisingly, both groups improved accuracy in the sound type to which they attended. However, the consonant-attending group did not improve in discriminating tones after training and neither did the tone-attending group in discriminating consonants -- despite both groups having equal exposure to the same training stimuli. When combined with previous results for consonant and vowel training, these results suggest that explicitly directing learners’ attention has a broadly facilitative effect on phonetic learning including of tonal contrasts.

Type
Chapter
Information
Second Language Speech Learning
Theoretical and Empirical Progress
, pp. 485 - 502
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baese-Berk, M. M. (2019). Interactions between speech perception and production during learning of novel phonemic categories. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 81(4), 9811005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baese-Berk, M. M., & Samuel, A. G. (2016). Listeners beware: Speech production may be bad for learning speech sounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 2336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2009). The role of prominence information in the real-time comprehension of transitive constructions: A cross-linguistic approach. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 1958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, L. A., & Humes, L. E. (1997). Identification of multidimensional stimuli containing speech cues and the effects of training. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 22972310.Google Scholar
Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 73101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delancey, S. (1981). An interpretation of split ergativity and related patterns. Language, 57, 626657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrest, L. B. (1996). Discourse goals and attentional processes in sentence production: The dynamic construal of events. Conceptual structure, discourse and language. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Francis, A. L., Baldwin, K., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2000). Effects of training on attention to acoustic cues. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 62, 16681680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, A. L., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2002). Selective attention and the acquisition of new phonetic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 349366.Google ScholarPubMed
Gandour, J. (1983). Tone perception in Far Eastern languages. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 149175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gandour, J. (1984). Tone dissimilarity judgments by Chinese listeners. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 12, 235261.Google Scholar
Gildea, S. (2012). The referential hierarchy and attention. Faits de Langues, 39, 3347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guion, S. G., & Pederson, E. (2007). Investigating the role of attention in phonetic learning. In Bohn, O. S. & Munro, M (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning (pp. 5777). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Iverson, P., Kuhl, P. K., Akahane-Yamada, R., Diesch, E., Tohkura, Y., Kettermann, A., & Siebert, C. (2003). A perceptual interference account of acquisition difficulties for non-native phonemes. Cognition, 87, 4757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Nelson, T. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1493), 9791000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., & Iverson, P. (1995). Linguistic experience and the perceptual magnet effect. In Strange, W (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 121154). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Logan, J. S., Lively, S. E., & Pisoni, D. B. (1991). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: A first report. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89, 874886.Google Scholar
Myachykov, A., Thompson, D., Scheepers, C., & Garrod, S. (2011). Visual attention and structural choice in sentence production across languages. Language and Linguistics Compass, 5(2), 95107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myachykov, A., Tomlin, R. S., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Attention and empirical studies of grammar. The Linguistic Review, 22, 347364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pederson, E., & Guion-Anderson, S. (2010). Orienting attention during phonetic training facilitates learning. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127, 5459.Google Scholar
Pisoni, D. B. (1993). Long-term memory in speech perception: Some new findings on talker variability, speaking rate and perceptual learning. Speech Communication, 13, 109125.Google Scholar
Polka, L. (1992). Characterizing the influence of native language experience on adult speech perception. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 52, 3752.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 225.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Peterson, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 2542.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In Robinson, P (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 332). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strange, W. (2011). Automatic selective perception (ASP) of first and second language speech: A working model. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 456466.Google Scholar
Strange, W., & Shafer, V. L. (2008). Speech perception in second language learners: The re-education of selective perception. In Edwards, J. G. H. & Zampini, M. L. (Eds.), Phonology and second language acquisition (pp. 153191). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomlin, R. S. (1995). Focal attention, voice, and word order. In Downing, P & Noonan, M (Eds.), Word order in discourse (pp. 517552). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Tomlin, R. S. (1997). Mapping conceptual representations into linguistic representations: The role of attention in grammar. In Nuyts, J & Pederson, E (Eds.), Language and conceptualization (pp. 162189). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tomlin, R. S., & Villa, V. (1994). Attention in cognitive science and second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 183203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Y., Spence, M. M., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. A. (1999). Training American listeners to perceive Mandarin tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 36493658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Phonemic and phonetic factors in adult cross-language speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 75, 18661878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×