Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T02:12:33.886Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 6 - Socio-affective inferential mechanisms involved in emotion recognition

from Part III - On the mechanics of affective social learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 August 2019

Daniel Dukes
Affiliation:
Université de Fribourg, Switzerland
Fabrice Clément
Affiliation:
Université de Neuchâtel, Switzerland
Get access

Summary

Among social stimuli, the human face is unique. Its properties and features, both static and dynamic, reveal different aspects of a person. Perceivers use this information when they aim, for example, to recognize individuals, identify their social groups, discern their personality, detect their focus of attention, understand their verbal and non-verbal behaviours, and infer their emotions. Emotions that we read from facial actions are crucial to social interaction: They provide information that helps us understand social intentions, how to react to others, and to evaluate the affective meaning of many events that we encounter in our environment. In this chapter, we focus on social appraisal – one of the central mechanisms involved in affective social learning. In particular, we discuss the notion that, through a particular socio-affective inferential mechanism, social appraisal plays a significant role when the emotional expression of person A is used to learn about the value of the emotion expressed by person B. The first section of this chapter provides a brief historical introduction to the relation between contextual information and emotion recognition in faces. Then, we focus on the construct of social appraisal and its manifestation in socio-affective inferential mechanisms involved in emotion recognition. In the third section, we present empirical evidence supporting the automaticity of such socio-affective inferential mechanisms and discuss its implication for the theoretical framework of affective social learning. Next, we discuss the idea that ambiguous situations may be particularly prone to social appraisal taking place. Finally, we discuss how social appraisal, underpinned by a socio-affective inferential mechanism, may be integrated into the affective social learning framework.

Type
Chapter
Information
Foundations of Affective Social Learning
Conceptualizing the Social Transmission of Value
, pp. 142 - 164
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, R. B., Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Perceived gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychological Science, 14, 644647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, R. B., (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facial communicated emotion. Emotion, 5, 311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aviezer, H., Bentin, S., Dudarev, V., & Hassin, R. R. (2011). The automaticity of emotional face–context integration. Emotion, 11, 14061414.Google Scholar
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R. R., Bentin, S., & Trope, Y. (2008). Putting facial expressions back in context. In Ambady, N. & Skowronsky, J. (Eds.), First impressions (pp. 255286). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J., Anderson, A., … Bentin, S. (2008). Angry, disgusted, or afraid? Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychological Science, 19, 724732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aviezer, H., Trope, Y., & Todorov, A. (2012). Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science, 338(6111), 12251229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayliss, A. P., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Affective evaluations of objects are influenced by observed gaze direction and emotional expression. Cognition, 104, 644653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., & Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 286290.Google Scholar
Bodenhausen, G. V., & Todd, A. R. (2010). Social cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(2), 160171. doi:10.1002/wcs.28Google Scholar
Bruder, M., Fischer, A., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2014). Social appraisal as a cause of collective emotions. In von Scheve, C. & Salmela, M. (Eds.), Collective emotions (pp. 141155). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clément, F., Bernard, S., Grandjean, D., & Sander, D. (2013). Emotional expression and vocabulary learning in adults and children. Cognition and Emotion, 27(3), 539548Google Scholar
Clément, F., & Dukes, D. (2013). The role of interest in the social transmission of values. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, R. B., (2017). Social appraisal and social referencing: Two components of affective social learning. Emotion Review, 9(3), 253261.Google Scholar
Cristinzio, C., N’Diaye, K., Seeck, M., Vuilleumier, P., & Sander, D. (2010). Integration of gaze direction and facial expression in patients with unilateral amygdala damage. Brain, 133, 248261.Google Scholar
Cline, M. G. (1956). The influence of social context on the perception of faces. Journal of Personality, 25(2), 142157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daw, N. D., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2014). Multiple systems for value learning. In Glimcher, P. W. & Fehr, E (Eds.), Neuroeconomics (2nd ed., pp. 393410). New York, NY: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Gelder, B., Meeren, H. K. M., Righart, R., Stock, J., van de Riet, W. A. C., & Tamietto, M. (2006). Beyond the face: Exploring rapid influences of context on face processing. Progress in Brain Research, 155, 3748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Gelder, B., & van den Stock, J. (2012). Real faces, real emotions: Perceiving facial expressions in naturalistic contexts of voices, bodies and scenes. In Calder, A. J., Rhodes, G., Haxby, J. V., & Johnson, M. H. (Eds.), The handbook of face perception. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
de Melo, C. M., Carnevale, P. J., Read, S. J., & Gratch, J. (2014). Reading people’s minds from emotion expressions in interdependent decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(1), 7388.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In Cole, J. K. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 207283). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression of emotion. American Psychologist, 48, 384392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., Freisen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face. New York, NY: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Evers, C., Fischer, A., Mosquera, P., & Manstead, A. (2005). Anger and social appraisal: A “spicy” sex difference? Emotion, 5, 258266.Google Scholar
Feinman, S. (1992). In the broad valley: An integrative look at social referencing. In Feinman, S. (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of reality in infancy (pp. 1554). New York, NY: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117140.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. H., Rotteveel, M., Evers, C., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004). Emotional assimilation: How we are influenced by others’ emotions. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 22, 223245.Google Scholar
Gendron, M., Mesquita, B., & Barrett, L. F. (2013). Emotion perception: Putting the face in context. In Reisberg, D. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology (pp. 379389). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Unpacking the cognitive architecture of emotion processes. Emotion, 8, 341351. doi:10.1037/1528- 3542.8.3.341CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haaker, J., Golkar, A., Selbing, I., & Olsson, A (2017). Assessment of social transmission of threats in humans using observational fear conditioning. Nature Protocols, 12(7), 13781386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852870). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hassin, R. R. (2013). Yes it can: On the functional abilities of the human unconscious. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 195207.Google Scholar
Hassin, R. R., Aviezer, H., & Benton, S. (2013). Inherently ambiguous: Facial expressions of emotions, in context. Emotion Review, 5, 6065.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hassin, R. R., Bargh, J. A., & Uleman, J. S. (2002). Spontaneous causal inferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 515522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Jakobs, E., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1997). Emotional experience as a function of social context: The role of the other. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 103130.Google Scholar
Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., Burriss, R. P., & Feinberg, D. R. (2007). Social transmission of face preferences among humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 274, 899903.Google Scholar
Klinnert, M. D., Campos, J. J., Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., & Svejda, M. (1983). Emotions as behavior regulators. In Plutchik, R. & Kellerman, H. (Eds.), Emotions: Theory, research, and experience (Vol. 2, pp. 5786). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
LaBar, K., & Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(1), 5464.Google Scholar
Landis, C. (1929). The interpretation of facial expression in emotion. Journal of General Psychology, 2(1), 5972.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 259289.Google Scholar
Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2001). Social appraisal: The social world as object of and influence on appraisal processes. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, method, research (pp. 221232). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P. C., Mesquita, B., Leu, J., Tanida, S., & de Veerdonk, E. V. (2008). Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 365381.Google Scholar
Moors, A. (2017). Integration of two skeptical emotion theories: Dimensional appraisal theory and Russell’s psychological construction theory. Psychological Inquiry, 28(1), 119.Google Scholar
Mudrik, L., Faivre, N., & Koch, C. (2014). Information integration without awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 488496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mumenthaler, C., & Sander, D. (2012). Social appraisal influences recognition of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 11181135.Google Scholar
Mumenthaler, C., (2015). Automatic integration of social information in emotion recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 392399.Google Scholar
Mumenthaler, C., Sander, D., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2018). Emotion recognition in simulated social interactions. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing. https://doi.org/10.1109/TAFFC.2018.2799593Google Scholar
Olkkonen, M., & Ekroll, V. (2016) Color constancy and contextual effects on color appearance. In Kremers, J., Baraas, R, & Marshall, N. (Eds.), Human color vision (Springer Series in Vision Research, Vol. 5, pp. 221232). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.Google Scholar
Olsson, A., Nearing, K. I., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Learning fears by observing others: The neural systems of social fear transmission. Social, Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 2, 210.Google Scholar
Olsson, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2004). Learned fear of unseen faces after Pavlovian, observational and instructed fear. Psychological Science, 15, 822828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parkinson, B., Phiri, N., & Simons, G. (2012). Bursting with anxiety: Adult social referencing in an interpersonal Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Emotion, 12(4), 817826. doi:10.1037/a0026434Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Simons, G. (2009). Worry spreads: Interpersonal transfer of problem-related anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 26(3), 462479.Google Scholar
Pool, E. R., Brosch, T., Delplanque, S., & Sander, D. (2016). Attentional bias for positive emotional stimuli: A meta-analytic investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 79106.Google Scholar
Righart, R., & de Gelder, B. (2008). Recognition of facial expressions is influenced by emotional scene gist. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 264272.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Fehr, B. (1987). Relativity in the perception of emotion in facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116(3), 223237.Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 470480.Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2005). A systems approach to appraisal mechanisms in emotion. Neural Networks, 18, 317352.Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2018). An appraisal-driven componential approach to the emotional brain. Emotion Review, 10(3), 219231.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Mortillaro, M., Rotondi, I., Sergi, I., & Trznadel, S. (2018). Appraisal-driven facial actions as building blocks for emotion inference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(3), 358379.Google Scholar
Sennwald, V., Pool, E., & Sander, D. (2017). Considering the influence of the Pavlovian system on behavior: Appraisal and value representation. Psychological Inquiry, 28(1), 5255.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Scott, H. S. (1997). A componential approach to the meaning of facial expressions. In Russell, J. A., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 229254). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 195.Google Scholar
Stussi, Y., Pourtois, G., & Sander, D. (2018). Enhanced Pavlovian aversive conditioning to positive emotional stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147, 905923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, Y., Harris, P. L., Zou, H., & Xu, Q. (2019). The impact of emotional expressions on children’s trust judgments. Cognition and Emotion, 33(2), 318331. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2018.1449735CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todorov, A. (2017). Face value: The irresistible influence of first impressions. Princeton, NJ and Oxford, UK: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Trope, Y. (1986). Identification and inferential processes in dispositional attribution. Psychological Review, 93, 239257.Google Scholar
van den Stock, J., Righart, R., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Whole body expressions influence recognition of facial expressions and emotional prosody. Emotion, 7, 487494.Google Scholar
van Doorn, E. A., Heerdink, M. W., & van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Emotion and the construal of social situations: Inferences of cooperation versus competition from expressions of anger, happiness, and disappointment. Cognition & Emotion, 26(3), 442461.Google Scholar
van Reekum, C., Johnstone, T., Banse, R., Etter, A., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. (2004). Psychophysiological responses to appraisal dimensions in a computer game. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 663688.Google Scholar
Wehrle, T., Kaiser, S., Schmidt, S., & Scherer, K. R. (2000). Studying the dynamics of emotional expression using synthesized facial muscle movements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 105119.Google Scholar
Wieser, M. J., & Brosch, T. (2012). Faces in context: a review and systematization of contextual influences on affective face processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 471.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×