Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T13:16:20.313Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Intelligence as Adaptive Resource Development and Resource Allocation: A New Look Through the Lenses of SOC and Expertise

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Ralf T. Krampe
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Human Development and Education, Berlin, Germany
Paul B. Baltes
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Human Development and Education, Berlin, Germany
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Elena L. Grigorenko
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

The psychology of intelligence has undergone major changes in theoretical orientation and in its empirical approach since its early days close to a hundred years ago (Sternberg, 1990; Sternberg and Detterman, 1986). For the pioneers of intelligence testing, most notably Binet and Stern, the concept of intelligence captured relatively stable, interindividual differences in general abilities and capacities that were relevant to acquiring new skills and learning in novel situations. The idea of adaptation in the sense of mastering the challenges of a changing environment was constituent for the concept of intelligence in its earliest forms. In the minds of the general public until today, having a high IQ was synonymous with being smart and having a large potential for successfully coping with all kinds of professional and everyday challenges.

Due to these underlying theoretical ambitions the construction of intelligence tests in the decades following the pioneering stages faced no less than the triple challenge (1) to identify basic capacities that (2) reflected stable interindividual differences, and (3) that were general in terms of their relevance for all kinds of real-life competencies and skills. In response to these challenges extant psychometric tests have narrowed down the conceptual scope of the intelligence concept quite considerably. Implicit in the psychometric approach to intelligence is a focus on measurement (as opposed to understanding the causes, contexts, and functions of intelligence) and the view that intelligence reflects a collection of fairly static or dispositional abilities that characterizes a person (as opposed to a dynamic system of contextualized and adaptive cognitive functions that individuals continue to acquire throughout their life course).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1988). Determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 288–318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, N. D., Craik, F. I. M., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (1998). The attentional demands of encoding and retrieval in younger and older adults: 1. Evidence from divided attention costs. Psychology and Aging, 13, 405–423CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baltes, M. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1997). Normal versus pathological cognitive functioning in old age: plasticity and testing-the-limits of cognitive/brain reserve capacity. In F. Forette, Y. Christen, & F. Boller (Eds.), Démences et longévité (pp. 77–101). Paris: Fondation Nationale de Gérontologie
Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611–626CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1–34). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Baltes, P. B., Dittmann-Kohli, F., & Kliegl, R. (1996). Reserve capacity of the elderly in aging-sensitive tests of fluid intelligence: replication and extension. Psychology and Aging, 1, 172–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Kliegl, R. (1992). Further testing of limits of cognitive plasticity: Negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust. Developmental Psychology, 28, 121–125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Lindenberger, U. (1988). On the range of cognitive plasticity in old age as a function of experience: 15 years of intervention research. Behavior Therapy, 19(3), 283–300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Schaie, K. W. (1976). On the plasticity of intelligence in adulthood and old age: Where Horn and Donaldson fail. American Psychologist, 31, 720–725CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baltes, P. B., & Singer, T. (2001). Plasticity and the ageing mind: An exemplar of the biocultural orchestration of brain and behaviour. European Review, 9, 59–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Lifespan psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 471–507CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baltes, P. B., & Willis, S. L. (1982). Plasticity and enhancement of intellectual functioning in old age: Penn State's Adult Development and Enrichment Program (ADEPT). In F. I. M. Craik & S. E. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 353–389). New York: Plenum PressCrossRef
Book, W. F. (1924). Voluntary motor ability of the world's champion typists. Journal of Applied Psychology, 8, 283–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosman, E. A. (1993). Age-related differences in the motoric aspects of transcription typing skill. Psychology and Aging, 8, 87–102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, L. A., Shumway-Cook, A., & Woollacott, M. H. (1999). Attentional demands and postural recovery: The effects of aging. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 54A, M165–M171Google Scholar
Buonomano, D. V., & Merzenich, M. M. (1998). Cortical plasticity: From synapses to maps. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 21, 149–186CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camp, C. J. (1998). Memory intervention for normal and pathological older adults. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 18, 155–189Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin
Charness, N. (1981a). Aging and skilled problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 21–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, N. (1981b). Search in chess: Age and skill differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and Performance, 7, 467–476Google Scholar
Charness, N. (1989). Expertise in chess and bridge. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 183–208). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Charness, N., Krampe, R. T., & Mayr, U. (1996). The role of practice and coaching in entrepreneurial skill domains: An international comparison of life-span chess skill acquisition. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts, sciences, sports, and games (pp. 51–80). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16, pp. 1–58). San Diego, CA: Academic PressCrossRef
Cotman, C. W., & Nieto-Sampedro, M. (1984). Cell biology of synaptic plasticity. Science, 225(4668), 1287–1294CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1986). Toward life-span research on the functions and pragmatics of intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & R. K. Wagner (Eds.), Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of competence in the everyday world (pp. 203–235). New York: Cambridge University Press
Doll, J., & Mayr, U. (1987). Intelligenz und Schachleistung-eine Untersuchung an Schachexperten [Intelligence and achievement in chess – A study of chess masters]. Psychologische Beitraege, 29, 270–289Google Scholar
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., & Taub, E. (1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science, 270, 305–307CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elbert, T., Sterr, A., & Rockstroh, B. (1996). Untersuchungen zu corticaler Plastizität beim erwachsenen Menschen: Was lernt das Gehirn beim Geige spielen? [Studies of cortical plasticity in adults: What does the brain learn from playing the violin?]. Musikphysiologie und Musikmedizin, 3, 57–65Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (1990). Peak performance and age: An examination of peak performance in sports. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 164–196). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725–747CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence on maximal adaptations on task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (1991). Prospects and limits of the empirical study of expertise: An introduction. In K. A. Ericsson & J. Smith (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits. New York: Cambridge University Press
Ferguson, G. A. (1954). On learning and human ability. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 8, 95–112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, G. A. (1965). On transfer and the abilities of man. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 10, 121–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleishman, E. A. (1972). On the relation between abilities, learning, and human performance. American Psychologist, 27, 1017–1032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). The orchestration of selection, optimization, and compensation: An action-theoretical conceptualization of a theory of developmental regulation. In W. J. Perrig & A. Grob (Eds.), Control of human behavior, mental processes, and consciousness (pp. 35–58). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (in press). Life-management strategies of selection, optimization, and compensation: Measurement by self-report and construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
Gentner, D. R. (1988). Expertise in typewriting. In M. T. H. Chi, R. Glaser, & M. J. Farr (Eds.), The nature of expertise (pp. 1–21). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Gottlieb, G. L., Corcos, D. M., Jaric, S., & Agarwal, G. C. (1988). Practice improves even the simplest movements. Experimental Brain Research, 73, 436–440CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley
Horn, J. L. (1982). The theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence in relation to concepts of cognitive psychology and ageing in adulthood. In F. I. M. Craik & E. Trehub (Eds.), Ageing and cognitive processes: Advances in the study of communication and affect (pp. 237–278). New York: Plenum PressCrossRef
Horn, J. L., & Masunaga, H. (2000a). New directions for research into aging and intelligence: The development of expertise. In T. J. Perfect & E. A. Maylor (Eds.), Models of cognitive aging (pp. 125–159). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Horn, J. L., & Masunaga, H. (2000b). On the emergence of wisdom: Expertise development. In W. S. Brown (Ed.), Understanding wisdom: Sources, science, & society (pp. 245–276). Philadelphia, PA: Templeton Foundation Press
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1999). Innate talents: Reality or myth?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399–442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaas, J. H. (1991). Plasticity of sensory and motor maps in adult mammals. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 14, 137–167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaminski, G., Mayer, R., & Ruoff, B. A. (1984). Kinder und Jugendliche im Leistungssport [Children and adolescents in high-performance sports]. Schorndorf, Germany: Hofmann
Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature, 377, 155–158Google ScholarPubMed
Keele, S. W., & Hawkins, H. L. (1982). Explorations of individual differences relevant to high level skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14, 3–23CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keele, S. W., Pokorny, R. A., Corcos, D. M., & Ivry, R. (1985). Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: A correlational analysis. Acta Psychologica, 60, 173–191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kliegl, R., & Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theory-guided analysis of mechanisms of development and aging through testing-the-limits and research on expertise. In C. Schooler & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Cognitive functioning and social structure over the life course (pp. 95–119). Norwood: NJ: Ablex
Kliegl, R., Krampe, R. T., Mayr, U., & Liebscher, T. (2001a). Context memory deficit constrains asymptotic recall accuracy. Potsdam, Germany: University of Potsdam
Kliegl, R., Krampe, R. T., Philipp, D., & Luckner, M. (in prep.). Where have I seen that face before? Face memory skill acquisition. Potsdam, Germany: University of Potsdam
Kliegl, R., Phillipp, D., Luckner, M., & Krampe, R. T. (2001b). Face memory skill acquisition. In N. Charness, D. C. Park, & B. A. Sabel (Eds.), Communication, technology and aging: Opportunities and challenges for the future (pp. 169–186). New York: Springer
Kliegl, R., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Testing-the-limits and the study of adult age differences in cognitive plasticity of a mnemonic skill. Developmental Psychology, 25, 247–256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kliegl, R., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1990). On the locus and process of magnification of adult age differences during mnemonic training. Developmental Psychology, 26, 894–904CrossRef
Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Cohen, N. J., Banich, M. T., McAuley, E., Harrison, C. R., Chason, J., Vakil, E., Bardell, L., Boileau, R. A., & Colcombe, A. (1999). Aging, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature, 400, 418–419CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krampe, R. T. (1994). Maintaining excellence: Cognitive-motor performance in pianists differing in age and skill level. Berlin, Germany: Edition Sigma
Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1995). Deliberate practice and elite musical performance. In J. Rink (Ed.), The practice of performance: Studies in musical interpretation (pp. 84–104). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 331–359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labouvie, G. V., Frohring, W. R., & Baltes, P. B. (1973). Changing relationship between recall performance and abilities as a function of stage of learning and timing of recall. Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 191–198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, R. M. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. New York: Cambridge University Press
Lerner, R. M., Freund, A. M., Stefanis, I., & Habermas, T. (2001). Understanding developmental regulation in adolescence: The use of the selection, optimization, and compensation model. Human Development, 44, 29–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, K. Z. H., Lindenberger, U., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Walking while memorizing: A SOC study of age-related differences in compensatory behavior under dual-task conditions. Psychological Science, 12, 230–237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, S.-C. (in press). Aging mind: Facets and levels of analyses. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences. Oxford, UK: Elsevier Science
Li, S.-C., Lindenberger, U., Prinz, W., Baltes, P. B., & Team, C.-M. R. (2000). A lifetime from differentiation to dedifferentiation: The fall and rise of covariations between intelligence, information processing, and sensory functioning across the lifespan. Berlin: Max Planck Institute for Human Development and Education
Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1994). Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: A strong connection. Psychology and Aging, 9, 339–355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1997). Intellectual functioning in old and very old age: Cross-sectional results from the Berlin Aging Study. Psychology and Aging, 12, 410–432CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindenberger, U., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Memorizing while walking: Increase in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age. Psychology and Aging, 15, 417–436CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsiske, M., Lang, F. R., Baltes, M. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Selective optimization with compensation: Life-span perspectives on successful human development. In R. A. Dixon & L. Bäckman (Eds.), Compensation for psychological defects and declines: Managing losses and promoting gains (pp. 35–79). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Masunaga, H., & Horn, J. (2001). Expertise in relation to aging changes in components of intelligence. Psychology and Aging, 16, 293–311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maylor, E. A. (1994). Ageing and the retrieval of specialized and general knowledge: Performance of masterminds. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 105–114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maylor, E. A., Allison, S., & Wing, A. M. (in press). Effects of spatial and nonspatial cognitive activity on postural stability. British Journal of Psychology
Maylor, E. A., & Wing, A. M. (1996). Age differences in postural stability are increased by additional cognitive demands. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 51B, 143–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDowd, J. M., & Craik, F. I. M. (1987). Effects of aging and task difficulty on divided attention performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 267–280Google Scholar
Morrow, D., & Leirer, V. (1997). Aging, pilot performance and expertise. In A. D. Fisk & W. A. Rogers (Eds.), Handbook of human factors and the older adult (pp. 199–230). New York: Academic Press
Morrow, D., Leirer, V., Altieri, P., & Fitzsimmons, C. (1994). When expertise reduces age differences in performance. Psychology and Aging, 9, 134–148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1993). Crystal quest. A search for the basis of maintenance of practised skills into old age. In A. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness, and control. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press
Ruoff, B. A. (1981). Psychologische Analysen zum Alltag jugendlicher Leistungssportler: Eine empirische Untersuchung (kognitiver Repräsentationen) von Tagesabläufen [Psychological investigations of everyday lives in adolescent top-performance athletes: Empirical studies of cognitive representations of daily routines]. Munich, Germany: Minerva
Salthouse, T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 345–371CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Expertise as the circumvention of human processing limitations. In K. A. Ericsson & J. Smith (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 286–300). New York: Cambridge University Press
Schmidt, L. R. (1971). Testing the limits im Leistungsverhalten: Möglichkeiten und Grenzen. In E. Duhm (Ed.), Praxis der klinischen Psychologie (Vol. 2, pp. 9–29). Göttingen: Hogrefe
Schulz, R., & Curnow, C. (1988). Peak performance and age among superathletes: Track and field, swimming, baseball, tennis and golf. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 43, 113–120CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, H. A., & Chase, W. G. (1973). Skill in chess. American Scientist, 61, 394–403Google Scholar
Singer, T., & Lindenberger, U. (2000). Plastizität. In H.-W. Wahl & C. Tesch-Römer (Eds.), Angewandte Gerontologie in Schlüsselbegriffen [Applied gerontology in keywords] (pp. 39–48). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer
Singer, T., Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Plasticity of memory for new learning in very old age: A story of major loss? Berlin: Max Planck Institute for Human Development
Singer, W. (1995). Development and plasticity of cortical processing architectures. Science, 270, 758–764CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Metaphors of mind: Conceptions of the nature of intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press
Sternberg, R. J. (2001). How much money should one put into the cognitive parking meter?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Detterman, D. K. (1986). What is intelligence? Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Teasdale, N., Bard, C., LaRue, J., & Fleury, M. (1993). On the cognitive penetrability of posture control. Experimental Aging Research, 19, 1–13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Telford, C. W., & Spangler, H. (1935). Training effects in motor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 141–147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhaeghen, P., Marcoen, A., & Goosens, L. (1992). Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: A meta-analytic study. Psychology and Aging, 7, 242–251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walsh, D. A., & Hershey, D. A. (1993). Mental models and the maintenance of complex problem solving skills in old age. In J. Cerella, J. Rybash, W. Hoyer, & M. Commons (Eds.), Adult information processing: Limits on loss (pp. 553–584). San Diego: Academic Press
Wang, X., Merzenich, M. M., Sameshima, K., & Jenkins, W. (1995). Remodelling of hand representation in adult cortex determined by timing of tactile stimulation. Nature, 378, 71–75CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×