Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T17:43:03.427Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Bringing a Population Health Perspective to Early Biodevelopment: An Emerging Approach

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 May 2011

Clyde Hertzman
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia
Daniel P. Keating
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Get access

Summary

This chapter describes the rationale for population-based monitoring of early child development (ECD); outlines our achievements to date in producing such ECD data for a large population (the kindergarten children of British Columbia, Canada); and shows how this work can support biodevelopmental research regarding the extent to which social gradients in brain and biological development are explained by experience “getting under the skin.”

GRADIENTS IN EARLY CHILD DEVELOPMENT

Inequalities in child development emerge in a systematic fashion over the first five years of life, according to well-recognized factors: family income, parental education, parenting style, neighborhood safety and cohesion, neighborhood socioeconomic status, and access to quality childcare and other developmental opportunities. Three broad domains of ECD are of special relevance: physical, social/emotional, and language/cognitive. By age 5, as one goes from the families with the lowest to highest incomes, least to most parental education, and least to most nurturing and interactive parenting style, the average quality of early child experiences gradually increases and produces a gradient in child development (Hertzman et al., 2002). For example, data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth from the late 1990s (Willms, 2002) showed a gradient in the risk of receptive language delay, increasing gradually from the children in the highest income decile of Canadian families (5.2 percent delayed) to the poorest decile (approximately 26 percent delayed).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barker, D. J. (1995). The Wellcome Foundation Lecture 1994. The fetal origins of adult disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: B Biological Science, 262, 37–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, M., & Lipman, E. (2002). Do places matter? Socioeconomic disadvantage and behavioural problems of children in Canada. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(2), 378–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G., Klebanov, P., & Sealand, N. (1993). Do neighborhoods influence child and adolescent development? American Journal of Sociology, 99(2), 353–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burton, L., & Jarrett, R. (2000). In the mix, yet on the margins: The place of families in urban neighborhood and child development research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 1114–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comb, M., & Goodman, H. M. (1990). CpG methylation inhibits proenkephalin gene expression and binding of the transcription factor AP-2. Nucleic Acids Res, 18, 3975–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, Lori J., Dooley, M., & Phipps, S. (2004). Child well-being and neighborhood quality: Evidence from the Canadian National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth. Social Science and Medicine, 58, 1917–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Morison, S. J., Chisholm, K., & Schuder, M. (2001). Salivary cortisols in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Development and Psychopathology. 13, 611–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., & Nelson, C. A. (1994). Event-related potentials in year-old infants: Relations with emotionality and cortisol. Child Development, 65, 80–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H Brooks Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Hertzman, C. (2000). The biological embedding of early experience and its effects on health in adulthood. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 896, 85–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertzman, C., McLean, S., Kohen, D., Dunn, J., & Evans, T. (2002). Early Development in Vancouver: Report of the Community Asset Mapping Project (CAMP). Human Early Learning Partnership [cited. Available from http://www.earlylearning.ubc.ca/vancouvermaps.pdf.]
Hertzman, C., Power, C., Matthews, S., & Manor, O. (2001). Using an interactive framework of society and life course to explain self-rated health in early adulthood. Social Science and Medicine, 53, 1575–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janus, M., & Offord, D. R. (2000). Reporting on readiness to learn in Canada ISUMA. Canadian Journal of Policy Research, 1, 71–5.Google Scholar
Jefferis, B., Power, C., & Hertzman, C. (2002). Birth weight, childhood socioeconomic environment, and cognitive development in the 1958 birth cohort. British Medical Journal, 325, 305–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jencks, C., & Mayer, S. (1990). The social consequences of growing up in a poor neighborhood. In Lynn, L. & McGeary, M. (Eds.), Inner-city poverty in the United States. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Jenuwein, T., & Allis, C. D. (2001). Translating the histone code. Science, 293, 1074–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, P. L. et al. (1998). Methylated DNA and MeCP2 recruit histone deacetylase to repress transcription. Nat Genet, 19, 187–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keating, D. P. (2007). Formative evaluation of the Early Development Instrument: Progress and prospects. Early Education and Development, 18(3), 561–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, D. P. (2009). Social interactions in human development: Pathways to health and capabilities. In Hall, P. & Lamont, M. (Eds.), Successful societies: Institutions, cultural repertoires, and population health. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Keating, P., & Hertzman, C. (1999). Developmental health and the wealth of nations: Social, biological, and educational dynamics. New York: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Kershaw, P., Irwin, L., Trafford, K., & Hertzman, C. (2006). British Columbia Atlas of Child Development. Vancouver: UBC Press.Google Scholar
Kohen, D., Brooks-Gunn, J., Leventhal, T., & Hertzman, C. (2002). Neighborhood income and physical and social disorder in Canada: Associations with young children's competencies. Child Development, 73(6), 1844–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kristenson, M., Orth-Goméer, K., Kucinskienë, Z., Bergdahl, B., Calkauskas, H., Balinkyniene, I., et al. (1998). Attenuated cortisol response to a standardized stress test in Lithuanian versus Swedish men: The LiVicordia study. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 5(1), 17–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, L., Power, C., Kelly, S., & Kirschbaum, C., & Hertzman, C. (2007). Lifetime socioeconomic position and cortisol secretion patterns in mid-life. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32(7), 824–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lupien, S. J., King, S., Meaney, M. J., & McEwen, B. S. (2001). Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 653–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEwen, B. (1998). Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. New England Journal of Medicine, 338, 171–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meaney, M. J. (2001). Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 1161–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nan, X., Campoy, F. J., & Bird, A. (1997). MeCP2 is a transcriptional repressor with abundant binding sites in genomic chromatin. Cell, 88, 471–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
,National Research Council Institute of Medicine. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
,Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development. (2006). Starting strong II. Early childhood education and care. Paris, France. ISBN 92–64–03545–1.Google Scholar
Power, C., Li, L., Atherton, K., & Hertzman, C. (2010). Psychological health throughout life and adult cortisol patterns at age 45y. Psychoneuroenocrinology, doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.06.010CrossRef
Power, C., Li, L., & Hertzman, C. (2006). Patterns of growth from birth to adulthood, adiposity and cortisol in mid-adulthood. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 91, 4264–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Power, C., Li, L., & Hertzman, C. (2008). Cognitive development and cortisol patterns in mid-life: Findings from a British birth cohort. Psychoneuroendorcinology, 33(4), 530–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramey, C. T., & Ramey, S. L. (2005). Early learning and school readiness: Can early intervention make a difference?Washington, DC: Georgetown University Center on Health and Education.Google Scholar
Razin, A., & Riggs, A. D. (1980). DNA methylation and gene function. Science, 210, 604–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Razin, A., & Szyf, M. (1984). DNA methylation patterns. Formation and function. Biochim Biophys Acta, 782, 331–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapolsky, R. M. (1992). Stress, the aging brain, and the mechanisms of neuron death. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schweinhart, L. J. (2004). The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study through Age 40. Summary, conclusions, and frequently asked questions. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Educational Research Foundation.Google Scholar
Strahl, B. D., & Allis, C. D. (2000).The language of covalent histone modifications. Nature, 403, 41–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szyf, M. (2003a). DNA methylation enzymology. In Encyclopedia of the human genome. New York: Macmillan Publishers, Nature Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Szyf, M. (2003b). Targeting DNA methylation in cancer. Ageing Res Rev, 2, 299–328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szyf, M. (2009). Early life, the epigenome, and human health. Acta Paediatrica, 98, 1082–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turley, R., & Lopez, T. (2003). When do neighborhoods matter? The role of race and neighborhood peers. Social Science Research, 32(1), 61–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, F. A., Champagne, F. A., Alessio, A. C. D., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., Dymov, S., Szyf, M., & Meaney, M. J. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 847–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weaver, I. C., Meaney, M. J., & Szyf, M. (2006). Maternal care effects on the hippocampal transcriptome and anxiety-mediated behaviors in the offspring that are reversible in adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 3480–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willms, J. D. (Ed.). (2002). Vulnerable children. Edmonton, AB: University of Alberta Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×