Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T13:26:49.199Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Expertise, Competence, and Creative Ability: The Perplexing Complexities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Dean Keith Simonton
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Elena L. Grigorenko
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Albert Einstein is often considered one of the greatest creators of the 20th century. Indeed, he is frequently viewed as a prototypical example of creative genius. Yet what was the psychological basis of his creativity? In particular, consider the following three issues:

  1. Samuel Johnson (1781, p. 5), the author of the first English dictionary, claimed that “the true Genius is a mind of large general powers, accidentally determined to some particular direction.” In other words, creativity may consist of a generalized information processing capacity that may be channeled to almost any endeavor. Does this statement hold for Einstein? Could he have become a Picasso or a Stravinsky had his childhood experiences only directed him toward art or music rather than toward science? Or did Einstein possess a more specialized ability that would not have served him well had he ventured outside of theoretical physics?

  2. Whether Einstein's creative powers were general or specific, where did his capacity originate? Was Einstein's creativity an innate ability, as expressed by John Dryden's (1693/1885, p. 60) famous remark that “genius must be born, and never can be taught”? Or was the capacity slowly and arduously acquired through education, practice, and training? Was Einstein merely the most expert and competent theoretical physicist of his day?

  3. […]

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amabile, T. M. (1982). Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 997–1013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M., (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview
Amelang, M., Herboth, G., & Oefner, I. (1991). A prototype strategy for the construction of a creativity scale. European Journal of Personality, 5, 261–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baer, J. (1993). Creativity and divergent thinking: A task-specific approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Baer, J. (1994). Divergent thinking is not a general trait: A multidomain training experiment. Creativity Research Journal, 7, 35–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barron, F. X. (1969). Creative person and creative process. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston
Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass & Stogdill's handbook of leadership: Theory, research, and managerial applications (3rd Ed.). New York: Free Press
Bennet, W. (1980, January–February). Providing for posterity. Harvard Magazine, pp. 13–16
Bouchard, T. J. Jr., (1994). Genes, environment, and personality. Science, 264, 1700–1701CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bramwell, B. S. (1948). Galton's “Hereditary” and the three following generations since 1869. Eugenics Review, 39, 146–153Google ScholarPubMed
Burt, C. (1943). Ability and income. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 12, 83–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burt, C. (1963). Is intelligence distributed normally?British Journal of Statistical Psychology, 16, 175–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Candolle, A. de (1873). Histoire des sciences et des savants depuis deux siècles. [History of sciences and scientists in the last two centuries.] Geneva, Switzerland: Georg
Cattell, R. B., & Butcher, H. J. (1968). The prediction of achievement and creativity. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill
Cox, C. (1926). The early mental traits of three hundred geniuses. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press
Davis, R. A. (1987). Creativity in neurological publications. Neurosurgery, 20, 652–663CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dellas, M., & Gaier, E. L. (1970). Identification of creativity: The individual. Psychological Bulletin, 73, 55–73CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, W. (1954a, September). Bibliographies of eminent scientists. Scientific Monthly, 79, 180–183Google Scholar
Dennis, W. (1954b). Productivity among American psychologists. American Psychologist, 9, 191–194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, W. (1955, April). Variations in productivity among creative workers. Scientific Monthly, 80, 277–278Google Scholar
Dennis, W. (1966). Creative productivity between the ages of 20 and 80 years. Journal of Gerontology, 21, 1–8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. M. Jr., (1986). The life-cycle research productivity of mathematicians and scientists. Journal of Gerontology, 41, 520–525CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dryden, J. (1885). Epistle to Congreve. In W. Scott & G. Saintsbury (Eds.), The works of John Dryden, Vol. 11 (pp. 57–60). Edinburgh, Scotland: Paterson (original work published 1693)
Elo, A. E. (1965). Age changes in master chess performance. Journal of Gerontology, 20, 289–299Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (1996a). The acquisition of expert performance: An introduction to some of the issues. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to expert performance: Empirical evidence from the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 1–50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.). (1996b). The road to expert performance: Empirical evidence from the arts and sciences, sports, and games. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725–747CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290–309CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frensch, P. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Expertise and intelligent thinking: When is it worse to know better? In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, Vol. 5 (pp. 157–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Macmillan
Galton, F. (1874). English men of science: Their nature and nurture. London: Macmillan
Gardner, H. (1993). Creating minds: An anatomy of creativity seen through the lives of Freud, Einstein, Picasso, Stravinsky, Eliot, Graham, and Gandhi. New York: Basic Books
Getzels, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision: A longitudinal study of problem finding in art. New York: Wiley
Getzels, J., & Jackson, P. W. (1962). Creativity and intelligence: Explorations with gifted students. New York: Wiley
Goertzel, M. G., Goertzel, V., & Goertzel, T. G. (1978). 300 eminent personalities: A psychosocial analysis of the famous. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass
Gough, H. G. (1979). A creative personality scale for the adjective check list. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1398–1405CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruber, H. E. (1989). The evolving systems approach to creative work. In D. B. Wallace & H. E. Gruber (Eds.), Creative people at work: Twelve cognitive case studies (pp. 3–24). New York: Oxford University Press
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill
Hargens, L. L. (1978). Relations between work habits, research technologies, and eminence in science. Sociology of Work and Occupations, 5, 97–112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (1989). The complete problem solver (2nd Ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Helson, R., & Crutchfield, R. S. (1970). Mathematicians: The creative researcher and the average Ph.D. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 250–257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hocevar, D., & Bachelor, P. (1989). A taxonomy and critique of measurements used in the study of creativity. In J. A. Glover, R. R. Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 53–75). New York: Plenum PressCrossRef
Howe, M. J. A. (1999). The psychology of high abilities. New York: New York University Press
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1998). Innate talents: Reality or myth?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399–442CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huber, J. C., (1998a). Invention and inventivity as a special kind of creativity, with implications for general creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 32, 58–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, J. C. (1998b). Invention and inventivity is a random, Poisson process: A potential guide to analysis of general creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 231–241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, J. C., (2000). A statistical analysis of special cases of creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 34, 203–225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, L., (1958). Undergraduate academic record of Fellows of the Royal Society. Nature, 182, 1326CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudson, L., & Jacot, B. (1986). The outsider in science. In C. Bagley & G. K. Verma (Eds.), Personality, cognition and values (pp. 3–23). London: MacmillanCrossRef
Johnson, S. (1781). The lives of the most eminent English poets, Vol. 1. London: Bathurst et al
Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 331–359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroeber, A. L. (1944). Configurations of culture growth. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press
Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd Ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Lehman, H. C. (1953). Age and achievement. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press
Lehman, H. C. (1962). More about age and achievement. Gerontologist, 2, 141–148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lotka, A. J. (1926). The frequency distribution of scientific productivity. Journal of the Washington Academy of Sciences, 16, 317–323Google Scholar
Ludwig, A. M. (1995). The price of greatness: Resolving the creativity and madness controversy. New York: Guilford Press
Lykken, D. T. (1982). Research with twins: The concept of emergenesis. Psychophysiology, 19, 361–373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., Tellegen, A., & Bouchard, T. J. Jr., (1992). Emergenesis: Genetic traits that may not run in families. American Psychologist, 47, 1565–1577CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKinnon, D. W. (1978). In search of human effectiveness. Buffalo, NY: Creative Education Foundation
Manis, J. G. (1951). Some academic influences upon publication productivity. Social Forces, 29, 267–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martindale, C. (1989). Personality, situation, and creativity. In J. A. Glover, R. R. Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 211–232). New York: Plenum PressCrossRef
McClelland, D. C. (1963). The calculated risk: An aspect of scientific performance. In C. W. Taylor & F. X. Barron (Eds.), Scientific creativity: Its recognition and development (pp. 184–192). New York: Wiley
McClelland, D. C. (1973). Testing for competence rather than for “intelligence”. American Psychologist, 28, 1–14CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDowell, J. M. (1982). Obsolescence of knowledge and career publication profiles: Some evidence of differences among fields in costs of interrupted careers. American Economic Review, 72, 752–768Google Scholar
McNemar, Q. (1964). Lost: Our intelligence? Why?American Psychologist, 19, 871–882CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220–232CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moles, A. (1968). Information theory and esthetic perception (J. E. Cohen, Trans.). Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press (original work published 1958)
Nicholls, J. G. (1972). Creativity in the person who will never produce anything original and useful: The concept of creativity as a normally distributed trait. American Psychologist, 27, 717–727CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohlsson, S. (1992). The learning curve for writing books: Evidence from Professor Asimov. Psychological Science, 3, 380–382CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Platz, A., & Blakelock, E. (1960). Productivity of American psychologists: Quantity versus quality. American Psychologist, 15, 310–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., & Petrill, S. A. (1997). Genetics and intelligence: What's new?Intelligence, 24, 53–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poincaré, H. (1921). The foundations of science: Science and hypothesis, the value of science, science and method (G. B. Halstead, Trans.). New York: Science Press
Quételet, A. (1968). A treatise on man and the development of his faculties. New York: Franklin. (Reprint of 1842 Edinburgh translation of 1835 French original.)
Raskin, E. A. (1936). Comparison of scientific and literary ability: A biographical study of eminent scientists and men of letters of the nineteenth century. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 31, 20–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, R., Kinney, D. K., Lunde, I., Benet, M., & Merzel, A. P. C. (1988). Assessing everyday creativity: Characteristics of the Lifetime Creativity Scales and validation with three large samples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 476–485CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roe, A. (1953). The making of a scientist. New York: Dodd, Mead
Root-Bernstein, R. S., Bernstein, M., & Garnier, H. (1993). Identification of scientists making long-term, high-impact contributions, with notes on their methods of working. Creativity Research Journal, 6, 329–343CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rostan, S. M. (1994). Problem finding, problem solving, and cognitive controls: An empirical investigation of critically acclaimed productivity. Creativity Research Journal, 7, 97–110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulz, R., & Curnow, C. (1988). Peak performance and age among super athletes: Track and field, swimming, baseball, tennis, and golf. Journal of Gerontology, 43, 113–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seelig, C. (1958). Albert Einstein: A documentary biography (M. Savill, Trans.). London: Staples Press
Shockley, W. (1957). On the statistics of individual variations of productivity in research laboratories. Proceedings of the Institute of Radio Engineers, 45, 279–290Google Scholar
Simon, R. J. (1974). The work habits of eminent scientists. Sociology of Work and Occupations, 1, 327–335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1976). Biographical determinants of achieved eminence: A multivariate approach to the Cox data. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 218–226CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonton, D. K. (1977). Creative productivity, age, and stress: A biographical time-series analysis of 10 classical composers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 791–804CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonton, D. K. (1980). Land battles, generals, and armies: Individual and situational determinants of victory and casualties. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 110–119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1983). Formal education, eminence, and dogmatism: The curvilinear relationship. Journal of Creative Behavior, 17, 149–162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1984a). Creative productivity and age: A mathematical model based on a two-step cognitive process. Developmental Review, 4, 77–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1984b). Is the marginality effect all that marginal?Social Studies of Science, 14, 621–622CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1984c). Leader age and national condition: A longitudinal analysis of 25 European monarchs. Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 111–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1985). Quality, quantity, and age: The careers of 10 distinguished psychologists. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 21, 241–254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1986). Popularity, content, and context in 37 Shakespeare plays. Poetics, 15, 493–510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1987a). Developmental antecedents of achieved eminence. Annals of Child Development, 5, 131–169Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1987b). Why presidents succeed: A political psychology of leadership. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press
Simonton, D. K. (1988). Age and outstanding achievement: What do we know after a century of research?. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 251–267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1989). Age and creative productivity: Nonlinear estimation of an information-processing model. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 29, 23–37CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonton, D. K. (1991a). Career landmarks in science: Individual differences and interdisciplinary contrasts. Developmental Psychology, 27, 119–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1991b). Emergence and realization of genius: The lives and works of 120 classical composers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 829–840CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1992). Leaders of American psychology, 1879–1967: Career development, creative output, and professional achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 5–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1994). Greatness: Who makes history and why. New York: Guilford Press
Simonton, D. K. (1995a). Drawing inferences from symphonic programs: Musical attributes versus listener attributions. Music Perception, 12, 307–322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1995b). Personality and intellectual predictors of leadership. In D. H. Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.), International handbook of personality and intelligence (pp. 739–757). New York: Plenum
Simonton, D. K. (1997a). Creative productivity: A predictive and explanatory model of career trajectories and landmarks. Psychological Review, 104, 66–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1997b). When giftedness becomes genius: How does talent achieve eminence? In N. Colangelo & G. A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of gifted education (2nd Ed., pp. 335–349). Boston: Allyn & Bacon
Simonton, D. K. (1998a). Fickle fashion versus immortal fame: Transhistorical assessments of creative products in the opera house. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 198–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1998b). Political leadership across the life span: Chronological versus career age in the British monarchy. Leadership Quarterly, 9, 195–206Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1999a). Origins of genius: Darwinian perspectives on creativity. New York: Oxford University Press
Simonton, D. K. (1999b). Talent and its development: An emergenic and epigenetic model. Psychological Review, 106, 435–457CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2000). Creative development as acquired expertise: Theoretical issues and an empirical test. Developmental Review, 20, 283–318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (in press). Creativity assessment. In R. Fernández-Ballesteros (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychological assessment. London: Sage Publications
Smith, H. (1974). Sonnets. In G. B. Evans (Ed.), The Riverside Shakespeare (pp. 1745–1748). Boston: Houghton Mifflin
Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man: Their nature and measurement. New York: Macmillan
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the crowd: Cultivating creativity in a culture of conformity. New York: Free Press
Sulloway, F. J. (1996). Born to rebel: Birth order, family dynamics, and creative lives. New York: Pantheon
Tweney, R. D. (1990). Five questions for computationalists. In J. Shrager & P. Langley (Eds.), Computational models of scientific discovery and theory information (pp. 471–484). San Mateo, CA: Kaufmann
Walberg, H. J., Strykowski, B. F., Rovai, E., & Hung, S. S. (1984). Exceptional performance. Review of Educational Research, 54, 87–112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waller, N. G., Bouchard, T. J. Jr., Lykken, D. T., Tellegen, A., & Blacker, D. M., (1993). Creativity, heritability, familiality: Which word does not belong?. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 235–237Google Scholar
White, K. G., & White, M. J. (1978). On the relation between productivity and impact. Australian Psychologist, 13, 369–374CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, R. K. (1931). The versatility of genius. Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 460–489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, H. (1977). Scientific elite. New York: Free Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×