Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-07T02:04:32.682Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Psychopathy and the detection of concealed information

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Bruno Verschuere
Affiliation:
University of Amsterdam
Bruno Verschuere
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Amsterdam
Gershon Ben-Shakhar
Affiliation:
Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Ewout Meijer
Affiliation:
Universiteit Maastricht, Netherlands
Get access

Summary

Overview: The most common application of concealed information detection is crime knowledge assessment in crime suspects. The validity of this application has mainly been investigated in healthy subjects. Criminals may differ in important aspects from healthy subjects. Psychopathy, for example, is quite common among criminal populations. Psychopathy is characterized by affective–interpersonal (e.g., shallow affect) and behavioral–lifestyle (e.g., impulsivity) features. The latter is associated with physiological hyporesponsivity, and could threaten the validity of concealed information detection. I will review empirical research that has examined this possibility. Directions for future research will be discussed.

The most common application of concealed information detection is to assess whether a crime suspect has intimate knowledge about the crime under investigation. A concern with this application is that – particularly under real-life circumstances (Elaad et al., 1992) – guilty suspects may escape detection. Several factors might explain this modest sensitivity. Guilty suspects may not remember certain crime details. Another, less investigated, source of error is individual differences in responsivity. Despite perfect recognition, some individuals do not respond to concealed information. From the very beginning, this was recognized by David Lykken, who stated that college students are “hardly representative of the average run of criminal suspects” and that “perhaps a proportion of the latter would not respond ‘normally’ in such a test” (p. 387; Lykken, 1959). Lykken did not specify who these non-normal responding individuals might be. We now know that mental disorders are very common in the prison population.

Type
Chapter
Information
Memory Detection
Theory and Application of the Concealed Information Test
, pp. 215 - 230
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

,American Psychological Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 4th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Balloun, K. D., and Holmes, D. S. (1979). Effects of repeated examinations on the ability to detect guilt with a polygraph examination: a laboratory experiment with a real crime. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 316–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Shakhar, G. (1985). Standardization within individuals: a simple method to neutralize individual differences in skin conductance. Psychophysiology, 22, 292–299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Shakhar, G., and Elaad, E. (2003). The validity of psychophysiological detection of information with the Guilty Knowledge Test: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 131–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Shakhar, G., Lieblich, I., and Kugelmas, S. (1970). Guilty knowledge technique: application of signal detection measures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54, 409–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., and Iacono, W. G. (2005). Psychopathy, startle blink modulation, and electrodermal reactivity in twin men. Psychophysiology, 42, 753–762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, J. (1957). A study of affective responsiveness in a lie detection situation. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 55, 11–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cleckley, H. (1941/1976). The Mask of Sanity. St. Louis, MO: Mosby.Google Scholar
Cooke, D. J., and Michie, C. (2001). Refining the construct of psychopathy: towards a hierarchical model. Psychological Assessment, 13, 171–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dindo, L. (2008). Dual Pathways to Psychopathy: Relations with Skin Conductance Reactivity. University of Iowa, Iowa.Google Scholar
Dindo, L., and Fowles, D. C. (2008). The skin conductance orienting response to semantic stimuli: significance can be independent of arousal. Psychophysiology, 45, 111–118.Google ScholarPubMed
Elaad, E., Ginton, A., and Jungman, N. (1992). Detection measures in real-life criminal guilty knowledge tests. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 757–767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazel, S., and Danesh, J. (2002). Serious mental disorder in 23 000 prisoners: a systematic review of 62 surveys. Lancet, 359, 545–550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowles, D. C., and Dindo, L. (2006). A dual-deficit model of psychopathy. In Patrick, C. J. (ed.), Handbook of Psychopathy (pp. 14–34). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gough, H. G. (1956). California Psychological Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Gudjonsson, G. H. (1982). Some psychological determinants of electrodermal responses to deception. Personality and Individual Differences, 3, 381–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, R. D. (2003). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist – Revised. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., and Hare, R. D. (1988). Factor structure of the Psychopathy Checklist. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 741–747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hathaway, S. R., and McKinley, J. C. (1943). The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Herpertz, S. C., Mueller, I. R., Wenning, B., Qunabi, M., Lichterfeld, C., and Herpertz-Dahlman, B. (2003). Autonomic responses in boys with externalizing disorders. Journal of Neural Transmission, 110, 1181–1195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honts, C. R., and Amato, S. L. (2002). Countermeasures. In Kleiner, M. (ed.), Handbook of Polygraph Testing (pp. 251–264). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kosson, D. S., Steuerwald, B. L., Newman, J. P., and Widom, C. S. (1994). The relation between socialization and antisocial behavior, substance use, and family conflict in college students. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63, 473–488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., and Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 151–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieblich, I., Shakhar, G. B., and Kugelmass, S. (1976). Validity of guilty knowledge technique in a prisoners sample. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 89–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lilienfeld, S. O., and Andrews, B. P. (1996). Development and preliminary validation of a self-report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal populations. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 488–524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorber, M. F. (2004). Psychophysiology of aggression, psychopathy, and conduct problems: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 531–552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenz, A. R., and Newman, J. P. (2002). Do emotion and information processing deficiencies found in Caucasian psychopaths generalize to African-American psychopaths?Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1077–1086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lykken, D. T. (1957). A study of anxiety in the sociopathic personality. Journal of Abnormal and Clinical Psychology, 55, 6–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lykken, D. T. (1959). The GSR in the detection of guilt. Journal of Applied Psychology, 43, 385–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meijer, E. H. (2008). Psychophysiology and the Detection of Deception – Promises and Perils. Maastricht University, Maastricht.Google Scholar
Meijer, E. H., Smulders, F. T. Y., Johnston, J. E., and Merckelbach, H. L. G. J. (2007). Combining psychophysiology and forced choice in the detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology, 44, 814–822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, A. R., and Rosenfeld, J. P. (2004). Response-specific scalp distributions in deception detection and ERP correlates of psychopathic personality traits. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 13–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, A. B., and Lilienfeld, S. O. (2000). A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 113–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrick, C. J. (1994). Emotion and psychopathy: startling new insights. Psychophysiology, 31, 319–330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrick, C. J. (ed.) (2006). Handbook of Psychopathy. New York: Guilford Press.
Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., and Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the criminal psychopath: startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 82–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrick, C. J., Bernat, E. M., Malone, S. M., Iacono, W. G., Krueger, R. F., and McGue, M. (2006). P300 amplitude as an indicator of externalizing in adolescent males. Psychophysiology, 43, 84–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., Benishay, D., Lencz, T., and Scarpa, A. (1997). Abnormal orienting in schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23, 75–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tellegen, A. (1982). Brief manual of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ). University of Minnesota.
Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., Clercq, A., and Koster, E. H. W. (2005). Psychopathic traits and autonomic responding to concealed information in a prison sample. Psychophysiology, 42, 239–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., Clercq, A., and Koster, E. H. W. (2007). Antisociality, underarousal and the validity of the Concealed Information Test. Biological Psychology, 74, 309–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waid, W. M., Orne, M. T., and Wilson, S. K. (1979). Effects of level of socialization on electrodermal detection of deception. Psychophysiology, 16, 15–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×