Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T10:23:30.435Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Intelligence Research and Assessment in the United Kingdom

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Ian J. Deary
Affiliation:
University of Edinburgh
Pauline Smith
Affiliation:
NFER-Nelson, London
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

OVERVIEW

British contributions to research and practice in human intelligence are described and discussed. The emphasis on individual differences in humans' cognitive abilities and the search for the origins of human intelligence differences are British contributions. Some applications of intelligence testing are described in education, in the workplace, and in clinical settings. In theory and research, British contributors commune with those from other countries, especially the United States and, therefore, their contributions are not distinctly different. In the application of intelligence testing there is more United Kingdom-specific practice, with tests and procedures that are specific to the United Kingdom. There are differences in practice even within the United Kingdom's nations.

DISSEMINATING AND CRITICIZING RESEARCH ON INTELLIGENCE: U.K. CONTRIBUTIONS

There are several U.K. academic psychologists who have written books on the research surrounding psychometric intelligence-in-the-round. These include introductory books and higher-level monographs.

Among the entry-level, introductory books – intended for lay people, junior students, and other non-experts – there is a range of opinions. Some are critical appraisals of the field but are from researchers whose work is within the psychometric tradition (e.g., Cooper, 1999; Deary, 2001b; Kline, 1991). Included in this group is Brand's (1996) The g Factor, which was withdrawn by the publisher soon after publication despite positive critical evaluations (e.g., Mackintosh, 1996a). Other books at this level are highly critical of the concept of intelligence and the psychometric approach more generally.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, M. (1992). Intelligence and development: A cognitive theory. Oxford: Blackwell
Anderson, M., Reid, C., & Nelson, J. (2001). Developmental changes in inspection time: What a difference a year makes. Intelligence, 29, 475–486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon
Baddeley, A. (1992a). Working memory. Science, 255, 556–559CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1992b). Working memory: The interface between memory and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 281–288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barbaux, S., Plomin, R., & Whitehead, A. S. (2000). Polymorphisms of genes controlling homocysteine/folate metabolism and cognitive function. Neuroreport, 11, 1133–1136CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, P. T., & Eysenck, H. J. (1992a). Brain electrical potentials and intelligence. In A. Gale & H. J. Eysenck (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences: Biological perspectives. New York: Wiley
Barrett, P. T., & Eysenck, H. J. (1992b). Brain-evoked potentials and intelligence: The Hendrickson paradigm. Intelligence, 16, 361–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, P. T., & Eysenck, H. J. (1994). The relationship between evoked potential component amplitude, latency, contour length, variability, zero-crossings, and psychometric intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 3–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, P., Eysenck, H. J., & Lucking, S. (1986). Reaction time and intelligence: A replicated study. Intelligence, 10, 9–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Bartram, D. J., & Lindley, P. A. (1997). Review of ability & aptitude tests (level A). Leicester, UK: British Psychological Society
Bates, T. C., & Eysenck, H. J. (1993a). Intelligence, inspection time, and decision time. Intelligence, 17, 523–531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, G. K., Seashore, H. G., & Wesman, A. G. (1997). Differential ability tests for schools. London: The Psychological Corporation
Bickley, P. G., Keith, T. Z., & Wolfle, L. M. (1995). The three-stratum theory of cognitive abilities: Test of the structure of intelligence across the life span. Intelligence, 20, 309–328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binet, A. (1905). New methods for the diagnosis of the intellectual level of subnormals. L'Annee Psychologique, 12, 191–244. (Translated in 1916 by E. S. Kite in The development of intelligence in children. Vineland, NJ: Publications of the Training School at Vineland.)
Blinkhorn, S., & Johnson, C. (1996–2002). TheABLEseries. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press
Blinkhorn, S. F., & Hendrickson, D. E. (1982). Average evoked responses and psychometric intelligence. Nature, 195, 596–597CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouchard, T. J. (1998). Genetic and environmental influences on adult intelligence and special mental abilities. Human Biology, 70, 257–279Google ScholarPubMed
Brand, C. R. (1979). General intelligence and mental speed: Their relationship and development. In J. P. Das & N. O'Connor (Eds.), Intelligence and learning (pp. 589–593). New York: Plenum
Brand, C. R. (1987). A rejoinder to Pellegrino. In S. Modgil & C. Modgil (Eds.), Arthur Jensen: Consensus and controversy. Brighton, UK: Falmer
Brand, C. R. (1996). The g factor. Chichester, UK: Wiley. (This book was withdrawn by the publisher shortly after publication, making it difficult to obtain.)
Brand, C., & Deary, I. J. (1982). Intelligence and “inspection time.” In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 133–148). Berlin: Springer-VerlagCrossRef
Burt, C. (1909–1910). Experimental tests of general intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 3, 94–177Google Scholar
Burt, C. (1940). The factors of the mind. London: University of London Press
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of processing in the Raven's Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97, 404–431CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carretta, T. R., & Ree, M. J. (1995). Near identity of cognitive structure in sex and ethnic groups. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 149–155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor analytic studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Carroll, J. B. (1995). Reflections on Stephen Jay Gould's The mismeasure of man (1981): A retrospective review. Intelligence, 21, 121–134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caryl, P. G. (1994). Event-related potentials correlate with inspection time and intelligence. Intelligence, 18, 15–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caryl, P. G., Golding, S. J. J., & Hall, B. J. D. (1995). Interrelationships among auditory and visual cognitive tasks: An event-related potential (ERP) study. Intelligence, 21, 297–326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caryl, P. G., & Harper, A. (1996). Event related potentials (ERPs) in elementary cognitive tasks reflect task difficulty and task threshold. Intelligence, 22, 1–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, J. McK. (1886a). The inertia of eye and brain. Brain, 8, 295–381Google Scholar
Cattell, J. McK. (1886b). The time taken up by cerebral operations. Mind, 11, 220–242, 377–392, 524–538Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin
Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth and action. Amsterdam: North Holland
Cattell, R. B. (1998). Where is intelligence? Some answers from the triadic theory. In J. J. McArdle & R. W. Woodcock (Eds.), Human cognitive abilities in theory and practice. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Cooper, C. (1999). Intelligence and abilities. London: Routledge
Crawford, J. R., Deary, I. J., Allan, K. M., & Gustafsson, J.-E. (1998). Evaluating competing models of the relationship between inspection time and psychometric intelligence. Intelligence, 26, 27–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, J. R., Deary, I. J., Starr, J., & Whalley, L. J. (2001). The NART as an index of prior intellectual functioning: A retrospective validity study covering a 66 year interval. Psychological Medicine, 31, 451–458CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12, 671–684CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniels, J., McGuffin, P., Owen, M. J., & Plomin, R. (1998). Molecular genetic studies of cognitive ability. Human Biology, 70, 281–296Google ScholarPubMed
Deary, I. J. (1986). Inspection time: Discovery or rediscovery?Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 625–631CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J. (1993). Inspection time and WAIS-R IQ subtypes: A confirmatory factor analysis study. Intelligence, 17, 223–236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J. (1994a). Sensory discrimination and intelligence: Postmortem or resurrection?American Journal of Psychology, 107, 95–115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J. (1994b). Intelligence and auditory discrimination: Separating processing speed and fidelity of stimulus representation. Intelligence, 18, 189–213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J. (1995). Auditory inspection time and intelligence: What is the direction of causation?Developmental Psychology, 31, 237–250CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J. (1996). Reductionism and intelligence: The case of inspection time. Journal of Biosocial Science, 28, 405–423CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deary, I. J. (1997). Intelligence and information processing. In H. Nyborg (Ed.), The scientific study of human nature: Tribute to Hans Eysenck at eighty (pp. 282–310). Oxford: Elsevier
Deary, I. J. (1999). Intelligence and visual and auditory information processing. In P. L. Ackerman, P. C. Kyllonen, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), Learning and individual differences: Process, trait, and content determinants (pp. 111–133). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Deary, I. J. (2000a). Looking down on human intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Deary, I. J. (2000b). Simple information processing and intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 267–284). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Deary, I. J. (2001a). Individual differences in cognition: British contributions over a century. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 217–237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J. (2001b). Intelligence: A very short introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Deary, I. J., & Caryl, P. G. (1993). Intelligence, EEG, and evoked potentials (pp. 259–315). In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), Biological approaches to the study of human intelligence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Deary, I. J., Caryl, P. G., Egan, V., & Wight, D. (1989). Visual and auditory inspection times: Their interrelationship and correlations with IQ in high ability subjects. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 525–533CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J., Der, G., & Ford, G. (2001). Reaction times and intelligence differences: A population-based cohort study. Intelligence, 29, 389–399CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J., Egan, V., Gibson, G. J., Brand, C. R., Austin, E., & Kellaghan, T. (1996). Intelligence and the differentiation hypothesis. Intelligence, 23, 105–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J., Head, B., & Egan, V. (1989). Auditory inspection time, intelligence and pitch discrimination. Intelligence, 13, 135–147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J., Hunter, R., Langan, S. J., & Goodwin, G. M. (1991) Inspection time, psychometric intelligence and clinical estimates of cognitive ability in pre-senile Alzheimer's disease and Korsakoff's psychosis. Brain, 114, 2543–2554CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deary, I. J., McCrimmon, R. J., & Bradshaw, J. (1997). Visual information processing and intelligence. Intelligence, 24, 461–479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J., & Pagliari, C. (1991). The strength of g at different levels of ability: Have Detterman and Daniel rediscovered Spearman's “Law of Diminishing Returns.”Intelligence, 15, 247–250CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J., Simonotto, E., Marshall, A., Marshall, I., Goddard, N., & Wardlaw, J. M. (2001). The functional anatomy of inspection time: A pilot fMRI study. Intelligence, 29, 497–510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J., & Stough, C. (1996). Intelligence and inspection time: Achievements, prospects, and problems. American Psychologist, 51, 599–608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J., Whalley, L. J., Lemmon, H., Crawford, J. R., & Starr, J. M. (2000). The stability of individual differences in mental ability from childhood to old age: Follow-up of the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey. Intelligence, 28, 49–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devlin, B., Daniels, M., & Roeder, K. (1997). The heritability of IQ. Nature, 388, 468–471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dulewicz, V., & Higgs, M. (1999). Emotional intelligence questionnaire: General. Windsor, UK: ASE
Duncan, J., Emslie, H., & Williams, P. (1996). Intelligence and the frontal lobe: The organisation of goal-directed behavior. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 257–303CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, J., & Owen, A. M. (2000). Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 475–483CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, J., Seitz, J., Kolodny, J., Bor, D., Herzog, H., Ahmed, A., Newell, F. N., & Emslie, H. (2000). A neural basis for general intelligence. Science, 289, 457–460CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: AGS
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C. & Burley, J. (1997). British picture vocabulary scale (2nd ed.). Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson
Egan, V. (1994). Intelligence, inspection time, and cognitive strategies. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 305–316CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egan, V., Chiswick, A., Santosh, C., Naidu, K., Rimmington, J. E., & Best, J. J. K. (1994). Size isn't everything: A study of brain volume, intelligence, and auditory evoked potentials. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 357–367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egan, V., Wickett, J. C., & Vernon, P. A. (1995). Brain size and intelligence: Erratum, addendum, and correction. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 113–115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, C. D. (1975). British Intelligence Scale takes shape. Education, 25, 460–461Google Scholar
Elliott, C. D. (1990). Differential ability scales. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation
Elliott, C. D. (1997). The differential ability scales. In D. P. Flanagan, J. L. Genshaft, & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues. New York: Guilford Press
Elliott, C. D., Murray, D. J., & Pearson, L. S. (1979). British Ability Scales. Slough, UK: NFER
Elliott, C. D., with Smith, P., & McCulloch, K. (1996). British Ability Scales (2nd ed.). Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson
Elliott, C. D., with Smith, P., & McCulloch, K. (1997). British Ability Scales (2nd ed.). Technical Manual. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson
Embretson, S. E. (1995). The role of working memory capacity and general control processes in intelligence. Intelligence, 20, 169–189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ewing, F. M. E., Deary, I. J., McCrimmon, R. J., Strachan, M. W. J., & Frier, B. M. (1998). Effect of acute hypoglycemia on visual information processing in adults with Type 1 diabetes mellitus. Physiology and Behavior, 64, 653–660CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. J. (1939). Primary mental abilities. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 9, 270–275CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). Intelligence assessment: A theoretical and experimental approach. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 37, 81–97CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. J. (Ed.). (1982). A model for intelligence. Berlin: Springer
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Can we study intelligence using the experimental method?Intelligence, 20, 217–228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feltham, R. T., Baron, H., & Smith, P. (1994). Developing fair tests. The Psychologist, 7, 23–25Google Scholar
Feltham, R. T., & Smith, P. (1993). Psychometric test bias: How to avoid it. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 1, 117–122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernandes, C., & Strand, S. (2000). CATandKS3/GCSEindicators: Technical report. Windor, UK: NFER-Nelson
Flashman, L. A., Andreasen, N. C., Flaum, M., & Swayze, V. W. (1998). Intelligence and regional brain volumes in normal controls. Intelligence, 25, 149–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171–191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, G., & Morrisby, M. J. (1991). Morrisby profile. Hemel Hempstead, UK: EITS
Frearson, W., & Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Intelligence, reaction time (RT) and a new “odd-man-out” RT paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 807–817CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuller, T. (1648, reprinted 1936). Of natural fools. In R. Vallance (Ed.), A hundred english essays. London: Nelson
Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty. London: Dent
Galton, F. (1888). Head growth in students at the University of Cambridge. Nature, 38, 14–15Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books
Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence reframed: Multiple intelligences for the 21st century. New York: Basic Books
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence: Why it can matter more thanIQ. New York: Bantam
Goleman, D. (1998). Working with emotional intelligence. London: Bloomsbury
Gottesman, I. I. (1997). Twins: En route to QTLs for cognition. Science, 276, 1522–1523CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, S. J. (1981, 2nd ed. 1997). The mismeasure of man. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin
Gustafsson, J.-E. (1984). A unifying model for the structure of mental abilities. Intelligence, 8, 179–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gustafsson, J.-E. (1992). The relevance of factor analysis for the study of group differences. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 27, 239–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hearnshaw, L. S. (1979). Cyril Burt: Psychologist. London: Hodder and Stoughton
Heim, A. W. (1947). An attempt to test high grade intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 37, 70–80Google ScholarPubMed
Heim, A. W., Watts, K. P., & Simmonds, V. (1956 to 1983; Dates follow tests). AH1X/Y (1977), AH2/3 (1974, 1978), AH4 (1967, 1975), AH5 (1956, 1975), AH6AG &AH6SEM (1970, 1983). Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson
Hendrickson, A. E. (1982). The biological basis of intelligence. Part 1: Theory. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence. Berlin: Springer
Hendrickson, D. E. (1982). The biological basis of intelligence. Part II: Measurement. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence. Berlin: Springer
Hick, W. E. (1952). On the rate of gain of information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4, 11–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, L., Craig, I. W., McGuffin, P., Lubinski, D., Thompson, L. A., Owen, M. J., & Plomin, R. (2001). A genome-wide allelic association scan of 1847 DNA markers for general cognitive ability: A five-stage design using DNA pooling. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 105, 586Google Scholar
Hills, J. R. (1989). Screening for potentially biased items in testing programs. Educational Measurement: Issues and Practice, 8, 5–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R. (1979). Godel, Escher, Bach: An eternal golden braid. New York: Basic Books
Horn, J. L. (1985) Remodelling old models of intelligence. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence: Theories, measurements, and applications. New York: Wiley
Horn, J. L. (1988). Thinking about human abilities. In J. R. Nesselroade & R. B. Cattell (Eds.), Handbook of multivariate experimental psychology (2nd ed.). New York: PlenumCrossRef
Horn, J. L. (1998). A basis for research on age differences in cognitive capabilities. In J. J. McArdle & R. W. Woodcock (Eds.), Human cognitive abilities in theory and practice. London: Erlbaum
Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1966). Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallised general intelligences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 57, 253–270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howe, M. J. A. (1988). Intelligence as an explanation. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 349–360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howe, M. J. A. (1997). IQin question: The truth about intelligence. London: Sage
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1998). Innate talents: Reality or myth?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399–407CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huarte, J. de San Juan. (1969, originally published 1575, originally translated 1594). Examen de ingenios, or, A triall of wits (The examination of mens wits). Amsterdam: Da Capo Press, Theatrum Orbis Terrarum. (This facsimile edition from a copy held in the Bodleian Library, Oxford, is an English translation [by R. Carew] from an Italian translation [by M. Camilio Camiili] of the original Spanish. Published by Richard Watkins, London, 1594.)
Humphreys, L. G. (1979). The construct of general intelligence. Intelligence, 3, 105–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, E. (1980). Intelligence as an information processing concept. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 449–474CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, A. R. (1987). Individual differences in the Hick paradigm. In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), Speed of information processing and intelligence (pp. 101–175). Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Johnson, R. C., McClearn, G. E., Yuen, S., Nagoshi, C. T., Ahern, F. M., & Cole, R. E. (1985). Galton's data a century later. American Psychologist, 40, 875–892CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, P. (1991). Intelligence: The psychometric view. London: Routledge
Korten, A. E., Jorm, A. F., Jiao, Z., Letenneur, L., Jacomb, P. A., Henderson, A. S., Christensen, H., & Rogers, B. (1999). Health, cognitive, and psychosocial factors as predictors of mortality in an elderly community sample. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 53, 83–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kranzler, J. H., & Jensen, A. R. (1989). Inspection time and intelligence: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 13, 329–347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyllonen, P. C. (1996). Is working memory capacity Spearman's g? In I. Dennis & P. Tapsfield (Eds.), Human abilities: Their nature and measurement (pp. 77–96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working memory capacity?!Intelligence, 14, 389–433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, P. (1992). Inspection time and its relation to intelligence: Issues of measurement and meaning. Personality and Individual Differences 13, 987–1002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindley, P. A. (2000). Review of personality assessment instruments (level B) for use in occupational settings. Leicester: British Psychological Society
Lohman, D. F., Thorndike, R. L., & Hagen, E. P. (2001). Cognitive Abilities Test. Form 6. Chicago: Riverside
Lynn, R. (1982). IQ in Japan and the United States shows a growing disparity. Nature, 297, 222–223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynn, R. (1987). The intelligence of the Mongoloids: A psychometric, evolutionary, and neurological theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 813–844CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynn, R. (1990). The role of nutrition in secular increases in intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 273–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynn, R. (1991). Race differences in intelligence: A global perspective. Mankind Quarterly, 31, 255–296Google Scholar
Lynn, R. (1992). Does Spearman's g decline at high IQ levels? Some evidence from Scotland. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 153, 229–230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynn, R. (1994). Some reinterpretations of the Minnesota transracial adoption study. Intelligence, 19, 21–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynn, R. (1996a). Racial and ethnic differences in intelligence in the United States on the Differential Ability Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 271–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynn, R. (1996b). Dysgenics: Genetic deterioration in modern populations. Westport, CT: Praeger
Lynn, R. (1998). Sex differences in intelligence: Data from a Scottish standardization of the WAIS-R. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 289–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynn, R. (2001a). Eugenics. Westport, CT: Praeger
Lynn, R. (2001b). The science of human diversity: A history of the pioneer fund. Lanham, MD: University Press of America
Lynn, R., Irwing, P., & Cammock, T. (2002). Sex differences in general knowledge. Intelligence, 30, 27–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynn, R., & Mulhearn, G. (1991). A comparison of sex differences on the Scottish and American standardization samples of the WISC-R. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 1179–1182CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQand the wealth of nations. Westport, CT: Praeger
Mackintosh, N. J. (1981). A new measure of intelligence?Nature, 289, 529–530CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackintosh, N. J. (1995). Cyril Burt: Fraud or framed? Oxford: Oxford University Press
Mackintosh, N. J. (1996a). The g factor (book review). Nature, 381, 33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackintosh, N. J. (1996b). Sex differences and IQ. Journal of Biosocial Science, 28, 559–571CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackintosh, N. J. (1998). IQand human intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Mackintosh, N. J. (2000). Intelligence: Evolutionary psychology meets g. Nature, 403, 378–379CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLullich, A. M. J., Seckl, J. R., Starr, J. M., & Deary, I. J. (1998). The biology of intelligence: From association to mechanism. Intelligence, 26, 63–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshalek, B., Lohman, D. F., & Snow, R. E. (1983). The complexity continuum in the radex and hierarchical models of intelligence. Intelligence, 7, 107–127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathews, S. (1999). Testing people fairly: The best practice handbook. Windsor, UK: ASE
McClearn, G. E., Johansson, B., Berg, S., Pedersen, N. L., Ahern, F., Petrill, S. A., & Plomin, R. (1997). Substantial genetic influence on cognitive abilities in twins 80 or more years old. Science, 276, 1560–1563CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrimmon, R. J., Deary, I. J., & Frier, B. M. (1997). Auditory information processing during acute insulin-induced hypoglycaemia in non-diabetic human subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1547–1553CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrimmon, R. J., Deary, I. J., Huntly, B. J. P., MacLeod, K. J., Frier, B. M., (1996). Visual information processing during controlled hypoglycaemia in humans. Brain, 119, 1277–1287CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGeorge, P., Crawford, J., & Kelly, S. W. (1996). The relationship between WAIS-R abilities and speed of processing in a word identification task. Intelligence, 23, 175–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGuffin, P., & Martin, N. (1999). Behaviour and genes. British Medical Journal, 319, 37–40CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuffin, P., Riley, B., & Plomin, R. (2001). Genomics and behavior: Toward behavioral genomics. Science, 291, 1232CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., Halpern, D. F., Loehlin, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., & Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, H. E. (1982). National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test manual. Windsor, UK: NFER: Nelson
Nelson, H. E., & Willison, J. (1991). National Adult Reading Test (NART) (2nd ed.): Test manual. Windsor, UK: NFER: Nelson
Nettelbeck, T. (1987). Intelligence and inspection time. In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), Speed of information processing and intelligence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Nettelbeck, T., & Lally, M. (1976). Inspection time and measured intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 17–22CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettelbeck, T., Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1992). Age, intelligence, and speed. Intelligence, 16, 189–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nettelbeck, T., Rabbitt, P. M. A., Wilson, C., & Batt, R. (1996). Uncoupling learning from initial recall: The relationship between speed and memory deficits in old age. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 593–607CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neubauer, A. C. (1997). The mental speed approach to the assessment of intelligence. In J. Kingma & W. Tomic (Eds.), Advances in cognition and education: Reflections on the concept of intelligence. Greenwich, Connecticut: JAI
Novartis Foundation. (2000). The nature of intelligence. Chichester, UK: Wiley
O'Carroll, R. (1995). The assessment of premorbid ability: A critical review. Neurocase, 1, 83–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, D. M., Crawford, J. R., & Stephen, E. (1999). Auditory inspection time and intelligence: A new spatial localization task. Intelligence, 27, 131–139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, K. (1906–1907). On the relationship of intelligence to size and shape of head, and to other physical and mental characters. Biometrika, 5, 105–146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrie, R. X. A., & Deary, I. J. (1989). Smoking and human information processing. Psychopharmacology, 99, 393–396CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrill, S. A., Ball, D., Eley, T., Hill, L., Plomin, R., McClearn, G., Smith, D. L., Chorney, K., Chorney, K., Hershz, M. S., Detterman, D. K., Thompson, L. A., Benbow, C., Lubinski, D., Daniels, J., Owen, M. J., & McGuffin, P. (1998). Failure to replicate a QTL association between a DNA marker identified by EST00083 and IQ. Intelligence, 25, 179–184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrill, S. A., & Deary, I. J. (2001). Inspection time and intelligence: Celebrating 25 years of research. Intelligence, 29, 441–442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrill, S. A., Plomin, R., Berg, S., Johansson, B., Pedersen, N. L., Ahern, F., & McClearn, G. E. (1998). The genetic and environmental relationship between general and specific cognitive abilities in twins age 80 and older. Psychological Science, 9, 183–189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrill, S. A., Plomin, R., McClearn, G. E., Smith, D. L., Vignetti, S., Chorney, M. J., Thompson, L. A., Detterman, D. K., Benbow, C., Lubinski, D., Daniels, J., Owen, M. J., & McGuffin, P. (1996). DNA markers associated with general and specific cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 23, 191–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrill, S. A., Saudino, K. S., Wilkerson, B., & Plomin, R. (2001). Genetic and environmental molarity and modularity of cognitive functioning in 2-year-old twins. Intelligence, 29, 31–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R. (2000). A genome scan for QTLs associated with general cognitive ability (“g”). Behavior Genetics, 30, 414–415Google Scholar
Plomin, R. (2001a). The genetics of g in human and mouse. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 136–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R. (2001b). Genetics and behaviour. The Psychologist, 14, 134–139Google Scholar
Plomin, R. (2003). Genetics, genes, genomics, and g. Molecular Psychiatry, 8, 1–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., & Crabbe, J. (2000). DNA. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 806–828CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., & Craig, I. (2001). Genetics, environment and cognitive abilities: Review and work in progress towards a genome scan for quantitative trait locus associations using DNA pooling. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, S41–S48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., & McGuffin, P. (2001). Behavioral Genetics (4th ed.). New York: Worth
Plomin, R., McClearn, G. E., Smith, D. L., Skuder, P., Vignetti, S., Chorney, M. J., Kasarda, S., Thompson, L. A., Detterman, D. K., Petrill, S. A., Daniels, J., Owen, M., & McGuffin, P. (1995). Allelic associations between 100 DNA markers and high versus low IQ. Intelligence, 21, 31–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., & Petrill, S. (1997). Genetics and intelligence: What's new?Intelligence, 24, 53–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Mitchell, R. F. (1967). Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological Review, 74, 392–409CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, T. S., Eley, T. C., Dale, P. S., Stevenson, J., Saudino, K., & Plomin, R. (2000). Genetic and environmental covariation between verbal and nonverbal cognitive development in infancy. Child Development, 71, 948–959CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Psychological Corporation, The. (1997). Wechsler Scorer Version 2.1. The Psychological Corporation
Psychometric Research and Development Ltd (1985). Graduate and Managerial Assessment. Windsor, UK: ASE
Rabbitt, P., Donlan, C., Bent, N., McInnes, L., & Abson, V. (1993). The University of Manchester Age and Cognitive Performance Research Centre and North-east Age Research longitudinal programs 1982 to 1987. Zeitschrift fur Gerontologie, 26, 176–183Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1938). Progressive Matrices. London: Lewis
Raven, J. C. (1939). The RECI series of perceptual tests: An experimental survey. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 18, 16–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raz, N., Willerman, L., & Yama, M. (1987). On sense and senses: Intelligence and auditory information processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 201–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, K. (1999). The making of intelligence. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson
Richardson, K., & Webster, D. S. (1996). Analogical reasoning and the nature of context: A research note. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 23–32CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, M. J., & Stevenson, N. J. (1996). Reasoning with Raven: With and without help. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 519–532CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, E. (1964). Die geschwindigkeit der verabeitung von information and ihr zusammenhang mit intelligenz. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 11, 616–622Google Scholar
Rushton, J. P., & Ankney, C. D. (1996). Brain size and cognitive ability: Correlations with age, sex, social class, and race. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 21–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rust, J. (1995). Wechsler Individual Achievement Test™. London: The Psychological Corporation
Salovey, P., & Meyer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 9, 185–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scottish Council for Research in Education. (1933). The intelligence of Scottish children: A national survey of an age group. London: University of London Press
SHL (1982–1991). Critical reasoning test battery. Thames Ditton, Surrey: SHL
Shipley, B. A., Deary, I. J., Tan, J., Christie, G., & Starr, J. M. (2002). Efficiency of temporal order discrimination as an indicator of bradyphrenia in Parkinson's disease: The inspection time loop task. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1488–1493CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, B. (1953/1971). Intelligence testing and the comprehensive school. Republished in Intelligence, Psychology, and Education: A Marxist critique. London: Lawrence & Wishart
Simpson, C. R., & Deary, I. J. (1997). Strategy use and feedback in inspection time. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 787–797CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. (1986). Application of the information processing approach to the design of a non-verbal reasoning test. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 56, 119–137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P., & Whetton, C. (1992). Critical reasoning tests. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson
Smith, P., Whetton, C., & Caspall, L. (1999). General ability tests – 2. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson
Snowdon, D. A., Kemper, S. J., Mortimer, J. A., Greiner, L. H., Wekstein, D. R., & Markesbery, W. R. (1996). Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer's disease in late life: Findings from the Nun Study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 275, 528–532CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spearman, C. (1904). “General intelligence” objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201–293CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spearman, C. (1923). The nature of intelligence and the principles of cognition. London: Macmillan
Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. London: Macmillan
Stauffer, J. M., Ree, M. J., & Carretta, T. R. (1996). Cognitive-components tests are not much more than g: An extension of Kyllonen's analyses. Journal of General Psychology, 123, 193–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, information processing, and analogical reasoning: The componential analysis of human abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Sternberg, R. J. (1978). Intelligence research at the interface between differential and cognitive psychology. Intelligence, 2, 195–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). BeyondIQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Sternberg, S. (1966). High speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652–654CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stough, C., Mangan, G., Bates, T., Frank, N., Kerkin, B., & Pellett, O. (1995). Effects of nicotine on perceptual speed. Psychopharmacology, 119, 305–310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strachan, M. W. J., Deary, I. J., Ewing, F. M. E., Ferguson, S. S. C., Young, M. J., & Frier, B. M. (2001). Acute hypoglycaemia impairs functions of the central but not the peripheral nervous system. Physiology and Behavior, 72, 83–92CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strand, S. (1999). NFER-NELSONnon-verbal reasoning 8/9 andKS2 chances tables: Autumn 1999. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson
Strand, S. (2001). CATandKS2 indicators: Technical report. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson
Thomson, G. H. (1939). The factorial analysis of human ability. London: University of London Press
Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Lorge, I. (1954–1993). Cognitive abilities test. Forms 1 to 5. Chicago: Riverside
Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Psychometric Monographs, No. 1
Turic, D., Fisher, P. J., Plomin, R., & Owen, M. J. (2001). No association between apolipoprotein E polymorphisms and general cognitive ability in children. Neuroscience Letters, 299, 97–100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, M. (1997). Psychological assessment of dyslexia. London: Whurr
Undheim, J. O. (1981a). On intelligence II: A neo-Spearmanian model to replace Cattell's theory of fluid and crystallised intelligence. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 22, 181–187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Undheim, J. O. (1981b). On intelligence IV: Toward a restoration of general intelligence. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 22, 251–265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vernon, P. A., (Ed.). (1987). Speed of information processing and intelligence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Vernon, P. A., (Ed.). (1993). Biological approaches to the study of human intelligence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Vernon, P. E. (1950). The structure of human abilities. London: Methuen
Vernon, P. E., (1961). The structure of human abilities (2nd ed.). London: Methuen
Vernon, P. E., and Parry, J. B. (1949). Personnel selection in the armed forces. London: University of London Press
Vickers, D., Nettelbeck, T., & Willson, R. J. (1972). Perceptual indices of performance: The measurement of “inspection time” and “noise” in the visual system. Perception, 1, 263–295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wahlsten, D. (1999). Single-gene influences on behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 599–624CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1989). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence (rev.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation
Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.)UK. London: Psychological Corporation
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation
Westby, G. (1953). Review of the Raven Progressive Matrices. In O. K. Buros (Ed.), The fourth mental measurements yearbook. Highland Park, NJ: Gryphon
Whalley, L. J., & Deary, I. J. (2001). Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and survival up to age 76. British Medical Journal, 322, 1–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Y., Caryl, P., & Deary, I. J. (1989a). Evoked potential correlates of inspection time. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 379–384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Y., Caryl, P., & Deary, I. J. (1989b). Evoked potentials, inspection time, and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 1079–1094CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×