Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T16:20:47.822Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - The landscape of intelligence

from Part II - Transcending anthropocentrism: How do we move beyond our own preconceptions of life, intelligence, and culture?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 November 2015

Lori Marino
Affiliation:
Emory University
Steven J. Dick
Affiliation:
Library of Congress, Washington DC
Get access

Summary

Introduction: astrobiology and intelligence

The question of how intelligence evolves on different planets is a subject of fervent interest in many scientific and public domains. Yet, it has received little, if any, serious scientific attention in astrobiology. Astrobiology relies on an elegant paradigm: Earth as a natural laboratory. It seeks to investigate how life arose and evolved on this planet and to apply that knowledge to detecting and understanding extraterrestrial life. Thus, the study of the evolution of intelligence fits squarely within the field of astrobiology. But, despite a wealth of accessible data from “mainstream” fields, astrobiology has limited itself to studying the origin and evolution of early life and has not made the connection between these basic processes and intelligence. Why, in its 50-year history, has there been essentially no empirical work within astrobiology on intelligence?

What is intelligence?

Intelligence is, by nature, a fuzzy concept. That is, there are no strict boundaries on it and there is no scientific consensus on its definition (Sternberg 2000). The study of intelligence, therefore, necessitates a strong reliance on “bottom-up” empirical descriptions of a range of phenomena rather than a “top-down” hunt for a precise exemplar. Intelligence is not a binary trait. Rather, it is a multidimensional phenomenon which expresses itself in varying phenotypes and levels of complexity and is interconnected with the entire psychological make-up of any animal. Nevertheless, if we wish to use a working definition of intelligence, then we can refer to intelligence as a level of cognitive complexity, i.e. how an individual acquires, processes, stores, analyzes, and acts upon information and circumstances.

Despite its complexities and fluid boundaries, the phenomenon of intelligence is amenable to empirical scientific investigation just as any other biological property. The absence of the study of intelligence from astrobiology is due to a complex set of historical and psychological roadblocks. One of these may be the mistaken assumption that intelligence is not scientifically tractable. But foremost of these is our species’ adherence to the wrong model of life on Earth, one that promotes misconceptions that impede the way forward in the scientific study of intelligence in astrobiology.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allman, J. M. 1999. Evolving Brains. New York, NY: Scientific American Library.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. and Gallup, G. G. Jr. (2011). “Which primates recognize themselves in mirrors?PLoS Biology 9(3): e1001024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arendt, D., Deans, A., Jekely, G., and Tessmar-Raible, K. 2008. “The evolution of nervous system centralization.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 363: 1523–1528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benard, J., Stach, S., and Giurfa, M. 2006. “Categorization of visual stimuli in the honey bee, Apis mellifera.” Animal Cognition 9: 257–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beran, M. J. 2006. “Quantity perception by adult humans (Homo sapiens), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) as a function of stimulus organization.” International Journal of Comparative Psychology 19: 386–397.Google Scholar
Beran, M. J. and Rumbaugh, D. M. 2001. “Constructive enumeration by chimpanzees on a computerized task.” Animal Cognition 4: 81–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biro, D. and Matsuzawa, T. 2001. “Use of numerical symbols by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): cardinals, ordinals and the introduction of zero.” Animal Cognition 4: 193–199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bitterman, M. E. 1996. “Comparative analysis of learning in honey bees.” Learning and Behavior 24: 123–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. 2012. Wild Cultures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boysen, S. T. and Berntson, G. G. 1989. “Numerical competence in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes).” Journal of Comparative Psychology 103: 23–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boysen, S. T., Berntson, G. G., Shreyer, T. A., and Quigley, K. S. 1993. “Processing of ordinality and transitivity by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).” Journal of Comparative Psychology 107: 208–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breuer, T., Ndoundou-Hockemba, M., and Fishlovk, V. 2005. “First observation of tool use in wild gorillas.” PLoS Biology 3(11): e380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, C. 2014. “Fish intelligence, sentience and ethics.” Animal Cognition, doi 10.1007/s10071-014-0761-0.
Clayton, N. S., Dally, J., and Emery, N. 2007. “Social cognition by food-caching corvids. The Western scrub-jay as a natural psychologist.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 362: 507–522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Couzin, I. D., Krause, J., James, R., Ruxton, G. D., and Franks, N. 2002. “Collective memory and spatial sorting in animal groups.” Journal of Theoretical Biology 218: 1–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowe, M. J. 2008. The Extraterrestrial Life Debate: Antiquity to 1900: A Source Book. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.Google Scholar
Dacke, M. and Srinivasan, M. 2008. “Evidence for counting in insects.” Animal Cognition 4: 683–689.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. 1871. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex (edn.). London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. 1859. On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life (edn.). London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Finn, J., Tregenza, T., and Norman, M. D. 2009. “Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus.” Current Biology 19: R1069–R1070.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiorito, G. and Scotto, P. 1992. “Observational learning in Octopus vulgaris.” Science 256, 545–546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friesen, W. O. and Kristan, W. B. 2008. “Leech locomotion: swimming, crawling, and decisions.” Current Opinion in Neurobiology 17: 704–711.Google Scholar
Garland, A. and Low, J. 2014. “Addition and subtraction in wild New Zealand robins.” Behavioural Processes. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635714001909
Giurfa, M., Zhang, S., Jenett, A., Menzel, R., and Mandyam, V. S. 2001. “The concepts of ʻsamenessʼ and ʻdifferenceʼ in an insect.” Nature 410: 930–933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giurfa, M., Reisenman, S. M., Gerber, B., and Lachnit, H. 2003. “The effect of cumulative experience on the use of elemental and configural visual discrimination strategies in honey bees.” Behavioral Brain Research 145: 161–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenspan, R. J. and van Swinderen, B. 2004. “Cognitive consonance: complex brain functions in the fruit fly and its relatives.” Trends in Neurosciences 27: 707–711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Held, S., Baumgartner, J., Kilbride, A., Byrne, R. W., and Mendl, M. 2005. “Foraging behaviour in domestic pigs (Sus scrofa): remembering and prioritizing food sites of different value.” Animal Cognition, 8: 114–121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Held, S., Mendl, M., Devereux, C., and Byrne, R. W. 2000. “Social tactics of pigs in a competitive foraging task: the ‘informed forager’ paradigm.” Animal Behaviour 59, 569–576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herman, L. M. 2012. “Body and self in dolphins.” Consciousness and Cognition 21: 526–545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herman, L. M. and Forestell, P. H. 1985. “Reporting presence or absence of named objects by a language-trained dolphin.” Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews 9: 667–691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herman, L. M., Pack, A. A., and Morrel-Samuels, P. 1993. “Representational and conceptual skills of dolphins.” In Roitblatt, H. R.et al. (eds.) Language and Communication: Comparative Perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 273–298.Google Scholar
Hochner, B. 2006. “The octopus: a model for a comparative analysis of the evolution of learning and memory mechanisms.” Biological Bulletin 210: 308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, G. R. and Gray, R. D. 2003. “Diversification and cumulative evolution in New Caledonian crow tool manufacture.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 270: 867–874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inoue, S. and Matsuzawa, T. 2007. “Working memory of numerals in chimpanzees.” Current Biology 17: R1004–R1005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inoue, S. and Matsuzawa, T. 2009. “Acquisition and memory of sequence order in young and adult chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).” Animal Cognition 12: S58–S69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaakkola, K., Fellner, W., Erb, L., Rodriguez, M., and Guarino, E. 2005. “Understanding of the concept of numerically ‘less’ by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus).” Journal of Comparative Psychology 119: 296–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaminski, J. P., Pitsch, A., and Tomasello, M. 2013. “Dogs steal in the dark.” Animal Cognition 16: 385–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kandel, E. R. 2001. “The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue between genes and synapses.” Science 294: 1030–1038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kandel, E. R. 2009. “The biology of memory: a forty year perspective.” The Journal of Neuroscience 29: 12748–12756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kilian, A., Yaman, S., von Fersen, L., and Gunturkun, O. 2003. “A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity.” Learning and Behaviour 31: 133–142.Google ScholarPubMed
Krutzen, M., Mann, J., Heithaus, M. R., et al. 2005. “Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102: 8939–8943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langridge, E. A., Sendova-Franks, A. B., and Franks, N. R. 2008. “How experienced individuals contribute to an improvement in collective performance in ants.” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62: 447–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leadbeater, E. and Chittka, L. 2009. “Bumble bees learn the value of social cues through experience.” Biology Letters 5: 310–312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyn, H., Greenfield, P. M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Gillespie-Lynch, K., and Hopkins, W. D. 2011. “Nonhuman primates do declare! A comparison of declarative symbol and gesture use in two children, two bonobos, and a chimpanzee.” Language and Communication 31: 63–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marino, L. 2002. “Convergence in complex cognitive abilities in cetaceans and primates.” Brain, Behavior and Evolution 59: 21–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marino, L. 2007. “Scala naturae.” In Bekoff, M. (eds.) The Encyclopedia of Human-Animal Relationships. Westport, CT: Greenwood Publishing Group, pp. 220–224.Google Scholar
McCowan, B., Marino, L.Vance, E., Walke, L., and Reiss, D. 2000. “Bubble ring play of bottlenose dolphins: implications for cognition.” Journal of Comparative Psychology 114: 98–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, W. C. 2011. “Pan symbolicus. A cultural primatologist's viewpoint.” In Henshilwood, C. S. and d’ Errico, F. (eds.) Homo Symbolicus: The Dawn of Language, Imagination and Spirituality. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 1–12.Google Scholar
Melis, A. P., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. 2006. “Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) conceal visual and auditory information from others.” Journal of Comparative Psychology 120: 154–162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messenger, J. B. 1996. “Neurotransmitters of cephalopods.” Invertebrate Neuroscience 2: 95–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morse, D. H. 2000. “The effect of experience on the hunting success of newly emerged spiderlings.” Animal Behavior 60: 827–835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulcahy, N. and Call, J. 2006. “How great apes perform on a modified trap tube task.” Animal Cognition 9:193–199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nawroth, C., Ebersbach, M., and von Borell, E. 2014. “Juvenile domestic pigs (Sus scrofa domesticus) use human-given cues in an object choice task.” Animal Cognition 17: 701–713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osvath, M. and Osvath, H. 2008. “Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool-use.” Animal Cognition 11: 661–674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pack, A. A. and Herman, L. M. 1995. “Sensory integration in the bottlenose dolphin: immediate recognition of complex shapes across the senses of echolocation and vision.” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 98: 722–733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Passino, K. M. 2010. “Honey bee swarm cognition: decision making performance and adaptation.” International Journal of Swarm Intelligence 1(2): 80–97.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M., 2006. “Ordinality and inferential abilities of a grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus).” Journal of Comparative Psychology 120: 205–216.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. 2009. The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Plotnik, J. M. and de Waal, F. B. M. 2014. “Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) reassure others in distress.” PeerJ 2: e278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plotnik, J. M., de Waal, F. B. M., and Reiss, D. 2006. “Self-recognition in an Asian elephant.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103: 17052–17057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prior, H., Schwarz, A., and Gunturkun, O. 2008. “Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): evidence of self-recognition.” PLoS Biology 6(8): e202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rankin, C. H. (2004) “Invertebrate learning: what can't a worm learn?Current Biology 14: R617–R618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reiss, D. and Marino, L. 2001. “Self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98: 5937–5942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rendell, L. and Whitehead, H. 2001. “Culture in whales and dolphins.” Behavioural and Brain Sciences 24: 309–324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumbaugh, D. M., Beran, M. J., and Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. 2003. “Language.” in Maestripieri, D. (ed.) Primate Psychology, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 395–423.Google Scholar
Santos, L. R., Barnes, J. L., and Mahajan, N. 2005. “Expectations about numerical events in four lemur species (Eulemur fulvus, Eulemur mongoz, Lemur catta and Varecia rubra).” Animal Cognition 8: 253–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherwood, C. C., Stimpson, C. D., Butti, C., et al. 2006. “Evolution of increased glia–neuron ratios in the human frontal cortex.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 103: 13606–13611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shulgina, G. I. 2006. “Learning of inhibition of behavior in the sea star, Asterias rubens.” Comparative and Ontogenic Physiology 42:161–165.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D. and Washburn, D. A. 2005. “Uncertainty monitoring and metacognition by animals.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 14: 19–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. 2000. Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Striedter, G. F. 2005. Principles of Brain Evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Taylor, A., Hunt, G., Holzaider, J. C., and Gray, R. D. 2007. “Spontaneous metatool use by New Caledonian crows.” Current Biology 17: 1504–1507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomonaga, M., and Matsuzawa, T. (2000). “Sequential responding to Arabic numerals with wild cards by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes).” Animal Cognition 3: 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turlejski, K. 1996. “Evolutionary ancient roles of serotonin: long-lasting regulation of activity and development.” Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis 56: 619–636.Google ScholarPubMed
West, R. E. and Young, R. J. 2002. “Do domestic dogs show any evidence of being able to count?Animal Cognition 5: 183–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodruff, G. and Premack, D. 1981. “Primitive mathematical concepts in the chimpanzee: proportionality and numerosity.” Nature 293: 568–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zilles, K. 1989. “Gyrification in the cerebral cortex of primates.” Brain, Behavior, and Evolution 34: 143–150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×