Published online by Cambridge University Press: 02 February 2010
University of California, Berkeley campus, early 1940s. A man and a young woman are strolling across the university campus. Suddenly the man stops and says, “Now.” The woman also stops, raises her arm, points to the building in front of her, and says, “The window on the second floor.” After contemplating the scene for a moment, she says “Three-and-a-half feet.” The man records this and other numbers. They begin walking again. A few moments later, the man stops once again and the same interaction repeats itself.
University of Constance, vision laboratory, mid-1980s. A young man enters a dark room. The experimenter welcomes him and then asks him to sit down in front of a large cubicle, to put his chin on a rest (thus making it impossible to move his head), and to look into the cubicle through two small holes. Its interior is completely dark. Suddenly a white square appears, only to disappear a few seconds later. Then, a tone is heard, which is followed by another white square to the right of the previous one. The young man says, “The left one.” This episode frequently repeats itself over the course of the next half hour.
Although both scenes represent the same genre, namely, psychological studies on the perception of object size, they share few common features. As we shall see shortly, their differences can be traced back to the fact that the Berkeley study was conceived by Egon Brunswik (1944; for a replication, see Dukes, 1951), whereas a contemporary experimental psychologist conducted the Constance study.
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.