Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T17:36:43.144Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Mother and Child: Preparing for a Life

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 May 2011

Ronald G. Barr
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia
Daniel P. Keating
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

In trying to understand what helps the lives of human infants get off to a good start, we have concentrated on the quite remarkable and particular relationship that occurs between mothers and their infants in the first few days and weeks of life. Of course, everyone is interested in mother-infant relationships, but the word “relationship” can take on many meanings. For most, the term conjures up a picture of a mother lovingly holding her infant in her arms, perhaps in an en face position in which they are looking directly into each other's eyes. The assumption is that this is a manifestation of a maternal nature of loving and supportive caring that the mother is hardwired to provide. However, in Sarah Blaffer Hrdy's thoughtful critique (Hrdy, 1999), she challenges the assumption that there is any such thing as an “essential” mothering nature. She argues that mothering, as with many other aspects of animal behavior, is subject to choices (or trade-offs) that may differ depending on the experiences and contexts in which mothering is taking place.

If Hrdy's position is correct, then the implications for understanding what gets the lives of human infants off to a good start are enormous. Among other things, the argument implies that good mothering is not guaranteed, but rather that it is contingent on a variety of factors, only some of which we know very much about.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barr, R. G. (1990a). The early crying paradox: a modest proposal. Human Nature, 1, 355–89.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G. (1990b). The normal crying curve: what do we really know? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 32, 356–62.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G., Bakeman, R., Konner, M., & Adamson, L. (1987). Crying in!Kung infants: distress signals in a responsive context [Abstract]. American Journal of Diseases of Children 141, 386.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G., & Elias, M. F. (1988). Nursing interval and maternal responsivity: effect on early infant crying. Pediatrics, 81, 529–36.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G., Holsti, L., Young, S. N., Paterson, J. A., & MacMartin, L. M. (2005). Breast feeding reduces biobehavioral responses to diphtheria-pertussis-tetanus immunizations in infants at two and four months Presented at the 3rd Annual Canadian Child Health Clinician Scientist National Symposium, Oct. 14-16, 2005.
Barr, R. G., Kramer, M. S., Leduc, D. G., Boisjoly, C., McVey-White, L., & Pless, I. B. (1988). Parental diary of infant cry and fuss behaviour. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 63, 380–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, R. G., Kramer, M. S., Pless, I. B., Boisjoly, C., & Leduc, D. (1989). Feeding and temperament as determinants of early infant cry/fuss behaviour. Pediatrics, 84, 514–21.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G., McMullan, S. J., Spiess, H., Leduc, D. J., Yaremko, J., Barfield, R. et al. (1991). Carrying as colic “therapy”: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics, 87, 623–30.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G., Pantel, M. S., Young, S. N., Wright, J. H., Hendricks, L. A., & Gravel, R. G. (1999a). The response of crying newborns to sucrose: is it a “sweetness” effect? Physiology and Behavior, 66, 409–17.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G., Quek, V., Cousineau, D., Oberlander, T. F., Brian, J. A., & Young, S. N. (1994). Effects of intraoral sucrose on crying, mouthing, and hand-mouth contact in newborn and six-week-old infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 36, 608–18.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G., & Young, S. N. (1999). A two-phase model of the soothing taste response: implications for a taste probe of temperament and emotion regulation. In Lewis, M. & Ramsay, D. (Eds.), Soothing and Stress (pp. 109–37). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Barr, R. G., Young, S. N., Wright, J. H., Gravel, R., & Alkawaf, R. (1999b). Differential calming responses to sucrose taste in crying infants with and without colic. Pediatrics, 103, 1–9.Google Scholar
Bell, S. M. & Ainsworth, D. S. (1972). Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child Development, 43, 1171–1190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben Shaul, D. M. (1962). The composition of the milk of wild animals. International Zoo Yearbook, 4, 333–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benton, D., Brett, V., & Brain, P. F. (1987). Glucose improves attention and reaction to frustration in children. Biological Psychology, 24, 95–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blass, E. M., & Barr, R. G. (2000). Evolutionary biology and medical practice: management of infant pain experience. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21, 283–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blass, E. M., & Ciaramitaro, V. (1994). A new look at some old mechanisms in human newborns: taste and tactile determinants of state, affect, and action. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(1), 1–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blass, E. M., Fillion, T. J., Rochat, P., Hoffmeyer, L. B., & Metzher, M. A. (1989). Sensorimotor and motivational determinants of hand-mouth coordination in 1–3-day-old human infants. Developmental Psychology, 25, 963–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blass, E. M., Fillion, T. J., Weller, A., & Brunson, L. (1990). Separation of opioid from nonopioid mediation of affect in neonatal rats: nonopioid mechanisms mediate maternal contact influences. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 625–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blass, E. M., Shide, D. J., Zaw-Mon, C., & Sorrentino, J.(1995). Mother as shield: differential effects of contact and nursing on pain responsivity in infant rats – evidence for nonopioid mediation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 342–53.CrossRef
Blass, E. M., & Smith, B. A. (1992). Differential effects of sucrose, fructose, glucose, and lactose on crying in 1- to 3-day-old human infants: qualitative and quantitative considerations. Developmental Psychology, 28(5), 804–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blurton-Jones, N. (1972). Comparative aspects of mother-infant contact. In Blurton-Jones, N. (Ed.), Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour (pp. 305–28). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ewer, R. F. (1968). Ethology of Mammals. London: Logos Press.CrossRef
Ghosh, S, Barr, R., Paterson, J., Huot, S., and Brody, M. (2007). Infants with and without colic demonstrate a similar diurnal pattern of sucrose-induced soothing. 10th International Infant Cry Research Workshop, Dragor, Denmark [July 5, 2007].
Gormally, S. M., Barr, R. G., Wertheim, L., Alkawaf, R., Calinoiu, N., & Young, S. N. (2001). Contact and nutrient caregiving effects on newborn infant pain responses. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43, 28–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graillon, A., Barr, R. G., Young, S. N., Wright, J. H., & Hendricks, L. A. (1997). Differential response to oral sucrose, quinine and corn oil in crying human newborns. Physiology and Behavior, 62, 317–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofer, M. A. (1984). Relationships as regulators: a psychobiologic perspective on bereavement. Psychosomatic Medicine, 46(3), 183–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofer, M. A. (1994a). Early relationships as regulators of infant physiology and behavior. Acta Paediatrica Supplement, 397, 9–18.Google Scholar
Hofer, M. A. (1994b). Hidden regulators in attachment, separation, and loss. In Fox, N. A. (Ed.), The Development of Emotion Regulation: Biological and Behavioral Considerations (pp. 192–207). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Hofer, M. A. (1996). Multiple regulators of ultrasonic vocalization in the infant rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 21(2), 203–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horne, P., Barr, R. G., Zelazo, P. R., Valiante, G., & Young, S. N. (2006). Glucose enhances newborn memory for spoken words. Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 574–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother Nature: A History of Mothers, Infants, and Natural Selection. New York: Pantheon Books.
Hubbard, F. O. A., & IJzendoorn, M. H. (1987). Maternal unresponsiveness and infant crying. a critical replication of the Bell and Ainsworth study. In Tavecchio, L. W. C. & IJzendoorn, M. H. (Eds.), Attachment in Social Networks (pp. 339–75). North Holland: Elsevier Science.
Hunziker, U. A., & Barr, R. G. (1986). Increased carrying reduces infant crying: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics, 77, 641–8.Google Scholar
Keating, D. P., & Hertzman, C. (1999). Developmental Health and the Wealth of Nations: Social, Biological, and Educational Dynamics. New York: The Guilford Press.
Kehoe, P., & Blass, E. M. (1986a). Behaviorally functional opioid systems in infant rats: II. Evidence for pharmacological, physiological, and psychological mediation of pain and stress. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 624–30.Google Scholar
Kehoe, P., & Blass, E. M. (1986b). Central nervous system mediation of positive and negative reinforcement in neonatal albino rats. Developmental Brain Research, 27, 69–75.Google Scholar
Kehoe, P., & Blass, E. M. (1986c). Opioid-mediation of separation distress in 10-day-old rats: reversal of stress with maternal stimuli. Developmental Psychobiology, 19, 385–98.Google Scholar
Kennell, J. H. (1980). Are we in the midst of a revolution? American Journal of Diseases of Children, 134, 303–10.Google Scholar
Konner, M. J. (1976). Maternal care, infant behavior, and development among the!Kung. In Lee, R. B. & DeVore, I. (Eds.), Kalahari Hunter-Gatherers, Studies of the!Kung San and Their Neighbors (pp. 218–45). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Konner, M. J., & Worthman, C. (1980). Nursing frequency, gonadal function, and birth spacing among!Kung hunter-gatherers. Science, 207, 788–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lozoff, B., & Brittenham, G. (1979). Infant care: cache or carry. The Journal of Pediatrics, 95, 478–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oberlander, T. F., Barr, R. G., Young, S. N., & Brian, J. A. (1992). Short-term effects of feed composition on sleeping and crying in newborn infants. Pediatrics, 90(5), 733–40.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1986). The neurochemistry of behavior. In Rosenzweig, M. R. & Porter, L. W. (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology, Volume 37 (pp. 77–107). Palo Alto: Annual Reviews Inc.
St. James-Roberts, I., Alvarez, M., Csipke, E., Abramsky, T., Goodwin, J., & Sorgenfrei, E. (2006). Infant crying and sleeping in London, Copenhagen, and when parents adopt a ‘proximal’ form of care. Pediatrics, 117, e1146–e1155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
St. James-Roberts, I., Hurry, J., & Bowyer, J. (1993). Objective confirmation of crying durations in infants referred for excessive crying. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 68(1), 82–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valiante, G., Barr, R. G., Zelazo, P. R., Papageorgiou, A., & Young, S. N. (2006). A typical feeding enhances memory for spoken words in healthy 2- to-3-day-old newborns. Pediatrics, 117, 476–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeifman, D., Delaney, S., & Blass, E. M. (1996). Sweet taste, looking, and calm in 2- and 4-week-old infants: the eyes have it. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1090–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. R., Weiss, M. J., Randolph, M., Swain, I. U., & Moore, D. S. (1987). The effects of delay on neonatal retention of habituated head-turning. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 417–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×