Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T13:20:33.841Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Expertise and Mental Disabilities: Bridging the Unbridgeable?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Elena L. Grigorenko
Affiliation:
Yale University and Moscow State University
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Elena L. Grigorenko
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Jean-Baptiste Grenouille, the main character of Patrick Süskind's Perfume, was an expert perfumer. His knowledge of aromas, his ability to differentiate, dissect, mix, and create various smells gave him a craft and made other people adore and hate him. And yet, from the very beginning of the book, it is clear that Grenouille was not a “typical” individual: His language development was delayed, his cognitive abilities were challenged, and his motor functioning appeared to be impaired. Yet he was a true expert of smell – he knew how to imitate the perfume of a blossom, the smell of a dog, various human odors, and even the aromas of those rare humans who inspire love. His skill was recognized and admired, and Grenouille kept developing and strengthening it.

The example of Grenouille poses an interesting question – can it really be that a person is simultaneously an expert in one domain and mentally handicapped in other domains? In broader terms, is it possible for mental disability and expert knowledge to coincide? To answer this question, I first cite and discuss various definitions of the word expertise. Second, I briefly review the literature on expertise. Third, I present a hypothesis suggesting the coexistence of expertise and disability, and support this model with a number of cases from the literature. Finally, I explore the application of this hypothesis to studies of the etiology of expertise.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adelson, B. (1984). When novices surpass experts: The difficulty of a task may increase with expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 483–495Google Scholar
Aram, D. M., Ekelman, B. L., & Healy, J. M. (1984, June). Reading profiles of hyperlexic children. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Neuropsychology Society, Aachen, West Germany
Aram, D. M., & Healy, J. M. (1988). Hyperlexia: A review of extraordinary word recognition. In L. Obler & D. Fein (Eds.), Neuropsychology of talent (pp. 70–102). New York: Guilford
Aram, D. M., Rose, D. F., & Horwitz, S. J. (1984). Hyperlexia: Developmental reading without meaning. In R. M. Joshi & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Dyslexia: A global issue (pp. 518–533). The Hague, Netherlands: Martinus NijhoffCrossRef
Backman, J., (1983). Psycholinguistic skills and reading acquisition: A look at early readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 18, 466–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blachman, B. A. (1997). Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Bowers, P. G., Sunseth, K., & Golden, J., (1999). The route between rapid naming and reading progress. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 31–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burd, L., & Kerbeshian, J., (1985). Hyperlexia and a variant of hypergraphia. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 60, 940–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burd, L., Kerbeshian, J., & Fisher, W. (1985). Inquiry into the incidence of hyperlexia in a statewide population of children with pervasive developmental disorder. Psychological Reports, 57, 236–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bus, A. G., & IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 403–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bus, A. G., IJzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D., (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read. A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65, 1–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, A. C. (1969). Special “isolated” abilities in severely psychotic young children. Psychiatry, 32, 137–149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carston, R. (1996). The architecture of mind: Modularity and modularization. In D. Green et al. (Eds.), Cognitive science: An introduction, (pp. 53–83). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell
Cassidy, J., & Vukelich, C. (1980). Do the gifted read early?The Reading Teacher, 33, 578–82Google Scholar
Charness, N. (1979). Components of skill in bridge. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33, 1–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). The mind's eye in chess. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 215–281). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16, pp. 1–58). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. (Eds.). (1989). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Cobrinik, L., (1974). Unusual reading ability in severely disturbed children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4, 163–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cobrinik, L. (1982). The performance of hyperlexic children on an “incomplete words” task. Neuropsychologia, 20, 569–78CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. J., Campbell, R. C., & Gelado, M., (1987). Hyperlexia: A variant of aphasia or dyslexia. Pediatric Neurology, 3, 22–28CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. J., Hall, J., & Riccio, C. A. (1997). Neuropsychological profiles of children diagnosed as specific language impaired with and without hyperlexia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 12, 223–229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Comuzzie, A. G., Hixson, J. E., Almasy, L., Mitchell, B. D., Mahaney, M. C., Dyer, T. D., Stern, M. P., MacCluer, J. W., & Blangero, J. (1997). A major quantitative trait locus determining serum leptin levels and fat mass is located on human chromosome 2. Nature Genetics, 15, 273–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cossu, G., & Marshall, J. C. (1986). Theoretical implications of the hyperlexic syndrome: Two new Italian cases. Cortex, 22, 579–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cossu, G., & Marshall, J. C., (1990). Are cognitive skills a prerequisite for learning to read and write?Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 21–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain-Thoreson, C., & Dale, P. S. (1992). Do early talkers become early readers? Lingustic precocity, preschool language, and emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology, 28, 421–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowe, R. R., Noyes, R., Pauls, D. L., & Slymen, D., (1983). A family study of panic disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40, 1065–69CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1988). Multiple regression analysis of twin data: Etiology of deviant scores versus individual differences. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae: Twin Research, 37, 205–216CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Groot, A. (1978). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton. (Original work published 1946.)
Hirsch, K., (1971). Are hyperlexics dyslexics?The Journal of Special Education, 5, 243–246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durkin, D. (1966). Children who read early. New York: Teachers College Press
Durkin, D. (1970). A language-arts program for pre-first grade children: Two-year achievement report. Reading Research Quarterly, 5, 9–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egan, D. E., & Schwartz, B. J. (1979). Chunking in recall of symbolic drawings. Memory and Cognition, 7, 149–158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, D. E., & Needleman, R. M., (1976). The syndrome of hyperlexia. Brain and Language, 3, 339–349CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engle, R. W., & Bukstel, L., (1978). Memory processes among bridge players of differing expertise. American Journal of Psychology, 91, 673–679CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (1988). Analysis of memory performance in terms of memory skill. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, Vol. 4 (pp. 137–179). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Ericsson, K. A. (1996). The acquisition of expert performance: An introduction to some of the issues. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 1–50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Ericsson, K. A. (1998). The scientific study of expert levels of performance: General implications for optimal learning and creativity. High Ability Studies, 9, 75–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2001a). Attaining excellence through deliberate practice: Insights from the study of expert performance. In M. Ferrari (Ed.), The pursuit of excellence in education (pp. 21–55). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Ericsson, K. A. (2001b). The path to expert performance: Insights from the masters on how to improve performance by deliberate practice. In P. Thomas (Ed.), Optimizing performance in golf (pp. 1–57). Brisbane, Australia: Australian Academic Press
Ericsson, K. A., Chase, W., & Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of a memory skill. Science, 208, 1181–1182CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C., (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence on maximal adaptations on task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., Patel, V. L., & Kintsch, W., (2000). How experts' adaptations to representative task demands account for the expertise effect in memory recall: Comment on Vicente and Wang (1998). Psychological Review, 107, 578–592CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (1991). Prospects and limits in the empirical study of expertise: An introduction. In K. A. Ericsson and J. Smith (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 1–38). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Ericsson, K. A., & Staszewski, J. (1989). Skilled memory and expertise: Mechanisms of exceptional performance. In D. Klahr and K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 235–267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Felton, R. H. (1998). The development of reading skills in poor readers: Educational implications. In C. Hulme & R. M. Joshi (Eds.). Reading and spelling: Development and disorders, (pp. 219–233). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Felton, R. H., & Brown, I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing, 2, 39–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felton, R. H., Naylor, C. E., & Wood, F. B. (1990). Neuropsychological profile of adult dyslexics. Brain and Language, 39, 485–497CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felton, R. H., & Wood, F. B. (1992). A reading level match study of non-word reading skills in poor readers with varying IQ. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 318–326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A., (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Fontenelle, S., & Alarcon, M., (1982). Hyperlexia: Precocious word recognition in developmentally delayed children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55, 247–252CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frensch, P. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Expertise and intelligent thinking: When is it worse to know better? In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, Vol. 5 (pp. 157–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Garfield, J. (1994). Modularity. In S. Guttenplan (Ed.), A companion to the philosophy of mind, (pp. 441–448). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell
Glaser, R. (1996). Changing the agency for learning: Acquiring expert performance. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games, (pp. 303–311). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Glosser, G., Friedman, R. B., & Roeltgen, D. P. (1996). Clues to cognitive organization of reading and writing from developmental hyperlexia. Neuropsychology, 10, 168–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glosser, G., Grugan, P., & Friedman, R. B. (1997). Semantic memory impairment does not impact on phonological and orthographic processing in a case of developmental hyperlexia. Brain and Language, 56, 234–47CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, T. E., & Rothermel, R. D. (1984). Hyperlexic children reading. Brain, 107, 759–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, J. (1972). A case study of an “autistic savant”: Mental function in the psychotic child with markedly discrepant abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 13, 267–278CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gott, S. P., Hall, E. P., Pokorny, R. A., Dibble, E., & Glaser, R. (1993). A naturalistic study of transfer: Adaptive expertise in technical domains. In D. K. Detterman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Transfer on trial: Intelligence, cognition, and instruction (pp. 258–288). Stamford, CT: Ablex Publishing Corp
Graziani, L. J., Brodsky, K., Mason, J. C., & Zager, R. P. (1983). Variability in IQ scores and prognoses in children with hyperlexia. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 22, 441–43CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigorenko, E. L., Wood, F. B., Meyer, M. S., Hart, L. A., Speed, W. C., Shuster, A., & Pauls, D. L. (1997). Susceptibility loci for distinct components of developmental dyslexia on chromosomes 6 and 15. American Journal of Human Genetics, 60, 27–39Google ScholarPubMed
Haseman, J. K., & Elston, R. C. (1972). The investigation of linkage between a quantitative trait and a marker locus. Behavior Genetics, 2, 3–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, J. M. (1982). The enigma of hyperlexia. Reading Research Quarterly, 17, 319–338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, J. M., & Aram, D. M. (1986). Hyperlexia and dyslexia: A family study. Annals of Dyslexia, 36, 226–253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Healy, J. M., Aram, D. M., Horwitz, S. J., & Kessler, J. W. (1982). A study of hyperlexia. Brain and Language, 9, 1–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hecht, H., & Proffitt, D. R. (1995). The price of expertise: Effects of experience on the water-level task. Psychological Science, 6(2), 90–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermelin, B., (2001). Bright splinters of the mind: A personal story of research with autistic savants. London, England: Jessica Kingsley Publishers
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1998). Innate talents: Reality or myth?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399–442CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, P. R., & Huttenlocher, J. A. (1973). A study of children with hyperlexia. Neurology, 23, 1107–1116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, N. E. (1988). Precocious reading ability: What does it mean?Gifted Child Quarterly, 32, 200–204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, N. E., (1992). Precocious reading of English: Origins, structure, and predictive significance. In P. S. Klein, & A. J. Tannenbaum (Eds.), To be young and gifted (pp. 171–203). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing
Jackson, N. E., & Biemiller, A. J. (1985). Letter, word, and text reading times of precocious and average readers. Child Developoment, 56, 196–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, N. E., & Donaldson, G. W. (1989). Precocious and second-grade readers' use of context in word identification. Learning and Individual Differences, 1, 255–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, N. E., Donaldson, G. W., & Cleland, L. N., (1988). The structure of precocious reading ability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 234–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, N. E., & Myers, M. G. (1982). Letter naming time, digit span, and precocious reading achievement. Intelligence, 6, 311–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jong, P. F., & Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanner, I. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–50Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J., (1993). Panic disorder in women: A population based twin study. Psychological Medicine, 23, 397–406CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Learning Disabilities Statistics Sheet. (1997). The Orton Dyslexia Society
Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 54 (6, # 248)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyon, R. G. (1995). Toward a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 35, 3–27Google Scholar
Mehegan, C. C., & Dreifuss, F. E. (1972). Hyperlexia: Exceptional reading ability in brain-damaged children. Neurology, 22, 1105–11CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. A., (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mills, J. R., & Jackson, N. E. (1990). Predictive significance of early giftedness: The case of precocious reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 410–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nation, K. (1999). Reading skills in hyperlexia: A developmental perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 338–355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noyes, R. Jr., Crowe, R. R., Harris, E. L., Hampa, B. J., McChesney, C. M., & Chaudry, D. R., (1986). Relationships between panic disorder and agoraphobia: A family study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 227–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, R., Wise, B., Conners, F., Rack, J., & Fulker, D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 339–48CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, S. W. (1919). Pseudo-latent for words. Psychological Clinics, 11, 1–7Google Scholar
Patel, P. G., & Patterson, P. (1982). Precocious reading acquisition: Psycholinguistic development, IQ, and home background. First Language, 13, 139–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pauls, D. (1993). Behavioural disorders: Lessons in linkage. Nature Genetics, 3, 4–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennington, B. F., Johnson, C., & Welsh, M. C. (1987). Unexpected reading precocity in a normal preschooler: Implications for hyperlexia. Brain and Language, 30, 165–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press
Perfetti, C. A., & McCutchen, D. (1987). Schooled language competence: Linguistic abilities in reading and writing. In S. Rosenberg (Ed.), Advances in applied psycholinguistics (pp. 105–41). New York: Cambridge University Press
Phillips, A. (1930). Talented imbeciles. Psychological Clinics, 18, 246–265Google ScholarPubMed
Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1996). Reading exception words and pseudowords: Are the two routes really necessary? In J. P. Levy, B. Bairaktaris, J. Bullinaria, & P. Cairns (Eds.), Proceedings of the Second Neural Computation and Psychology Workshop (pp. 145–59). London: University College London Press
Reitman, J., (1976). Skilled perception in GO: Deducing memory structures from interresponse times. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 336–356CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richman, L. C., & Kitchell, M. M. (1981). Hyperlexia as a variant of developmental language disorder. Brain and Language, 12, 203–212CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Risch, N. J., & Zhang, H. (1996). Mapping quantitative trait loci with extreme discordant sib pairs: Sampling considerations. American Journal of Human Genetics, 58, 836–43Google ScholarPubMed
Rispens, J., & Van Berckelaer, I. A. (1991). Hyperlexia: Definition and criterion. In R. M. Joshi (Ed.), Written language disorders (pp. 143–163). The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic PublishersCrossRef
Roedell, W. C., Jackson, N. E., & Robinson, H. B. (1980). Gifted young children. New York: Teachers College Press
Scarborough, H. S. (1984). Continuity between childhood dyslexia and adult reading. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 329–48CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seymour, P. H. K., & Evans, H. M. (1992). Beginning reading without semantics: A cognitive study of hyperlexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 89–122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, L. S. (1984). A longitudinal study of a hyperlexic child: Hyperlexia as a language disorder. Neuropsychologia, 22, 577–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silberberg, N. E., & Silberberg, M. C. (1967). Hyperlexia: Specific word recognition skills in young children. Exceptional Children, 34, 41–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silberberg, N. E., & Silberberg, M. C., (1968). Case histories in hyperlexia. Journal of School Psychology, 7, 3–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silberberg, N. E., & Silberberg, M. C., (1971). Hyperlexia: The other end of the continuum. Journal of Special Education, 3, 233–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, H. A., & Gilmartin, K. (1973). A simulation of memory for chess positions. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 165–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smoller, J. W., & Tsuang, M. T. (1998). Panic and phobic anxiety: Defining phenotypes for genetic studies. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 1152–62CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia (2d ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell
Snowling, M. J., & Frith, U., (1986). Comprehension in hyperlexic readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 392–415CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E., Nathan, R. G., & Zolman, J. E., (1988). The developmental lag hypothesis in reading: Longitudinal and matched reading-level comparisons. Child Development, 59, 71–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S., (1994). Phenotype performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., (1998). Abilities are forms of developing expertise. Educational Researcher, 27, 11–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the crowd: Cultivating creativity in a culture of conformity. New York: Free Press
Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., & Ferrari, M. (2002). Fostering intellectual excellence through developing expertise. In M. Ferrari (Ed.), The pursuit of excellence through education (pp. 57–84). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Subotnik, R. F., & Arnold, K. D. (1993). Longitudinal studies of giftedness: Investigating the fulfillment of promise. In K. A. Heller & F. J. Moenks (Eds.), International handbook of research and development of giftedness and talent (pp. 149–160). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press, Inc
Tobin, A. W., & Pikulski, J. J. (1988). A longitudinal study of the reading achievement of early and non-early readers through sixth grade. In J. Readance & R. S. Baldwin (Eds.), Dialogues in literacy research. Thirty-seventh yearbook of the National Reading Conference (pp. 49–58). Chicago, IL: National Reading Conference
T⊘nnessen, F. E., (1999). Options and limitations of the cognitive psychological approach to the treatment of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 386–393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second- to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treffert, D. A., (1988). The idiot savant: A review of the syndrome. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 563–572Google ScholarPubMed
Tsuang, M. T., Faraone, S. V., & Lyons, M. J., (1993). Identification of the phenotype in psychiatric genetics. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 243, 131–42CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daal, V., & Leij, A. (1999). Developmental dyslexia: Related to specific or general deficits?Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 71–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vogel, S. A., & Adelman, P. B., (1992). The success of college students with learning disabilities: Factors related to educational attainment. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 430–441CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmons, K., & Rashotte, C. A., (1993). Development of young readers' phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 83–103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A., (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weissman, M. M., Wickramaratne, P., Adams, P. B., Lish, J. D., Horwath, E., Charney, D., Woods, S. W., Leeman, F., & Frosch, E., (1993). The relationship between panic disorder and major depression: A new family study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 767–80CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitehouse, D., & Harris, J. (1984). Hyperlexia in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 281–89CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, M. (1997). A provisional, integrative account of phonological and naming speed deficits in dyslexia: Implications for diagnosis and intervention. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Cognitive and linguistic foundations of reading acquisition: Implications for intervention research (pp. 177–210). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Wood, F. B., & Felton, R. H., (1994). Separate linguistic and attentional factors in the development of reading. Topics in Language Disorders, 14, 42–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yopp, H. K., (1988). The validity and reliability of phonemic awareness tests. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 159–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×