Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T09:46:17.808Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

40 - Self-Control Interventions

from Part III - Behavior Change Interventions: Practical Guides to Behavior Change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 July 2020

Martin S. Hagger
Affiliation:
University of California, Merced
Linda D. Cameron
Affiliation:
University of California, Merced
Kyra Hamilton
Affiliation:
Griffith University
Nelli Hankonen
Affiliation:
University of Helsinki
Taru Lintunen
Affiliation:
University of Jyväskylä
Get access

Summary

Research has shown self-control to be an important factor in determining behavior and outcomes in multiple contexts (e.g., health, education, workplace, interpersonal relationships). Self-control requires effortful pursuit of distal goals in favor of more immediately rewarding, proximal goals, particularly when those goals conflict. Individuals with good self-control exhibit well-developed self-regulatory skills that help them manage these conflicts or avoid them altogether. Over time, such skills enable strategic automization of behaviors in service of distal goals. Although self-control is often viewed as “trait-like”, research has suggested that self-control can be incrementally improved. A prominent means to improve self-control is through self-control or inhibitory control training, which involves repeated engagement in tasks that require inhibition of prepotent responses. Repetition of behaviors to develop habits and training individuals on inhibitory control tasks have been shown to be effective in improving self-control. There is a need for more high-quality studies using ecologically valid behavioral measures and long-term follow-up to provide more robust evidence on self-control training interventions. Preliminary guidelines for self-control interventions suggest that practicing self-control for a specified period of time in a particular domain or self-control will improve self-control in other domains. However, research needs to develop protocols involving meaningful, engaging training tasks that are acceptable in “real-world” contexts.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adriaanse, M. A., Kroese, F. M., Gillebaart, M., & De Ridder, D. T. D. (2014). Effortless inhibition: Habit mediates the relation between self-control and unhealthy snack consumption. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 444. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00444Google Scholar
Allom, V., Mullan, B., & Hagger, M. (2016). Does inhibitory control training improve health behaviour? A meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review, 10, 168186. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2015.1051078Google Scholar
Ayduk, O., Mendoza-Denton, R., Mischel, W., Downey, G., Peake, P. K., & Rodriguez, M. (2000). Regulating the interpersonal self: Strategic self-regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 776792. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.776Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2014). Self-regulation, ego depletion, and inhibition. Neuropsychologia, 65, 313319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.012Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 12521265. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.74.5.1252Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. & Vohs, K. D. (2016). Strength model of self-regulation as limited resource: Assessment, controversies, update. In Zanna, M. & Olson, J. (Eds.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 54 (pp. 67127). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 351355. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.xGoogle Scholar
Beames, J. R., Schofield, T. P., & Denson, T. F. (2018). A meta-analysis of improving self-control with practice. In de Ridder, D. T. D., Adriaanse, M. A., & Fujita, K. (Eds.), Routledge International Handbook of Self-Control in Health and Well-Being (pp. 405417). Abingdon: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315648576-32Google Scholar
Berkman, E. T. (2018). Self-regulation training. In Vohs, K. D. & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory and Applications (3rd ed., pp. 440457). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cheung, T. T., Gillebaart, M., Kroese, F. M., & De Ridder, D. T. D. (2014). Why are people with high self-control happier? The effect of trait self-control on happiness as mediated by regulatory focus. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 722. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00722Google Scholar
De Houwer, J. (2011). Why the cognitive approach in psychology would profit from a functional approach and vice versa. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 202209. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611400238Google Scholar
De Ridder, D. T. D., Kroese, F. M., & Gillebaart, M. (2018). Whatever happened to self-control? A proposal for integrating notions from trait self-control studies into state self-control research. Motivation Science, 4, 3949. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000062Google Scholar
De Ridder, D. T. D., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 7699. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311418749Google Scholar
De Ridder, D. T. D., Van der Weiden, A., Gillebaart, M., Benjamins, J. S., & Ybema, J. F. (2019). Just do it: Engaging in self-control on a daily basis improves the capacity for self-control. Manuscript under review.Google Scholar
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333, 959964. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204529CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donders, F. C. (1969). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412431. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(69)90065–1Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., & Gross, J. J. (2016). Situational strategies for self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 3555. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615623247Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Matthews, M. D., & Kelly, D. R. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 10871101. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1087Google Scholar
Ent, M. R., Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (2015). Trait self-control and the avoidance of temptation. Personality and Individual Differences, 74, 1215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.031Google Scholar
Finkel, E. J., DeWall, C. N., Slotter, E. B., Oaten, M., & Foshee, V. A. (2009). Self-regulatory failure and intimate partner violence perpetration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 483499. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015433Google Scholar
Friese, M., Frankenbach, J., Job, V., & Loschelder, D. D. (2017). Does self-control training improve self-control? A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 10771099. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617697076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friese, M., Hofmann, W., & Wiers, R. W. (2011). On taming horses and strengthening riders: Recent developments in research on interventions to improve self-control in health behaviors. Self and Identity, 10, 336351. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2010.536417Google Scholar
Friese, M., Loschelder, D. D., Gieseler, K., Frankenbach, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2019). Is ego depletion real? An analysis of arguments. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23, 107131. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868318762183Google Scholar
Fujita, K. (2011). On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 352366. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311411165Google Scholar
Galla, B. M., & Duckworth, A. L. (2015). More than resisting temptation: Beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 508525. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000026Google Scholar
Gieseler, K., Loschelder, D. D., & Friese, M. (2019). What makes for a good theory? How to evaluate a theory using the Strength Model of Self-Control as an example. In Sassenberg, K. & Vliek, M. (Eds.), Social Psychology in Action: Evidence-Based Interventions from Theory to Practice (pp. 322). Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
Gillebaart, M., & Adriaanse, M.A. (2017). Self-control predicts exercise behavior by force of habit, a conceptual replication of Adriaanse et al. (2014). Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 190. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00190Google Scholar
Gillebaart, M., Benjamins, J. S. Van der Weiden, A., Ybema, J. F., & De Ridder, D. T. D. (2019). Practice makes perfect: Repeatedly dealing with response conflicts facilitates its identification and speed of resolution. Manuscript under review.Google Scholar
Gillebaart, M., & De Ridder, D. T. D. (2015). Effortless self‐control: A novel perspective on response conflict strategies in trait self‐control. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9, 8899. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Förster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 13181335. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026545CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 162176. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01116.xGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Luhmann, M., Fisher, R. R., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2014). Yes, but are they happy? Effects of trait self‐control on affective well‐being and life satisfaction. Journal of Personality, 82, 265277. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12050Google Scholar
Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 174180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006Google Scholar
Hofmann, W., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). What people desire, feel conflicted about, and try to resist in everyday life. Psychological Science, 23, 582588. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612437426Google Scholar
Houben, K., Nederkoorn, C., Wiers, R. W., & Jansen, A. (2011). Resisting temptation: Decreasing alcohol-related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition. Drug and Alcohol Dependency, 116, 132136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.12.011Google Scholar
Inzlicht, M., & Friese, M. (2019). The past, present, and future of ego depletion. Social Psychology, 50, 370378. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000398Google Scholar
Inzlicht, M., Legault, L., & Teper, R. (2014). Exploring the mechanisms of self-control improvement. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 302307. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414534256Google Scholar
Job, V., Friese, M., & Bernecker, K. (2015). Effects of practicing self-control on academic performance. Motivation Science, 1, 219232. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000024Google Scholar
Jones, A., & Field, M. (2013). The effects of cue-specific inhibition training on alcohol consumption in heavy social drinkers. Experimental Clinical Psychopharmacology, 21, 816. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030683Google Scholar
Jones, A., Di Lemma, L. C., Robinson, E. et al. (2016). Inhibitory control training for appetitive behavior change: A meta-analytic investigation of mechanisms of action and moderators of effectiveness. Appetite, 97, 1628. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.11.013Google Scholar
Jones, A., McGrath, E., Robinson, E., Houben, K., Nederkoorn, C., & Field, M. (2018). A randomized controlled trial of inhibitory control training for the reduction of alcohol consumption in problem drinkers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86, 9911004. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000312Google Scholar
Lappin, J. S., & Eriksen, C. W. (1966). Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 805811. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0021266Google Scholar
Lawrence, N. S., Verbruggen, F., Morrison, S., Adams, R. C., & Chambers, C. D. (2015). Stopping to food can reduce intake: Effects of stimulus-specificity and individual differences in dietary restraint. Appetite, 85, 91103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.11.006Google Scholar
Miles, E., Sheeran, P., Baird, H., Macdonald, I., Webb, T. L., & Harris, P. R. (2016). Does self-control improve with practice? Evidence from a six-week training program. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 10751091. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000185Google Scholar
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. I. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933938. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2658056Google Scholar
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D. et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 26932698. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010076108Google Scholar
Muraven, M., Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1999). Longitudinal improvement of self-regulation through practice: Building self-control strength through repeated exercise. The Journal of Social Psychology, 139, 446457. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224549909598404Google Scholar
Prinsen, S., Evers, C., Wijngaards, L., Van Vliet, R., & De Ridder, D. T. D. (2018). Does self-licensing benefit self-regulation over time? An ecological momentary assessment study of food temptations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 914927. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218754509Google Scholar
Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F., & Stokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred years of social psychology quantitatively described. Review of General Psychology, 7, 331363. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.7.4.331Google Scholar
Savani, K., & Job, V. (2017). Reverse ego-depletion: Acts of self-control can improve subsequent performance in Indian cultural contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 589607. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000099Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72, 271324. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.xGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y., & Fishbach, A. (2000). Counteractive self-control in overcoming temptation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 493506. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.79.4.493CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veling, H., Aarts, H., & Papies, E. K. (2011). Using stop signals to inhibit chronic dieters’ responses toward palatable foods. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 771780. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.08.005Google Scholar
Veling, H., Lawrence, N. S., Chen, Z., Van Koningsbruggen, G. M., & Holland, R. W. (2017). What is trained during food go/no-go training? A review focusing on mechanisms and a research agenda. Current Addiction Reports, 4, 3541. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-017-0131-5Google Scholar
Verplanken, B., & Orbell, S. (2003). Reflections on past behavior: A self‐report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 13131330. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.xGoogle Scholar
Wang, J., Raoa, Y., & Houserb, D. E. (2017). An experimental analysis of acquired impulse control among adult humans intolerant to alcohol. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 12991304. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1610902114Google Scholar
Watts, T. W., Duncan, G. J., & Quan, H. (2018). Revisiting the Marshmallow Test: A conceptual replication investigating links between early gratification delay and later outcomes. Psychological Science, 29, 11591177. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618761661Google Scholar
Wiers, R. W., Rinck, M., Kordts, R., Houben, K., & Strack, F. (2010). Retraining automatic action‐tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers. Addiction, 105, 279287. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02775.xGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×