Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T18:07:24.460Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Something to make peace for: conflict management and resolution

from Part II - How conflicts shape societies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2016

Ivan Norscia
Affiliation:
Università degli Studi, Pisa
Elisabetta Palagi
Affiliation:
Università degli Studi, Pisa
Get access

Summary

Man is neither, by nature, peaceful nor warlike. Some conditions lead to war, others do not.

Otterbein 1997, p. 272

The end of social groups

After the death of King Alfonso XI of Castile in 1350, his eldest son Peter took control of the territory. Peter's mother, Maria of Portugal, had Alfonso's mistress, Eleanor de Guzman, killed. The death of Eleanor, who had given birth to ten children (Peter's half-siblings) split the royal descendants (legitimate and illegitimate) into two rival factions: Peter and his allies, and Alfonso's children by Eleanor, the Trastámaras, led by Henry. In the fights over the territory, Peter defeated the coalition led by his half-brother Henry in 1356 and 1360 and had two half-brothers (Henry's full brothers John and Peter) executed. Henry was forced to flee but later gained the support of Aragon, France, and many nobles of Castile. Meanwhile, Peter allied with Edward, heir to the English throne. This alliance allowed Peter to maintain the control upon the territory, until when Edward fell ill and, for political reasons, withdrew his support to Peter. Eventually, Henry took over the territory as Henry II and was responsible for the death of his half-brother Peter. John II, the great-grandson of Henry II, came to power after his mother Catherine of Lancaster, the regent, died in 1418. John II lacked authority and the territory became a battlefield for the nobles, more or less related to the ruling family, to gain power. As a result, the Trastámara family was, again, divided into two main factions: John II (with his supporting nobles) and his cousin Alfonso V of Aragon with his allies and brothers, Henry and John. In 1420, John II was kidnapped by his cousin Henry and then liberated by an ally, Álvaro de Luna. In 1429, Alfonso V ordered his brothers Henry and John to lead a joint attack (which was unsuccessful) against their cousin John II. In 1443 John II was once again captured by his cousin John of Aragon and the territory fell into near anarchy until 1445, when the group of nobles supporting John II, led by Álvaro de Luna, won a battle and Henry of Aragon was killed.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Missing Lemur Link
An Ancestral Step in the Evolution of Human Behaviour
, pp. 112 - 145
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Antonacci, D., Norscia, I. & Palagi, E. (2010). Stranger to familiar: wild strepsirrhines manage xenophobia by playing. PLos ONE, 5(10) e13218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013218.Google Scholar
Arnold, K. & Aureli, F. (2007). Postconflict reconciliation. In: Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, A. C., Panger, M. & Bearder, S. (eds.), Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press, pp. 592–608.
Arnold, K. & Whiten, A. (2001). Post-conflict behaviour of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo forest, Uganda. Behaviour, 138, 649–690.Google Scholar
Aureli, F. & de Waal, F. B. M. (1997). Inhibition of social behavior in chimpanzees under high-density conditions. American Journal of Primatology, 41, 213–228.Google Scholar
Aureli, F. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2000). Natural Conflict Resolution. California: Regents of the University of California.
Aureli, F. & Schaffner, C. M. (2002). Empathy as a special case of emotional mediation of social behavior. Behavioural and Brain Science, 25, 23–24.Google Scholar
Aureli, F. & Schaffner, C. M. (2007). Aggression and conflict management at fusion in spider monkeys. Biology Letters, 3, 147–149.Google Scholar
Aureli, F. & van Schaik, C. P. (1991). Post-conflict behaviour in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis): coping with the uncertainty. Ethology, 89, 101–114.Google Scholar
Aureli, F., Das, M. & Veenema, H. C. (1997). Differential kinship effect on reconciliation in three species of macaques (Macaca fascicularis, M. fuscata, and M. sylvanus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111(1), 91–99.Google Scholar
Aureli, F., Cords, M. & van Schaik, C. P. (2002). Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: a predictive framework. Animal Behaviour, 64, 325–343.Google Scholar
Baan, C., Bergmüller, R., Smith, D. W. & Molnar, B. (2014). Conflict management in free-ranging wolves, Canis lupus.Animal Behaviour, 90, 327–334.Google Scholar
Balasubramaniam, K. N., Dittmar, K., Berman, C. M., et al. (2012). Hierarchical steepness, counter-aggression, and macaque social style scale. American Journal of Primatology, 74, 915–925.Google Scholar
Blumstein, D. T., Ebensperger, L. A., Hayes, L. D., et al. (2010). Toward an integrative understanding of social behavior: new models and new opportunities. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 1–9.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. & Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Boesch, C., Head, J., Tagg, N., et al. (2006). Fatal chimpanzee attack in Loango National Park, Gabon. International Journal of Primatology, 28, 1025–1034.Google Scholar
Butovskaya, M., Verbeek, P., Ljungberg, T. & Lunardini, A. (2000). A multi-cultural view of peacemaking among young children. In: Aureli, F. and Waal, F. B. M. de (eds), Natural Conflict Resolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press, pp. 243–258.
Calhoun, J. B. (1962). Population density and social pathology. Scientific American, 206, 139–148.Google Scholar
Call, J., Aureli, F. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Postconflict third party affiliation in stumptailed macaques. Animal Behaviour, 63, 209–216.Google Scholar
Chaffin, C. L., Friedlen, K. & de Waal, F. B. M., (1995). Dominance style of Japanese macaques compared with rhesus and stumptail macaques. International Journal of Primatology, 25, 1283–1312.Google Scholar
Ciani, F., Dall'Olio, S., Stanyon, R. & Palagi, E. (2012). Social tolerance and adult play in macaque societies: a comparison with different human cultures. Animal Behaviour, 84, 1313–1322.Google Scholar
Clayton, N. S. & Emery, N. J. (2007). The social life of corvids. Current Biology, 17, R652–R656.Google Scholar
Deutsch, M. & Coleman, P. T. (2012). Psychological components of sustainable peace: an introduction. In: Psychological Components of Sustainable Peace. New York: Springer.
Cools, A. K. A., van Hout, A. J. M. & Nelissen, M. H. J. (2008). Canine reconciliation and third-party-initiated postconflict affiliation: do peacemaking social mechanisms in dogs rival those of higher primates?Ethology, 114, 53–63.Google Scholar
Cooper, M. A., Bernstein, I. S. & Hemelrijk, C. K. (2005). Reconciliation and relationship quality in Assamese macaques (Macaca assamensis). American Journal of Primatology, 65, 269–282.Google Scholar
Cordoni, G. & Norscia, I. (2014). Peace-making in marsupials: the first study in the red-necked wallaby (Macropus rufogriseus). PLoS ONE, 9(1), e86859. http://dx/doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086859.Google Scholar
Cordoni, G. & Palagi, E. (2007). Response of captive lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) to different housing conditions: testing the aggression/density and coping models. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121, 171–180.Google Scholar
Cordoni, G. & Palagi, E. (2008). Reconciliation in wolves (Canis lupus): new evidence for a comparative perspective. Ethology, 114, 298–308.Google Scholar
Cordoni, G. & Palagi, E. (2015). Being a victim or an aggressor: Different functions of triadic post-conflict interactions in wolves (Canis lupus lupus). Aggressive Behavior, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.21590.Google Scholar
Cordoni, G., Palagi, E. and Borgognini-Tarli, S. M. (2006). Reconciliation and consolation in captive western gorillas. International Journal of Primatology, 27, 1365–1382.Google Scholar
Cords, M. (1997). Friendship, alliances, reciprocity and repair. In: Whiten, A. & Byrne, R. W., (eds), Machiavellian Intelligence II. Cambridge University Press, pp. 24–49.
Cords, M. & Aureli, F. (2000). Reconciliation and relationship qualities. In: Aureli, F. & Waal, F. B. M. de (eds), Natural Conflict Resolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press, pp. 177–198.
Cozzi, A., Sighieri, C., Gazzano, A., Nicol, C. J. & Baragli, P. (2010). Post-conflict friendly reunion in a permanent group of horses (Equus caballus). Behavioural Processes, 85, 185–190.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (1987). Tension regulation and nonreproductive functions of sex in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). National Geographic Research, 3, 318–335.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (1989a). Peacemaking Among Primates. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
de Waal, F. B. M. (1989b). The myth of a simple relation between space and aggression in captive primates. Zoo Biology Suppl., 1, 141–148.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good Natured. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
de Waal, F. B. M. (2000). Primates—a natural heritage of conflict resolution. Science, 289, 586–590.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (2009). The Age of Empathy. New York: Harmony Books.
de Waal, F. B. M. & Aureli, F. (1996). Consolation, reconciliation and a possible cognitive difference between macaques and chimpanzees. In: Russon, A. E., Bard, K. A., Parker, S. T. (eds), Reaching into Thought: The Minds of Great Apes.Cambridge University Press, pp. 80–110.
de Waal, F. B. M. & Luttrell, L. M. (1989). Toward a comparative socioecology of the genus Macaca: different dominance style in rhesus and stumptailed monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 19, 83–109.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. & van Roosmalen, A. (1979). Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 5, 55–66.Google Scholar
Waal, F. B. M. de & Yoshihara, D. (1983). Reconciliation and redirected affection in rhesus monkeys. Behaviour, 85, 224–241.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M., Aureli, F. & Judge, P. G. (2000). Coping with crowding. Scientific American, 282, 76–81.Google Scholar
Demaria, C. & Thierry, B. (2001). A comparative study of reconciliation in rhesus and Tonkean macaques. Behaviour, 138, 397–410.Google Scholar
dos Reis, M., Inoue, J., Hasegawa, M., et al. (2012). Phylogenomic datasets provide both precision and accuracy in estimating the timescale of placental mammal phylogeny. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 3491–3500.Google Scholar
Ezenwa, V. O., Gerardo, N. M., Inouye, D. W., Medina, M. & Xavier, J. B. (2012). Animal behavior and the microbiome. Science, 338, 198–199.Google Scholar
Fagen, R. (1981). Animal Play Behavior. New York: Oxford University Press, p. 684.
Field, T. (2014). Touch. MIT Press.
Firth, R. (1972). Verbal and bodily rituals of greeting and parting. In: J. S. La Fontaine (ed.), The Interpretation of RitualLondon, UK: Routledge, pp. 1–38.
Floyd, K. (1999). All touches are not created equal: effects of form and duration on observers’ interpretations of an embrace. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 283–299.Google Scholar
Floyd, K. (2001). Human affection exchange: I. Reproductive probability as a predictor of men's affection with their sons. The Journal of Men's Studies, 10, 39–50.Google Scholar
Fraser, O. N. & Aureli, F. (2008). Reconciliation, consolation and postconflict behavioral specificity in chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 1–10.Google Scholar
Fraser, O. N. & Bugnyar, T. (2010). Do ravens show consolation? Responses to distressed others. PLoS ONE, 5(5), e10605, http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010605.Google Scholar
Fraser, O. N, & Bugnyar, T. (2011). Ravens reconcile after aggressive conflicts with valuable partners. PLoS ONE, 6(3), e18118, http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018118.Google Scholar
Fraser, O. N., Schino, G. & Aureli, F. (2008). Components of relationship quality in chimpanzees. Ethology, 114, 834–843.Google Scholar
Fruth, B. & Hohmann, G. (2002). How bonobos handle hunts and harvests: why share food? In: Boesch, C., Hohmann, G. & Marchant, L. F. (eds), Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 231–243.
Fry, D. P. (2006). The Human Potential for Peace: An Anthropological Challenge to Assumptions About War and Violence.New York: Oxford University Press.
Fry, D. P. (2012). Life without war. Science, 336, 879–884.Google Scholar
Fry, D. P. (ed.) (2013). War, Peace, and Human Nature: the convergence of evolutionary and cultural views.Oxford University Press.
Fujisawa, K. K., Kutsukake, N. & Hasegawa, T. (2005). Reconciliation pattern after aggression among Japanese preschool children. Aggressive Behavior, 31, 138–152.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. & Berman, P. (2000). Reason for Hope: A Spiritual Journey.Grand Central Publishing.
Goodall, J., Bandura, A., Bergmann, E., et al. (1979). Inter-community interactions in the chimpanzee populations of the Gombe National Park. In: Hamburg, D. and McCown, E. (eds), The Great Apes.Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings, pp. 13–53.
Haller, J. & Kruk, M. R. (2006). Normal and abnormal aggression: human disorders and novel laboratory models. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 292–303.Google Scholar
Harcourt, A. H. (1979). Social relationships between adult male and female mountain gorillas in the wild. Animal Behaviour, 27, 325–342.Google Scholar
Hare, B., Wobber, V. & Wrangham, R. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Animal Behaviour, 83, 573–585.Google Scholar
Hart, D. & Sussman, R. W. (2011). In: Sussman, R. W. & Cloninger, C. R. (eds), Origins of Altruism and Cooperation. New York: Springer, pp. 19–40.
Hemelrijk, C. K., Wantia, J. & Isler, K. (2008). Female dominance over males in primates: Self-organisation and sexual dimorphism. PLoS ONE, 3(7), e2678, http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0002678.Google Scholar
Higginbottom, K. & Croft, D. B. (1999). Social learning in marsupials. In: Box, H. O. and Gibson, K. R. (eds), Mammalian Social Learning – Comparative and Ecological Perspectives. Cambridge University Press, pp. 80–101.
Hood, L. C. & Jolly, A. (1995). Troop fission in female Lemur catta at Berenty Reserve, Madagascar. International Journal of Primatology, 16, 997–1015.Google Scholar
Huxley, T. H. (1893). Evolution and Ethics. Prolegomena. http://aleph0.clarku.edu/huxley/CE9/E-EProl.html
Ichino, S. (2006). Troop fission in wild ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) at Berenty, Madagascar. American Journal of Primatology, 68, 97–102.Google Scholar
Ichino, S. & Koyama, N. (2006). Social changes in a wild population of ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta) at Berenty, Madagascar. In: Jolly, A.Sussman, R. W., Koyama, N. & Rasamimanana, H. R. (eds), Ringtailed Lemur Biology: Lemur catta in Madagascar.New York: Springer, pp. 233–244.
Isler, K. (2011). Energetic trade-offs between brain size and offspring production: marsupials confirm a general mammalian pattern. BioEssays, 33(3), 173–179.Google Scholar
Jardin, J. P. (2015). La reina María de Portugal, entre padre, marido, hijo e hijastros: la mediación imposible. e-Spania, 20, http://dx.doi.org/10.4000/e-spania.24140.
Jarman, P. J. (2000). Males in macropod society. In: Kappeler, P. (ed), Primate Males: Causes and Consequences of Variation in Group Composition. Cambridge University Press, pp. 21–33.
Johnson, R. N. (1972). Aggression in Man and Animals.Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
Jolly, A. (2004). Lords and Lemurs: Mad Scientists, Kings with Spears, and the Survival of Diversity in Madagascar. Houghton MifflinHarcourt.
Jolly, A. (2012). Berenty Reserve, Madagascar: A long time in a small space. In: Kappeler, P. M. & Watts, D. P. (eds), Long-Term Field Studies of Primates. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, pp. 21–44.
Jolly, A.Rasamimanana, H. R., Kinnaird, M. F., et al. (1993). Territoriality in Lemur catta groups during the birth season at Berenty, Madagascar. In: Kappeler, P. M. & Ganzhorn, J. U. (eds), Lemur Social Systems and their Ecological Basis.New York: Plenum, pp. 85–109.
Jolly, A.Rasamimanana, H. R., Braun, M. A., et al. (2006) Territory as bet-hedging: Lemur catta in a rich forest and an erratic climate. In: Jolly, A.Sussman, R. W., Koyama, N. & Rasamimanana, H. R. (eds), Ringtailed Lemur Biology: Lemur catta in Madagascar.New York: Springer, pp. 187–207.
Judge, P. G. (2000). Coping with crowded conditions. In: Aureli, F. & de Waal, F. B. M. (eds), Natural Conflict Resolution.Berkeley: University of California Press, pp. 129–154.
Judge, P. G. & de Waal, F. B. M. (1997). Rhesus monkey behaviour under diverse population densities: coping with long-term crowding. Animal Behaviour, 54, 643–662.Google Scholar
Judge, P. G. & Mullen, S. H. (2005). Quadratic postconflict affiliation among bystanders in a hamadyras baboon group. Animal Behaviour, 69, 1345–1355.Google Scholar
Kaburu, S. S. K., Inoue, S. & Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2013). Death of the alpha: Within-community lethal violence among chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park. American Journal of Primatology, 75, 789–797.Google Scholar
Kappeler, P. M. (1993). Reconciliation and post-conflict behaviour in ringtailed lemurs, Lemur catta and redfronted lemurs, Eulemur fulvus rufus. Animal Behaviour, 45(5), 901–915.Google Scholar
Kappeler, P. M. & van Schaik, C. P. (1992). Methodological and evolutionary aspects of reconciliation among primates. Ethology, 92, 51–69.Google Scholar
Kappeler, P. M., Barrett, L., Blumstein, D. T. & Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2013). Constraints and flexibility in mammalian social behavior: introduction and synthesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 368, 1–10.Google Scholar
Karlen, S. J. & Krubitzer, L. (2007). The functional and anatomical organization of marsupial neocortex: evidence for parallel evolution across mammals. Progress in Neurobiology, 82, 122–141.Google Scholar
Kempes, A. (2008). Preface to special issue on natural conflict resolution in humans. Behaviour, 145, 1493–1496.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. & Ferber, A. (1973). A description of some human greetings In: Michael, R. P. & Crook, J. H., Comparative Ecology and Behaviour of Primates. London, UK: Academic Press, pp. 591–668.
Koenig, A. (2002). Competition for resources and its behavioral consequences among female primates. International Journal of Primatology, 23, 759–783.Google Scholar
Koski, S. E. & Sterck, E. H. M. (2007). Triadic post-conflict affiliation in captive chimpanzees: does consolation console?Animal Behaviour, 73, 133–142.Google Scholar
Koski, S. E. & Sterck, E. H. (2009). Post-conflict third-party affiliation in chimpanzees: what's in it for the third party?American Journal of Primatology, 71, 409–418.Google Scholar
Koyama, N. (1991). Troop division and inter-troop relationships of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) at Berenty, Madagascar. In: Ehara, A., Kimura, T., Takenaka, O., Iwamoto, M. (eds), Primatology Today.Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 173–176.
Koyama, N., Nakamichi, M, Ichino, S., & Takahata, Y. (2002). Population and social dynamics changes in ring-tailed lemur troops at Berenty, Madagascar between 1989–1999. Primates, 43, 291–314.Google Scholar
Leone, A., Mignini, M., Mancini, G. & Palagi, E. (2010). Aggression does not increase friendly contacts among bystanders in geladas (Theropithecus gelada). Primates, 51, 299–305.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1966). On Aggression, trans. Marjorie, Latzke. London: Methuen.
Luo, Z-X., Yuan, C-X., Meng, Q-J. and Ji, Q. (2011). A Jurassic eutherian mammal and divergence of marsupials and placentals. Nature, 476, 442–445.Google Scholar
Martin, R. D. (1990). Primate Origins and Evolution.Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
McCallum, I. (2012). A wild psychology. In: Kahn, P. H., Jr. and Hasbach, P. H. (eds), Ecopsychology. Science, Totems, and the Technological Species. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, pp. 139–156.
Mech, L. D. (1977). Wolf-pack buffer zones as prey reservoirs. Science, 198, 320–321.Google Scholar
Mech, L. D. (1999). Alpha status, dominance, and division of labor in wolf packs. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 77, 1196–1203.Google Scholar
Mech, L. D. & Boitani, L. (2003). Wolf social ecology. In: Mech, D. & Boitani, L. (eds), Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 1–34.
Meredith, R. W., Westerman, M., Case, J. A. & Springer, M. S. (2008). A phylogeny and timescale for marsupial evolution based on sequences for five nuclear genes. Journal of Mammal Evolution, 15, 1–26.Google Scholar
Miklósi, Á. (2014). Dog Behaviour, Evolution, and Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Muller, M. N. (2002). Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees. In: Boesch, C., Hohmann, G. & Marchant, L. F. (eds), Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos.Cambridge University Press, pp. 112–124.
Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2002). Relationships of male chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. In: Boesch, C., Hohmann, G. & Marchant, L. F. (eds), Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos.Cambridge University Press, pp. 125–137.
Norscia, I. & Palagi, E. (2011). Do wild brown lemurs reconcile? Not always. Journal of Ethology, 29(1), 181–185.Google Scholar
O'Brien, T. G. (1993). Allogrooming behaviour among adult female wedge-capped capuchin monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 46, 499–510.Google Scholar
Oldroyd, B. P. (2013). Social evolution: policing without genetic conflict. Current Biology, 23(5), R208–R210.Google Scholar
Otterbein, K. F. (1997). The origins of war. Critical Review, 11, 251–277.Google Scholar
Palagi, E. (2009). Adult play fighting in a prosimian (Lemur catta): modalities and roles of tail signals. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123, 1–9.Google Scholar
Palagi, E. & Cordoni, G. (2009). Postconflict third-party affiliation in Canis lupus: do wolves share similarities with the great apes?Animal Behaviour, 78, 97–986.Google Scholar
Palagi, E. & Cordoni, G. (2012). The right time to happen: play developmental divergence in the two Pan species. PLoS ONE, 7(12), e52767. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052767.Google Scholar
Palagi, E. & Dapporto, L. (2006). Beyond odor discrimination: demonstrating individual recognition by scent in Lemur catta. Chemical Senses, 31, 437–443.Google Scholar
Palagi, E. & Dapporto, L. (2007). Females do it better. Individual recognition experiments reveal sexual dimorphism in Lemur catta (Linnaeus 1758) olfactory motivation and territorial defence. Journal of Experimental Biology, 210, 2700–2705.Google Scholar
Palagi, E. & Norscia, I. (2013). Bonobos protect and console friends and kin. PLoS ONE, 8, e79290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079290.Google Scholar
Palagi, E. & Norscia, I. (2015). The season for peace: reconciliation in a despotic species (Lemur catta). PLoS ONE, 10(11), e0142150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/ journal.pone.0142150.
Palagi, E., Paoli, T. & Borgonini-Tarli, S. (2004). Reconciliation and consolation in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). American Journal of Primatology, 62, 15–30.Google Scholar
Palagi, E., Cordoni, G. & Borgognini-Tarli, S. M. (2004). Immediate and delayed benefits of play behaviour: New evidence from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology, 110, 949–962.Google Scholar
Palagi, E., Paoli, T. & Tarli, S. B. (2005). Aggression and reconciliation in two captive groups of Lemur catta. International Journal of Primatology, 26, 279–294.Google Scholar
Palagi, E., Paoli, T. & Borgognini-Tarli, S. M. (2006). Immediate and delayed benefits of play behaviour: new evidence from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). International Journal of Primatology, 27, 1257–1270.Google Scholar
Palagi, E., Cordoni, G. & Borgonini-Tarli, S. (2006). Possible roles of consolation in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 129, 105–111.Google Scholar
Paoli, T., Tacconi, G., Borgognini-Tarli, S. & Palagi, E. (2007). Influence of feeding and short-term crowding on the sexual repertoire of captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). Annales Zoologici Fennici, 44, 84–88.Google Scholar
Palagi, E., Antonacci, D. & Norscia, I. (2008a). Peacemaking on treetops: first evidence of reconciliation from a wild prosimian (Propithecus verreauxi). Animal Behaviour, 76, 737–747.Google Scholar
Palagi, E., Chiarugi, E. & Cordoni, G. (2008b). Peaceful post-conflict interactions between aggressors and bystanders in captive lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). American Journal of Primatology, 70, 949–955.Google Scholar
Palagi, E, Dall'Olio, S, Demuru, E, & Stanyon, R. R. (2014a). Exploring the evolutionary foundations of empathy: consolation in monkeys. Evolution and Human Behavior.Google Scholar
Palagi, E, Norscia, I, & Spada, G (2014b) Relaxed open mouth as a playful signal in wild ring-tailed lemurs. American Journal of Primatology, 76, 1074–1083.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., Roseth, C. J., Mliner, S., et al. (2007). Social dominance in preschool classrooms. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121, 54–64.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., Van Ryzin, M. J., Roseth, C. J., et al. (2011a). Behavioral and social cognitive processes in preschool children's social dominance. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 1–10.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., Bohn-Gettler, C. M., Dupuis, D., et al. (2011b). An empirical examination of sex differences in scoring preschool children's aggression. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 232–238.Google Scholar
Pellis, S. M. (2002a). Sex differences in play fighting revisited: traditional and nontraditional mechanisms of sexual differentiation in rats. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 17–26.Google Scholar
Pellis, S. M. & Pellis, V. C. (1997). Targets, tactics, and the open mouth face during play fighting in three species of primates. Aggressive Behavior, 23, 41–57.Google Scholar
Pennisi, E. (2013). How do microbes shape animal development?Science, 340, 1159–1160.Google Scholar
Pereira, M. E. & Kappeler, P. M. (1997). Divergent systems of agonistic behaviour in lemurid primates. Behaviour, 134, 225–274.Google Scholar
Peterson, R. O., Jacobs, A. K., Drummer, T. D., Mech, L. D. & Smith, D. W. (2002). Leadership behaviour in relation to dominance and reproductive status in grey wolves, Canis lupus. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 80, 1405–1412.Google Scholar
Petit, O. & Thierry, B. (1994). Aggressive and peaceful interventions in conflicts in Tonkean macaques. Animal Behaviour, 48, 1427–1436.Google Scholar
Petit, O, Abegg, C, Thierry, B (1997). A comparative study of aggression and conciliation in three ercopithecine monkeys (Macaca fuscata, Macaca nigra, Papio papio). Behaviour, 134, 415–432.Google Scholar
Polizzi di Sorrentino, E. P., Schino, G., Visalberghi, E. & Aureli, F. (2010). What time is it? Coping with expected feeding time in capuchin monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 80, 117–123.Google Scholar
Port, M., Clough, D. & Kappeler, P. M. (2009). Market effects offset the reciprocation of grooming in free-ranging redfronted lemurs, Eulemur fulvus rufus. Animal Behaviour, 77, 29–36.Google Scholar
Rilling, J. K., Scholz, J., Preuss, T. M., et al. (2012). Differences between chimpanzees and bonobos in neural systems supporting social cognition. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 7, 369–379.Google Scholar
Roeder, J. J., Fornasieri, I. & Gosset, D. (2002). Conflict and postconflict behaviour in two lemur species with different social organizations (Eulemur fulvus and Eulemur macaco): a study on captive groups. Aggressive Behavior, 28, 62–74.Google Scholar
Rolland, N. & Roeder, J. J. (2000). Do ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta) reconcile in the hour post-conflict?: a pilot study. Primates, 41, 223–227.Google Scholar
Romero, T., Castellanos, M. A. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2011). Post-conflict affiliation by chimpanzees with aggressors: other-oriented versus selfish political strategy. PLoSONE, 6(7), e22173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022173.Google Scholar
Roseth, C. J., Pellegrini, A. D., Bohn, C. M., Van Ryzin, M. & Vance, N. (2007). Preschoolers’ aggression, affiliation, and social dominance relationships: An observational, longitudinal study. Journal of School Psychology, 45, 479–497.Google Scholar
Roseth, C. J., Pellegrini, A. D., Dupuis, D. N., et al. (2008). Teacher intervention and U.S. preschoolers’ natural conflict resolution after aggressive competition. Behaviour, 145, 1601–1626.Google Scholar
Roseth, C. J., Pellegrini, A. D., Dupuis, D. N., et al. (2011). Preschoolers’ bistrategic resource control, reconciliation, and peer regard. Social Development, 1, 185–211.Google Scholar
Rubenstein, D. R. (2012). Family feuds: social competition and sexual conflict in complex societies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 2304–2313.Google Scholar
Rubin, J. Z., Pruitt, D. G. & Kim, S. H. (1994). Social Conflict: Escalation, Stalemate, and Settlement, McGraw-Hill, New York.
Sapolsky, R. M. (2006). Social cultures among nonhuman primates. Current Anthropology, 47, 641–656.Google Scholar
Sapolsky, R. M. (2013). Rousseau with a tail. Maintaining a tradition of peace among baboons. In: Fry, D. P. (ed.), War, Peace, and Human Nature. The Convergence of Evolutionary and Cultural Views.New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 421–438.
Schino, G. (1998). Reconciliation in domestic goats. Behaviour, 135, 343–356.Google Scholar
Schino, G. & Marini, C. (2012). Self-protective function of post-conflict bystander affiliation in mandrills. PLoS ONE, 7(6), e38936. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.-pone.0038936.Google Scholar
Schino, G., Geminiani, S., Rosati, L. & Aureli, F. (2004). Behavioral and emotional response of Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata) mothers after their offspring receive an aggression. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 340–346.Google Scholar
Scofield, B. & Margulis, L. (2012). Psychological discontent: Self and science on our symbiotic planet. In, Kahn, P. H., Jr. and Hasbach, P. H. (eds), Ecopsychology. Science, Totems, and the Technological Species. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press pp. 219–240.
Silk, J. B. (2002). The form and function of reconciliation in primates. Annual Reviews of Anthropology, 31, 21–44.Google Scholar
Soma, T. & Koyama, N. (2013). Eviction and troop reconstruction in a single matriline of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta): what happened when ‘grandmother’ died? In: Masters, J., Gamba, M., Génin, F. & Tuttle, R. (eds), Leaping Ahead: Advances in Prosimian Biology. New York: Springer, pp. 137–146.
Stiver, K. A., Dierkes, P., Taborsky, M., Gibbs, H. L. & Balshine, S. (2005). Relatedness and helping in fish: examining the theoretical predictions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272, 1593–1599.Google Scholar
Sustainable Development Solutions Network (2013). An action agenda for sustainable development. Report for the UN Secretary Geneneral.
Tacconi, G. & Palagi, E. (2009). Play behavioural tactics under space reduction: social challenges in bonobos, Pan paniscus. Animal Behaviour, 78, 469–476.Google Scholar
Takahata, Y, Koyama, N, Ichino, S, & Miyamoto, N (2005). Inter- and within-troop competition of female ring-tailed lemurs: a preliminary report. African Study Monographs, 26, 1–14.Google Scholar
Thierry, B. (1985a). Social development in three species of macaque (Macaca mulatta, M. fascicularis, M. tonkeana): A preliminary report on the first ten weeks of life. Behavioural Processes, 11, 89–95.Google Scholar
Thierry, B. (1985b). Patterns of agonistic interactions in three species of macaque (Macaca mulatta, Macaca fascicularis, Macaca tonkeana). Aggressive Behavior, 11, 223–233.Google Scholar
Thierry, B. (1986). A comparative study of aggression and response to aggression in three species of macaque. In: Else, J. G. & Lee, P. C. (eds), Primate Ontogeny, Cognition, and Social Behavior. Cambridge University Press, pp. 307–313.
Thierry, B. (1990). Feedback loop between kinship and dominance: the macaque model. Journal of theoretical Biology, 145(4), 511–522.Google Scholar
Thierry, B. (2000). Covariation and conflict management patterns across macaque species. In: Aureli, F. & Waal, F. B. M. de (eds), Natural conflict resolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press, pp. 106–128.
Thierry, B. (2007). Behaviourology divided: shall we continue?Behaviour, 144, 861–878.Google Scholar
Thierry, B. (2013). Identifying constraints in the evolution of primate societies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 368, 20120342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0342.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1968). On war and peace in animals and man. An ethologist's approach to the biology of aggression. Science, 160, 1411–1418.Google Scholar
Valdeón Baruque, J. (2002). Pedro I el Cruel y Enrique de Trastámara: ¿la primera guerra civil española?Barcelona: Aguilar, 2003.
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1967). The facial displays of catarrhine monkeys and apes. In Morris, D. (Ed.). Primate Ethology (pp. 7–68). New York: Aldine.
van Schaik, C. P. & Aureli, F. (2000). The natural history of valuable relationships in primates. In Aureli, F. and de Waal, F. B. M. (eds), Natural Conflict Resolution (pp. 307–333). Berkeley: University of California Press.
van Wolkenten, M. L., Davis, J. M., Gong, M. L. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Coping with acute crowding byCebus apella. International Journal of Primatology, 27, 1241–1256.Google Scholar
van Zweden, J. S., Cardoen, D. & Wenseleers, T. (2012). Social evolution: when promiscuity breeds cooperation. Current Biology, 22, R922–R924.Google Scholar
Verbeek, P. (2008). Peace ethology. Behaviour, 145, 1497–1524.Google Scholar
Verbeek, P. (2013). An ethological perspective on war and peace. In: Fry, D. P. (ed.), War, Peace, and Human Nature: The Convergence of Evolutionary and Cultural Views.New York: Oxford University Press.
Verbeek, P. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2001). Peacemaking among preschool children. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 7, 5–28.Google Scholar
Verbeek, P., Iwamoto, T. & Murakami, N. (2007). Differences in aggression among wild type and domesticated fighting fish are context dependent. Animal Behaviour, 73, 75–83.Google Scholar
Wahaj, S. A., Guse, K. R. & Holekamp, K. E. (2001). Reconciliation in spotted hyenaa (Crocuta crocuta). Ethology, 107, 1057–1074.Google Scholar
Watts, D. (1995). Post-conflict social events in wild mountain gorillas (Mammalia, Hominoidea). I. Social interactions between opponents. Ethology, 100, 139–157.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. (1996). Comparative socioecology of mountain gorillas. In: McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F. & Nishida, T. (eds), Great Ape Society: Cambridge University Press, pp. 16–28.
Watts, D, & Mitani, J. (2000). Infanticide and cannibalism by male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Primates, 41, 357–365.Google Scholar
Watts, D., Muller, M., Amsler, S., Mbabazi, G. & Mitani, J. (2006). Lethal intergroup aggression by chimpanzees in the Kibale National Park, Uganda. American Journal of Primatology, 68, 161–180.Google Scholar
Weaver, A. (2003). Conflict and reconciliation in captive bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. Marine Mammal Science, 19, 836–846.Google Scholar
Wenseleers, T., Bacon, J. P., Alves, D. A., et al. (2013). Bourgeois behavior and freeloading in the colonial orb web spider Parawixia bistriata (Araneae, Araneidae). The American Naturalist, 182, pp. 120–129.Google Scholar
White, T. D., Asfaw, B., Beyene, Y., et al. (2009). Ardipithecus ramidus and the paleobiology of early hominids. Science, 326(5949), 64–86.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (2000). Sociobiology: The New Synthesis.Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Wikberg, E., Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. L. (2007). Kin mediated reconciliation substitutes for direct reconciliation in female baboons. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274, 1109–1115.Google Scholar
Wright, P. C. (1999). Lemur traits and Madagascar ecology: coping with an island environment. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 110, 31–72.Google Scholar
Yanagi, A & Berman, C. M. (2014). Body signals during social play in free-ranging rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): a systematic analysis. American Journal of Primatology, 76, 168–179.Google Scholar
Zimen, E. (1981). The Wolf, a Species in Danger.Delacorte Press.
Zuckerman, S. (1932). The Social Life of Monkeys and Apes. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×