Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T04:26:24.174Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Referring expression generation in interaction: A graph-based perspective

from Part II - Reference

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2014

Emiel Krahmer
Affiliation:
Tilburg University
Martijn Goudbeek
Affiliation:
Tilburg University
Mariët Theune
Affiliation:
University of Twente
Amanda Stent
Affiliation:
AT&T Research, Florham Park, New Jersey
Srinivas Bangalore
Affiliation:
AT&T Research, Florham Park, New Jersey
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Buying new chairs can be complicated. Many constraints have to be kept in mind, including your financial situation, the style and color of the furniture you already own and possibly also the taste of your partner. But once you have made a tentative choice (say, the chair in Figure 6.1 on the left), there is one final hurdle: you have to inform the seller of your desire to buy it. Furniture stores tend to contain many chairs, so somehow you need to refer to your chair of choice, for example I'd like to buy the wooden chair with the thin legs and solid seat, the red one with the open back. It is hardly surprising that many people in this situation resort to pointing (that one). Of course, it would be helpful to know that salespeople might refer to this chair as the red Keystone chair, because that would allow you to adapt to their way of referring.

This problem illustrates the importance of reference in everyday interactions: people can only exchange information about objects when they agree on how to refer to those objects. How this agreement may arise, and how we can model this in natural language generation, is the topic of this chapter. We argue that two possibly competing forces play a role. On the one hand, speakers may have inherent preferences for certain properties when referring to objects in a given domain. On the other, they may also have a tendency to adapt to the references produced by their dialogue partner. We describe how preferences can be determined, and how they interact with adaptation. We model this tradeoff using a graph-based referring expression generation algorithm (Krahmer et al., 2003).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2014

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arts, A. (2004). Overspecification in Instructive Texts. PhD thesis, Tilburg University.Google Scholar
Belz, A. and Gatt, A. (2008). Intrinsic vs. extrinsic evaluation measures for referring expression generation. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Language Technologies (ACL-HLT), pages 197-200, Columbus, OH. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., Pearson, J., and McLean, J. F. (2010). Linguistic alignment between people and computers. Journal of Pragmatics, 42(9):2355-2368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S. E. and Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22(6):1482-1493.Google ScholarPubMed
Buschmeier, H., Bergmann, K., and Kopp, S. (2010). Modelling and evaluation of lexical and syntactic alignment with a priming-based microplanner. In Krahmer, E. and Theune, M., editors, Empirical Methods in Natural Language Generation, pages 85-104. Springer, Berlin.Google Scholar
Chartrand, G. and Oellermann, O. R. (1993). Applied and Algorithmic Graph Theory. McGraw-Hill, New York, NY.Google Scholar
Croitoru, M. and van Deemter, K. (2007). A conceptual graph approach to the generation of referring expressions. In Proceedings of the International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence (IJCAI), pages 2456-2461, Hyderabad, India. International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence.Google Scholar
Dale, R. (1989). Cooking up referring expressions. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (ACL), pages 68-75, Vancouver, Canada. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Dale, R. and Haddock, N. (1991). Content determination in the generation of referring expressions. Computational Intelligence, 7(4):252-265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dale, R. and Reiter, E. (1995). Computational interpretation of the Gricean maxims in the generation of referring expressions. Cognitive Science, 19(2):233-263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Ruiter, J. P., Bangerter, A., and Dings, P. (2012). The interplay between gesture and speech in the production of referring expressions: Investigating the tradeoff hypothesis. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(2):232-248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Eugenio, B., Jordan, P. W., Thomason, R. H., and Moore, J. D. (2000). The agreement process: An empirical investigation of human-human computer-mediated collaborative dialogue. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 53(6):1017-1076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelhardt, P. E., Bailey, K. G. D., and Ferreira, F. (2006). Do speakers and listeners observe the Gricean maxim of quantity?Journal of Memory and Language, 54(4):554-573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V. S., Slevc, L. R., and Rogers, E. S. (2005). How do speakers avoid ambiguous linguistic expressions?Cognition, 96(3):263-284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garey, M. R. and Johnson, D. S. (1979). Computers and Intractability: A Guide to the Theory of NP-Completeness. W. H. Freeman, NewYork, NY.Google Scholar
Gatt, A. (2007). Generating Coherent References to Multiple Entities. PhD thesis, Department of Computing Science, University of Aberdeen.Google Scholar
Gatt, A. and Beiz, A. (2010). Introducing shared tasks to NLG: The TUNA shared task evaluation challenges. In Krahmer, E. and Theune, M., editors, Empirical Methods in Natural Language Generation, pages 264-293. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.Google Scholar
Gatt, A., Belz, A., and Kow, E. (2008). The TUNA challenge 2008: Overview and evaluation results. In Proceedings of the International Workshop on Natural Language Generation (INLG), pages 198-207, Salt Fork, OH. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Gatt, A., Belz, A., and Kow, E. (2009). The TUNA-REG challenge 2009: Overview and evaluation results. In Proceedings of the European Workshop on Natural Language Generation (ENLG), pages 174-182, Athens, Greece. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Gatt, A., Goudbeek, M., and Krahmer, E. (2011). Attribute preference and priming in reference production: Experimental evidence and computational modelling. In Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci), pages 2627-2632, Boston, MA. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Gatt, A., van der Sluis, I., and van Deemter, K. (2007). Evaluating algorithms for the generation of referring expressions using a balanced corpus. In Proceedings of the European Workshop on Natural Language Generation (ENLG), pages 49-56, Saarbrücken, Germany. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Gibbons, A. M. (1985). Algorithmic Graph Theory. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Gorniak, P. J. and Roy, D. (2004). Grounded semantic composition for visual scenes. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 21:429-470.Google Scholar
Goudbeek, M. and Krahmer, E. (2010). Preferences versus adaptation during referring expression generation. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (ACL), pages 55-59, Uppsala, Sweden. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Guhe, M. and Bard, E. G. (2008). Adapting referring expressions to the task environment. In Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci), pages 2404-2409, Washington, DC. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Gupta, S. and Stent, A. (2005). Automatic evaluation of referring expression generation using corpora. In Proceedings of the Workshop on Using Corpora for Natural Language Generation (UCNLG), Birmingham, UK. ITRI, University of Brighton.Google Scholar
Hartigan, J. and Wong, M. (1979). Algorithm AS 136: A k-means clustering algorithm. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series C (Applied Statistics), 28(1):100-108.Google Scholar
Janarthanam, S. and Lemon, O. (2010). Learning adaptive referring expression generation policies for spoken dialogue systems. In Krahmer, E. and Theune, M., editors, Empirical Methods in Natural Language Generation, pages 67-84. Springer, Berlin.Google Scholar
Jordan, P. W. and Walker, M. A. (2005). Learning content selection rules for generating object descriptions in dialogue. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 24(1):157-194.Google Scholar
Koolen, R., Gatt, A., Goudbeek, M., and Krahmer, E. (2009). Need I say more? On factors causing referential overspecification. In Proceedings of the CogSci workshop on the Production of Referring Expressions, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Available from http://pre2009.uvt.nl/. Accessed on 11/24/2013.Google Scholar
Krahmer, E., Theune, M., Viethen, J., and Hendrickx, I. (2008). GRAPH: The costs of redundancy in referring expressions. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Natural Language Generation (INLG), pages 227-229, Salt Fork, OH. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Krahmer, E., van Erk, S., and Verleg, A. (2003). Graph-based generation of referring expressions. Computational Linguistics, 29(1):53-72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Butcher, J., Lieven, E., and Tomasello, M. (2012). Two- and four-year-olds learn to adapt referring expressions to context: Effects of distracters and feedback on referential communication. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(2):184-210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellish, C, Scott, D., Cahill, L., Paiva, D., Evans, R., and Reape, M. (2006). A reference architecture for natural language generation systems. Natural Language Engineering, 12(1):1-34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, D. R. (1970). Language and thought: Aspects of a cognitive theory of semantics. Psychological Review, 77(4):257-273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Passonneau, R. (1996). Using centering to relax Gricean informational constraints on discourse anaphoric noun phrases. Language and Speech, 39(2-3):229-264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pechmann, T. (1989). Incremental speech production and referential overspecification. Linguistics, 27(1):89-110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. and Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(2):169-226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiter, E. and Dale, R. (2000). Building Natural Language Generation Systems. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonnenschein, S. (1984). The effect of redundant communication on listeners: Why different types may have different effects. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 13(2):147-166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoia, L., Shockley, D. M., Byron, D., and Fosler-Lussier, E. (2006). Noun phrase generation for situated dialogs. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Natural Language Generation (INLG), pages 81-88, Sydney, Australia. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Theune, M., Koolen, R., Krahmer, E., and Wubben, S. (2011). Does size matter: How much data is required to train a REG algorithm? In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Language Technologies (ACL-HLT), pages 660-664, Portland, OR. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
van Deemter, K., Gatt, A., van der Sluis, I., and Power, R. (2012). Generation of referring expressions: Assessing the Incremental Algorithm. Cognitive Science, 36(5):799-836.Google ScholarPubMed
van Deemter, K. and Krahmer, E. (2007). Graphs and booleans: On the generation of referring expressions. In Bunt, H. and Muskens, R., editors, Computing Meaning, volume 3, pages 397-422. Springer, Dordrecht, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
van Deemter, K., van der Sluis, I., and Gatt, A. (2006). Building a semantically transparent corpus for the generation of referring expressions. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Natural Language Generation (INLG), pages 130-132, Sydney, Australia. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
van der Sluis, I. and Krahmer, E. (2007). Generating multimodal referring expressions. Discourse Processes, 44(3):145-174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Der Wege, M. (2009). Lexical entrainment and lexical differentiation in reference phrase choice. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(4):448-463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viethen, J. and Dale, R. (2006). Algorithms for generating referring expressions: Do they do what people do? In Proceedings of the International Conference on Natural Language Generation (INLG), pages 63-72, Sydney, Australia. Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Viethen, J., Dale, R., Krahmer, E., Theune, M., and Touset, P. (2008). Controlling redundancy in referring expressions. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC), Marrakech, Morocco. European Language Resources Association.Google Scholar
Walker, M. A., Stent, A., Mairesse, F., and Prasad, R. (2007). Individual and domain adaptation in sentence planning for dialogue. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 30(1):413-456.Google Scholar
White, M., Rajkumar, R., Ito, K., and Speer, S. R. (2009). Eye tracking for the online evaluation of prosody in speech synthesis: Not so fast! In Proceedings of the International Conference on Spoken Language Processing (INTERSPEECH), pages 2523-2526, Brighton, UK. International Speech Communication Association.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×